Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T12:41:08.889Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Metalinguistic contribution to reading comprehension: A comparison of Primary 3 students from China and Singapore

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Sun Baoqi*
Affiliation:
Nanyang Technological University, National Institute of Education, Singapore
Guangwei Hu
Affiliation:
Department of English, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University
Xiao Lan Curdt-Christiansen
Affiliation:
Department of Education, University of Bath
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined the within- and cross-language metalinguistic contribution of three components of metalinguistic awareness (i.e., phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and syntactic awareness) to reading comprehension in monolingual Chinese-speaking children from Mainland China (n = 190) and English–Chinese bilingual children from Singapore (n = 390). Moreover, the effect of home language use on the relationship between metalinguistic awareness and reading performance was investigated. For monolingual children, hierarchical regression analyses revealed that after partialing out the effects of age, nonverbal intelligence, and oral vocabulary, syntactic awareness uniquely predicted 7%–13% of the variance in reading comprehension measures, whereas this relationship was not observed between morphological awareness and reading comprehension. For the bilingual children, within-language regression analyses revealed that English/Chinese morphological awareness and syntactic awareness both contributed significantly to English/Chinese reading measures over and above vocabulary and phonological awareness. Cross-linguistically, structure equation modeling results demonstrated that the bilingual children’s English and Chinese metalinguistic awareness were closely related and jointly supported reading comprehension in both languages, thus lending support to Koda’s transfer facilitation model. Furthermore, home language use was found to contribute to the bilingual children’s reading proficiency via its impact on metalinguistic awareness. The paper concludes with a discussion of the policy and pedagogical implications that can be drawn from these findings.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. C., & Chen, X. (2013). Chinese reading development in monolingual and bilingual learners: Introduction to the special issue. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, C. (2006). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 2744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1988). Metalinguistic functioning in children. Sydney, Australia: UNSW Press.Google Scholar
Brimo, D., Apel, K., & Fountain, T. (2017). Examining the contributions of syntactic awareness and syntactic knowledge to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40, 5774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cha, K., & Goldenberg, C. (2015). The complex relationship between bilingual home language input and kindergarten children’s Spanish and English oral proficiencies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 935953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chik, P. P. M., Ho, C. S. H., Yeung, P. S., Chan, D. W. O., Chung, K. K. H., Luan, H., … Lau, W. S. Y. (2012). Syntactic skills in sentence reading comprehension among Chinese elementary school children. Reading and Writing, 25, 679699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, W., & Hu, C. (2007). Morphological awareness and learning to read Chinese. Reading and Writing, 20, 441461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2007). Exploring trilingual Chinese children’s language use. Sociolinguistic Studies, 1, 6785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2016). Conflicting language ideologies and contradictory language practices in Singaporean bilingual families. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 37, 694709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curdt-Christiansen, X. L., & Silver, R. E. (2013). New wine into old skins: The enactment of literacy policy in Singapore. Language and Education, 27, 246260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curdt-Christiansen, X. L., & Sun, B. (2016). Nurturing bilingual learners: Challenges and concerns in Singapore. International Journal of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education, 19, 689705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Kieffer, M. (2018). Understanding how syntactic awareness contributes to reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1, 7286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Tong, X., & Francis, K. (2017). The relationship of morphological analysis and morphological decoding to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 411424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Ehri, L., Nunes, S., Willows, D., Schuster, B., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, E. (2007). Monolingualism: The unmarked case. Sociolinguistic Studies, 7, 173196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galambos, S. J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. G. (1990). The effect of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition, 34, 156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F., & Nicoladis, E. (2007). Bilingual first language acquisition. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (Eds.), Handbook of language development (pp. 324342). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombert, J. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A., Yan, B., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 530542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hair, J. F., Tatham, R. L., Anderson, R. E., & Black, W. (2006). Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Hiebert, E. H., & Reutzel, D. R. (2010). Revisiting silent reading: New directions for teachers and researchers. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, C. S. H., & Bryant, P. E. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, G. (2002). Psychological constraints on the utility of metalinguistic knowledge in second language production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 347386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, G. (2011). Metalinguistic knowledge, metalanguage, and their relationship in L2 learners. System, 39, 6377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2012). Knowledge of words, knowledge about words: Dimensions of vocabulary in first and second language learners in sixth grade. Reading and Writing, 25, 347373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2005). Comprehension. In Paris, S. G. & Stahl, S. A. (Eds.), Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 89110). New YorkRoutledge.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A cross-linguistic approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koda, K. (2008). Impacts of prior literacy experience on learning to read in a second language. In Koda, K. & Zehler, A. M. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages: Cross-linguistic relationships in first- and second-language literacy development (pp. 6896). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ku, Y.-M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L.-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2008). Conceptual and methodological issues in comparing metalinguistic awareness across languages. In Koda, K. & Zehler, A. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 3967). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Li, H., & Rao, N. (2000). Parental influences on Chinese literacy development: A comparison of preschoolers in Beijing, Hong Kong, and Singapore. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 8290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, W., Anderson, R. C., Nagy, W., & Zhang, H. (2002). Facets of metalinguistic awareness that contribute to Chinese literacy. In Li, W., Gaffney, J., & Packard, J. (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading acquisition: Theoretical and pedagogical issues. (pp.87106). London: Kluwer Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Lin, L. C., & Johnson, C. J. (2010). Phonological patterns in Mandarin-English bilingual children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 24, 369386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 6273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y. B., & Goh, H. H. (2006). A critical examination of the newly revised Chinese language syllabus and textbooks for Singapore primary school students (Report 3). (Report No. CRP 41/03 SG). Singapore: Nanyang Technological University.Google Scholar
Mattingly, G. (1992). Linguistic awareness and orthographic form. In Katz, L & Frost, R (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 1126). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., … Muse, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, H. B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Literacy Research and Instruction, 46, 7394.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E. & Anderson, R. C. (1998) Metalinguistic awareness and literacy acquisition in different languages. In Wagner, D., Venezy, R, & Street, B. (Eds.), Literacy: An international handbook (pp. 155160). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Berningger, V. W., Abbott, R. C., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packard, J. L. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: A linguistic and cognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Luo, Y. C., & Ramirez, G. (2011). Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 2342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization and universals in the development of reading skill: How Chinese research informs a universal science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
P. R. China Ministry of Education. (2001). Yuwen kecheng biaozhun [Chinese Language Primary Syllabus]. Beijing, China: Beijing Normal University Press.Google Scholar
Rego, L. L. B., & Bryant, P. E. (1993). The connection between phonological, syntactic and semantic skills and children’s reading and spelling. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 235246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ren, L., & Hu, G. W. (2013). A comparative study of family social capital and literacy practices in Singapore. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 13, 98130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, G. J. (2001). Wide Range Achievement Test––Expanded edition. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.Google Scholar
Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 180202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seymour, P. H. (2006). Theoretical framework for beginning reading in different orthographies. In Joshi, R. M. & Aaron, P. G. (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 441462). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., & Duncan, L. G. (2001). Learning to read in English. Psychology: The Journal of the Hellenic Psychological Society, 8, 281299.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthography–phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 5662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 2747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singaporean Ministry of Education. (2007). Chinese language (primary) syllabus. Singapore: Author.Google Scholar
Singaporean Ministry of Education. (2010). English language syllabus for primary and secondary schools. Singapore: Author.Google Scholar
Singapore Department of Statistics. (2012). Census of the population. education, language and religion. Retrieved from http://www.singstat.gov.sgGoogle Scholar
Siu, C. T. S., & Ho, C. S. H. (2015). Cross-language transfer of syntactic skills and reading comprehension among young Cantonese–English bilingual students. Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 313336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica: Rand Corporation.Google Scholar
Sun, B., & Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2016). Morphological awareness and reading development in bilingual English-Chinese children in Singapore. In Silver, R. & Bokhorst-Heng, W. (Eds.), Quadrilingual education in Singapore: Pedagogical innovation in language education (pp. 83101). Singapore: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, B., & Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2017). Chinese instruction in Singapore: Voices of children and views of teacher. In Lee, G. & Ma, W. (Eds.), Educating Chinese-Heritage students in the global-localnexus: Identities, challenges, and opportunities (pp. 209226). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, B., Hu, G., & Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2018). Metalinguistic contribution to writing competence: A study of monolingual children in China and bilingual children in Singapore. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 14991523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taiwan Ministry of Education. (2003). Guomin zhongxiaoxue jiunian yiguan kecheng gangyao [Chinese syllabus for nine-year compulsory education]. Retrieved from http://www.k12ea.gov.tw/97_sid17Google Scholar
Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Chinese-English biscriptal reading: Cognitive component skills across orthographies. Reading and Writing, 23, 293310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2015). A tale of two writing systems: Double dissociation and metalinguistic transfer between Chinese and English word reading among Hong Kong children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48, 130145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tong, X., Tong, X., Shu, H., Chan, S., & McBride, C. C. (2014). Discourse-level reading comprehension in Chinese children: What is the role of syntactic awareness? Journal of Research in Reading, 37, S48S70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Cheng, C. X., & Chen, S. W. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 542553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Ko, I. Y., & Choi, J. (2009). The importance of morphological awareness in Korean-English biliteracy acquisition. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 132142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, X., Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Shu, H., Jiang, W., Chen, X., … Gaffney, J. S. (2009). Morphological awareness and Chinese children’s literacy development: An intervention study. Scientific Studies in Reading, 13, 2652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xue, J., Shu, H., Li, H., Li, W., & Tian, X. (2013). The stability of literacy-related cognitive contributions to Chinese character naming and reading fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 433450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, P., Ho, C. S., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K., & Wong, Y. (2013). A model of reading comprehension in Chinese elementary school children. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, P., Ho, C. S., Chik, P. P., Lo, L., Luan, H., Chan, D. W., & Chung, K. K. (2011). Reading and spelling Chinese among beginning readers: What skills make a difference? Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 285313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, D., Chin, C.-F., & Li, L. (2017). Metalinguistic awareness in bilingual children’s word reading: A cross-lagged panel study on cross-linguistic transfer facilitation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 395426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar