Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:12:53.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological sensitivity, rapid naming, and beginning reading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Virginia Cronin*
Affiliation:
Mount Saint Vincent University
Paula Carver
Affiliation:
Mount Saint Vincent University
*
Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, Nova Scotia B3M 2j6, Canada. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Reading acquisition was related to phonological sensitivity and rapid naming in a longitudinal study with young children. Phonological assessment consisted of rhyme and initial consonant discrimination, while the rapid naming tasks were made up of pictures, letters, and numbers. The subjects were 95 children from two grade levels, primary and first grade. They were tested in the fall and spring of the first year and the spring of the second year. It was found that the phonological and rapid naming tests each predicted unique variance in reading attainment, as measured at the end of the second year of the study. The rapid naming responses became more automatic early in the first grade year, while naming times generally became faster. Although many researchers regard rapid naming as part of the phonological core, the present article discusses the various advantages of considering rapid naming as a separate factor in reading development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Badian, N. A. (1993). Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing and reading. Readingand Writing, 5, 87100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badian, N. A. (1994). Preschool prediction: Orthographic and phonological skills, and reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badian, N. A., McAnulty, G. B., Duffy, F. H., & Als, H. (1990). Predication of dyslexia in kindergarten boys. Annals of Dyslexia, 40, 152169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blachman, B. A. (1994). What we have learned from longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading, and some unanswered questions: A response to Torgesen, Wagner, and Rashotte. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 287291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Determinants of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P., Golden, J., Kennedy, A., & Young, A. (1994). In Berninger, V. W. (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge. I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 173218). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R., & Tate, E. (1988). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 304319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G. & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 6985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature. 271, 746747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read-A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., MacLean, M. & Bradley, L. (1990). Rhyme, language, and children's reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, L. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1987). Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19, 531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1979). Does word training increase or decrease interference in a Stroop Task? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 352364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N., & Large, B. (1988). The early stages of reading: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2, 4776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1994). Naming speed in children with dyslexia. Journal of Learnng Disabilities, 27, 641646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 3959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felton, R. H., Naylor, C., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felton, R. H., Wood, F. B., Brown, I. S., & Campbell, S. K. (1987). Separate verbal memory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability. Brain and Language, 31, 171184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guttentag, R. E., & Haith, M. M. (1978). Automatic processing as a function of age and reading ability. Child Development, 49, 707716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F. & Share, D. L. (1983). Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jules, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437447.Google Scholar
Korhonen, T. T. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: A nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 232239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
La Berge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemoine, H. E., Levy, B. A., & Hutchinson, A. (1993). Increasing the naming speed of poor readers: Representations formed across repetitions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 297328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. A., & Hinchley, J. (1990). Individual and developmental differences in the acquisition of reading skills. In Carr, T. H. & Levy, B. A. (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 81128). San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovett, M. W. (1992). Developmental dyslexia. In Segalowitz, S. J. & Rapin, I. (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 7). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lovett, M. W., Warren-Chaplin, P. M., Ransby, M. J., & Borden, S. L. (1990). Training the word recognition skills of reading disabled children: Treatment and transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 769780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 33, 255282.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, T. R. (1983). Dyslexia: The pattern of difficulties. London: Collins.Google Scholar
Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 154167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35, 159182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, R. E.,. Jenkins, J. R., & Slocum, T. A. (1995). Transfer among phonological tasks in kindergarten: Essential instructional content. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 202217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., & Rack, J. (1990). Organization, heritability, and remediation of component word recognition and language skills. In Carr, T. H. & Levy, B. A. (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 261322). San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283319.Google Scholar
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 2953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L., Jorm, A. E, Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 13091324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, L. S. (1986). Phonological deficits in children with a reading disability. Canadian Journal of Special Education, 2, 4554.Google Scholar
Spring, C., & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, C., & Davis, J. M. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 315334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-varietypoor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Concepts in developmental theories of reading skill: Cognitive resources, automaticity, and modularity. Developmental Review, 10, 72100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early readingacquisition. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Balthazar, M., Davis, C., Morgan, S., Simmons, K., Stage, S., & Zirps, F. (1989). Developmental and individual differences in performance on phonological synthesis tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 491505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tunmer, W. E., & Hoover, W. A. (1993). Components of variance models of language-related factors in reading disability: A conceptual overview. In Joshi, R. M. & Leong, C. K. (Eds.), Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes (pp. 135173). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Nesdale, A. R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321363.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. (in press). A provisional, integrative account of phonological and naming-speed deficits in dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In Blachman, Benita (Ed.), Cognitive and linguistic foundations of reading acquisition: Implications for intervention research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 9881005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early developmental dyslexia, and a specific deficit hypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., & Ovrut, M. (1990). Rate variables and automatized naming in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 556575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Yap, R. L., & van der Leif, A. (1994). Testing the automatization deficit hypothesis of dyslexia via a dual-task paradigm. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 660665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar