Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:42:47.626Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lending a helping hand: Linguistic input and sign language acquisition in deaf children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Philip M. Prinz*
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University
Louise Masin
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University
*
Dr. Philip M. Prinz, Division of Special Education and Communication Disorders, Pennsylvania State University, 217 Moore Building, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802.

Abstract

This study examined the effects of adult “recasting” in sign language on the acquisition of specific syntactic-semantic structures by six deaf children between 9 and 76 months who were primarily at the one-sign utterance stage of development. In “recast” replies in conversation, the child's utterance is redisplayed in an altered sentence structure that still refers to the central meanings of the first sentence. Syntactic-semantic structures targeted for input intervention by teachers and parents using recasts included subject–verb relations, attribution, negation, subject–verb–object relations, conjunction, and conditionality. Recasting triggered the acquisition of new syntactic-semantic structures in American Sign Language and English which were evident in the spontaneous production of previously non-used sign utterances.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baker, N. D., & Nelson, K. E.Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advantages by young children. First Language, 1984, 5, 322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blount, B. G. Ethnography and caretaker–child interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1977.Google Scholar
Bonvillian, J. Personal communication, 1984.Google Scholar
Brown, R.A first language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1973.Google Scholar
Brown, R. Introduction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1977.Google Scholar
Buckhalt, J., Rutherford, R., & Goldberg, K.Verbal and nonverbal interaction of mothers with Down's Syndrome and nonretarded infants. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1978, 82, 337343.Google ScholarPubMed
Buium, N., Rynders, J., & Turnure, J.Early maternal linguistic environment of normal and Down's Syndrome language learning children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1974, 79, 5258.Google ScholarPubMed
Calculator, S. Examining caregivers' speaking modifications to developmentally delayed infants as a cumulative effect of the caregiver–infant social system. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Wisconsin at Madison, 1980.Google Scholar
Cicourel, A. V., & Boese, R. J. Sign language acquisition and the teaching of deaf children. In Cazden, C. B., John, V. P., & Hymes, D. (Eds.), Functions of language in the classroom. New York: Teachers College Press, 1972.Google Scholar
Cokely, D.When is a pidgin not a pidgin? An alternate analysis of the ASL–English contact situation. Sign Language Studies, 1983, 38, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. G. Mothers' speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1977.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H.Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language, 1979, 6, 423442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H.The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 1984, 2, 4379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmeister, R. J., Moores, D. F., & Ellenberger, R. L.Some procedural guidelines for the study of the acquisition of sign language. Sign Language Studies, 1975, 7, 121136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kantor, R.Communicative interaction: Mother modification and child acquisition of American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 36, 233282, 1982.Google Scholar
Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U.The signs of language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. On the nature of syntactic development. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Language development (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1982.Google Scholar
Lasky, E., & Klopp, K.Parent–child interactions in normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1982, 47, 718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, S.Levels of development in the language of deaf children: ASL grammatical processes, SE structures, and semantic features. Sign Language Studies, 1983, 40, 193286.Google Scholar
Loew, R. C. Roles and reference in American Sign Language: A developmental perspective. Unpublished dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1983.Google Scholar
Maestas y Moores, J.Early linguistic environment: Interactions of deaf parents with their infants. Sign Language Studies, 1980, 26, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meadow, K. P., Greenberg, M. T., Erting, C., & Carmichael, H.Interactions of deaf mothers and deaf preschool children: Comparisons with three other groups of deaf and hearing dyads. American Annals of the Deaf, 1981, 126(4), 454468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, R. Icons, analogues, and morphemes: The acquisition of verb agreement in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at San Diego, 1982.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E.Facilitating children's syntax acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 1977, 13, 101107.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E.Theories of the child's acquisition of syntax: A look at rare events and at necessary, catalytic, and irrelevant components of mother–child conversation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1980, 345, 4567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Carskaddon, G., & Bonvillian, J. D.Syntax acquisition: Impact of experimental variation in adult verbal interaction with the child. Child Development, 1973, 44, 497504.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. “Mother, I'd rather do it myself.” Some effects and noneffects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1977.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. Acquisition of American Sign Language. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1985.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Supalla, T. The structuring of language: Clues from the acquisition of signed and spoken language. In Bellugi, U. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (Eds.), Signed and spoken language: Biological constraints on linguistic form. Proceedings of Dahlem Conference. Weeinheim/Deerfield Beach, Fla.: Basel: Verlag Chemie, 1980.Google Scholar
Prinz, P. M., & Dinner, M. B. The acquisition and evaluation of sign language skills in children. In L. J. Bradford and F. N. Martin (Eds.), Audiology: An Audio Journal, 1981, 6(9).Google Scholar
Prinz, P. M., & Prinz, E. A.If only you could hear what I see: Discourse development in sign language. Discourse Processes, 1985, 8, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, F. P.Accelerating language learning in young children. Journal of Child Language, 1984, 2, 89107.Google Scholar
Schodorf, J., & Edwards, H.Comparative analysis of parent–child interactions with language-disordered and linguistically normal children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 1983, 16, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. Conversations with children. In Fletcher, P. & Garmen, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. Classification and description of sign languages. In Sebeok, T. (Ed.), Current trends in linguistics (Vol. 12). The Hague: Mouton. 1972.Google Scholar
Wells, G. Apprenticeship in meaning. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 2). New York: Gardner Press, 1980.Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. G.American Sign Language and sign systems. Baltimore, Md.: University Park Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Woodward, J. C.Some characteristics of Pidgin Sign English inter-rule implication in American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 1973, 3956.Google Scholar