Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T14:20:19.538Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory for rote linguistic routines and sensitivity to rhyme: A comparison of low-income children with and without specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Barbara B. Fazio*
Affiliation:
Indiana University
*
Speech & Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This report describes two studies on memory for rote linguistic sequences and sensitivity to rhyme in young children with and without language impairment. In the first study, 10 low income kindergarteners with specific language impairment (SLI) were compared with age- and income-matched classmates on reciting common nursery rhymes, reciting the alphabet, and rote counting. Children with SLI displayed lower performance on most of the rote linguistic sequence tasks, especially on (heir knowledge of Mother Goose nursery rhymes. The second study examined the learning and retention of nursery rhymes in 8 young children with SLI after 6 weeks of classroom instruction. Low-income, 4- and 5-year-old children with SLI and their age- and income-matched classmates were taught five novel Mother Goose rhymes during a large-group classroom activity. Children were tested before and after the intervention on their ability to recite nursery rhymes and to detect rhyme. When compared with their peers, children with SLI had difficulty repeating the nursery rhymes, despite daily classroom exposure. Although the performance of children with SLI on rhyme recitation and detection tasks was poor, their relative performance was better on a cloze task based on the set of nursery rhymes. The results of this study suggest that children with SLI have difficulty storing and/or retrieving lines of memorized text. Traditional informal techniques for teaching rote linguistic sequences may need to be modified to give children with SLI more opportunities to practice rote sequences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abram, D., Ekelman, B. L., & Nation, J. E. (1984). Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 232244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Alexander, M. J., & Entwisle, D. R. (1988). Achievement in the first 2 years of school: Patterns and processes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 53 (2, Serial No. 218).Google ScholarPubMed
American National Standards Institute. (1973). American National Standards Specifications for Audiometers (ANSI S3.6–1969, R1973). New York: American National Standards Institute.Google Scholar
Bashir, A. S., & Scavuzzo, A. (1992). Children with language disorders: Natural history and academic success. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 5365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders, and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 10271050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, R., & Bjork, L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In Healy, A. F., Kosslyn, S. M., & Shiffrin, R. M. (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 34–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature. 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., Bryant, P., & Maclean, M. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255281.Google Scholar
Brady, S. A. (1991). The role of working memory in reading disability. In Brady, S. A. & Shankweiler, D. (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 129151). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., Maclean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M., Bradley, L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgemeister, B., Blum, L., & Lorge, I. (1972). The Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 313321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrow-Woolfolk, C. (1985). Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language - Revised. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Catts, H. (1989). Phonological processing deficits and reading disabilities. In Kamhi, A. & Cayys, H. (Eds.), Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective (pp. 101132). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Catts, H. (1991). Early iden ti ficat ion of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow -up st udy of speech-language impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H.(1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. (1996). Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder: An expanded view. Topics in Language Disorders, 16, 1429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtiss, S., & Tallal, P. (1991). On the nature of the impairment in language-impaired children. In Miller, J. (Ed.), Research on child language disorders: A decade of progress (pp. 189210). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Demster, F. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89. 63100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1976). Naming of objects drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. B. (1994). Counting abilities of children with specific language impairment: A comparison of oral and gestural tasks. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 358368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. B. (1995). Competent numerical knowledge but inadequate counting performance in preschool children with specific language impairment.Paper presented at the meeting on Language and Mathematical Thinking: Current Issues in Developmental, Neuropsychological and Educational Research,London, England.Google Scholar
Fazio, B. B. (1996a). The mathematical abilities of children with specific language impairment: A two-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 111.Google Scholar
Fazio, B. B. (1996b). Serial memory in children with specific language impairment: Examining specific content areas for assessment and intervention. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 5871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, B. B. (1996c). Comparing serial memory for symbolic and non-symbolic representations in children with specific language impairment.Paper presented at the Second International Conference on Memory,Padova, Italy.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, B. B. (1997). Learning a new poem: Memory for connected speech and phonological awareness in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 40, 956966.Google ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C., & Connell, P. C. (1996). Tracking children at risk for specific language impairment: A three year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 5670.Google Scholar
Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., & Larrivee, L. S. (1995). Preparing preschoolers for the academic and social challenges of school. In Fey, M. E., Windsor, J., & Warren, S. F. (Eds.), Language intervention: Preschool through the elementary years (pp. 338). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J., Shaywitz, S., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Liberman, I., Stuebing, K., Francis, D., Fowler, A., & Shaywitz, B. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, syllables, and phonemes. A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried-Oken, M. (1984). The development of naming skills in normal and language deficient children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1993). Working memory and language. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhymes: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
German, D. (1984). Diagnosis of word-finding disorder in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 353359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
German, D. (1987). Spontaneous language profiles of children with word-finding problems. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 217230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
German, D. (1992). Word-finding intervention for children and adolescents. Topics in Language Disorders, 13, 3350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R. B., Cowan, B., & Day, L. S. (1995). Sequential memory in children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 393402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsburg, H. P. (1977). Children's arithmetic: The learning process. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Goswami, C., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skill and learning to read. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Henry, L. A. (1991). Effects of word length and phonemic similarity in young children's short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 3552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, M. L., & Brainerd, C. J. (1989). Development of children's long-term retention. Developmental Review, 9, 301340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1990). The development of memory in children (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Kail, R., Hale, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Nippold, M. A. (1984). Lexical storage and retrieval in language impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 3749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., & Leonard, L. B. (1986). Word-finding abilities in language-impaired children.” ASHA Monographs, 25.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1, 337347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchner, D., & Klatzky, R. (1985). Verbal rehearsal and memory in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 556565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. D., Nippold, M. A., Kail, R., & Hale, C. (1983). Picture naming in language impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. A., Nicholls, A., & Kohen, D. (1993). Repeating reading: Process benefits for good and poor reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 303327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y. (1982). A language-oriented view of reading and its disabilities. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maclean, M., Bryant, P. E., & Bradley, L. (1987). Nursery rhymes, rhymes and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255282.Google Scholar
Magnusson, E., & Naucler, K. (1990). Reading and spelling in language-disordered children linguistic and metalinguistic prerequisites: Report on a longitudinal study. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 4, 4961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1983). Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of Learning - Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
MacGinitie, W. H., & MacGinitie, R. K. (1989). Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests (Level R) (3rd ed.). Chicago: Riverside.Google Scholar
McGregor, K. K., & Leonard, L. B. (1989). Facilitating word-finding skills of language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 141147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P. (1964). Comparison of grammar of children with functionally deviant and normal speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 7, 109121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P., &Looney, P. L. (1972). A problem of language disorder: Length vs. structure. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 264279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, R. R., Kasprow, W. J., & Schactman, T. R. (1986). Retrieval variability: Sources and consequences. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 145218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morais, J., Carey, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morais, J., Cluyens, M., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1986). Speech mediated retention in dyslexia. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 119126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, F., McMahon, E. H., & Williamson, G. A. (1993). Two strikes from the start: Individual differences in early literacy. Society for Research in Child Development Abstracts, 9, 219.Google Scholar
Nelson, L. K., Kamhi, A. G., & Apel, K. (1987). Cognitive strengths and weaknesses in language-impaired children: One more look. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 3036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcomer, P. & Hammill, D. (1988). Test of Language Development - Primary {Revised). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
O'Connor, R. E., Jenkins, J. R., Leicester, N., & Slocum, T. A. (1993). Teaching phonological awareness to young children with learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 59, 532546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisma, P. (1988). Reading practice for beginners: Effects of guided reading, reading-while-listening, and independent reading with computer-based speech feedback. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 219235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, H., Bernstein, S., & Katz, R. G. (1989). Effect of cues on object naming in first grade good and poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, H., & Liberman, I. (1983). Exploring the oral and written language errors made by language disabled children. Annals of Dyslexia, 33, 111120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schorr, L. (1988). Within our reach. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Segal, D., & Wolf, M. (1993). Automaticity, word-retrieval and vocabulary development in reading disabled children. In Meltzer, L. (Ed.), Strategy assessment and instruction for students with learning disabilities: From theory to practice (pp. 141165). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Shear, P. K., Tallal, P., & Delis, D. C. (1992). Verbal learning and memory in language impaired children. Neuropsychologia, 30, 451458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sininger, Y.S., Klatzky, R. L., & Kirchner, D. M. (1989). Memory scanning speed in language disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 289297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1983). Language and literacy: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. In Torgesen, J. & Wong, B. (Eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 87131). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1988). The right and the wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 154180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 38. 175190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, J., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction and achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development, 57, 646659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, L. (1978). Verbal encoding effects on the visual short-term memory of learning disabled and normal readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 539544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 8393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tough, J. (1982). Language, poverty, and disadvantage in school. In Feagans, L. & Farran, D. (Eds.), The language of children reared in poverty (pp. 218). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
van Kleeck, A. (in press). Departing from phonics and whole language: Emphasizing form and meaning separately in prereading and early reading instruction. Topics in Language Disorders.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F., & Scanlon, D. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321363.Google Scholar
Wagner, R., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrick, N., Rubin, H., & Rowe-Walsh, S. (1993). Phoneme awareness in language-delayed children: Comparative studies and intervention. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 153173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weill, J. (1991). Child poverty in America. Clearinghouse Review, 25, 336.Google Scholar
Wiig, E. H., & Semel, E. M. (1984). Language assessment and intervention for the learning disabled (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Wing, C. S. (1990). A preliminary investigation of generalization to untrained words following two treatments of children's word-finding problems. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 151156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, S. H. (1993). Teaching word-finding strategies to severely language-impaired children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 28. 165175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. P. (1980). Teaching decoding with an emphasis on phoneme analysis and phoneme blending. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. (1984). Naming, reading and the dyslexias: A longitudinal overview. Annals of Dyslexia, 34, 87115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficits, developmental dyslexia, and a specific deficit hypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar