Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:09:32.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

I pledge a legiance tothe flag: Three studies in word segmentation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Carolyn Chaney*
Affiliation:
San Francisco State University
*
Carolyn Chaney, Department of Speech and Communication Studies, San Francisco State University, 1600 Holloway Avenue, San Francisco, CA 94132

Abstract

Three experimental tasks explored young children's metalinguistic awareness of word boundaries. First, preschool children recited word-by-word pieces which differed in abstractness and familiarity of vocabulary. Second, a teaching task was employed to train and then sample selected items taken from the recited pieces, representing words with cards. The data suggested that several strategies were employed: segment-by-phrase, segment-by-syllable, and segment-by-word. Children were less successful in segmenting the more abstract piece than the concrete one, and they tended to revert to a phrase strategy and/or to change abstract words into more common vocabulary. Error analysis revealed growing awareness of the functors a and the. In a third experiment, preschool and kindergarten children segmented phrases using the functors a, an, my, or and phrases using phonetically similar embedded syllables (e.g., hold my nose; gold miner). Segmentation scores increased with age; even the 4½- to 5-year-olds were highly successful, segmenting with 60% accuracy, and by age 6–6½, they were correctly segmenting 75% of the phrases. Children who had begun to read performed better overall than their same-age peers who had not. Children were more successful with my, or, and word-medial a than with an and word-initial a; embedded syllables tended to be easier than functors, especially for the younger subjects. In conclusion, the years from age 4½- to 6½ appear to be a period of vigorous development of word segmentation skills. During this period, children use increasingly complex strategies – first phrasal, then syllabic, and finally a full word strategy – and they demonstrate a growing knowledge of function words as well as content words. Functors vary in difficulty and developmental sequence due to differences in stress, amount of ambiguity with phonetically similar embedded syllables, semantic complexity, and salience of reference to the young child.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, R. R., & Brown, K. L. (1976). Developing communicative competence in children (A report of the Speech Communication Association's national project on speech communication competencies). Skokie, IL: National Textbook.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986). Children's concept of word. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 1332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). A metacognitive framework for the development of first and second language skills. In Forrest-Pressley, D. L., MacKinnon, G. E., & Waller, T. G. (Eds.), Meta-cognition, cognition, and human performance. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Berthoud-Papandropoulou, I. (1978). An experimental study of children's ideas about language. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. J., & Levelt, W. J. M. (Eds.), The child's conception of language. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Tunmer, W. E. (1984). Word awareness in children. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Brown, M. W. (1947). Goodnight moon. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chancy, C. F., & Estrin, E. T. (1987, November). Metalinguistic awareness. Miniseminar presented at American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Anderson, E. S. (1979, 03). Spontaneous repairs: Awareness in the process of acquiring language. Paper presented at Symposium on Reflections on Metacognition, Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco. Cited in W. E. Tunmer & M. Herriman (1984), Development of metalinguistic abilities: A conceptual overview. In W. E. Tunmer, C. Pratt, & M. L. Herriman (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children (p. 18). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
DeHart, G., & Maratsos, M. P. (1984). Children's acquisition of presuppositional usages. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Pickar, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore: University Park.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1975). Word consciousness in readers and pre-readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 204212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1979). Linguistic insight: Threshold of reading acquisition. In Waller, T. G. & MacKinnon, G. E. (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Evans, M., Taylor, N., & Blum, I. (1979). Children's written language awareness and its relation to reading acquisition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11, 719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, syllables and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, N. A. (1976). Children's awareness of segmentation in speech and print. Reading, 10, 1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, N. W. M., Walker, R. F., & Gray, B. (1977). The language of children: A key to literacy. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Holden, M. H., & MacGinitie, W. H. (1972). Children's conception of word boundaries in speech and print. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 551557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In Fletcher, P. & Garmon, M. (Eds.), language acquisition: Studies in first language development (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University.Google Scholar
Kingston, A. J., Weaver, W. W., & Figa, L. E. (1972). Experiments in children's perception of words and word boundaries. In Greene, F. P. (Ed.), Investigations relating to mature reading. Milwaukee, WI: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
Marascuilo, L., & Levin, J. (1983). Multivariate statistics in the social sciences: A researcher's guide. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. (1974). Preschool children's use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development, 45, 446455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNinch, G. (1971). Auditory perceptual factors and measured first-grade reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 472492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNinch, G. (1974). Awareness of aural and visual word boundary within a sample of first graders. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 11271134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D. (1980). Beginning reader's concept of word. In Henderson, E. H. & Beers, J. W. (Eds.), Development and cognitive aspects of learning to spell: A reflection of word knowledge. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Nesdale, A. R., & Tunmer, W. E. (1984). Development of metalinguistic abilities: A methodological overview. In Thnmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Papandropoulou, I., & Sinclair, H. (1974). What is a word? Experimental study of children's ideas on grammar. Human Development, 17, 241258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Statview 512+. (1986). Calabasas, CA: Brainpower, Inc.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., Bowey, J. A., & Grieve, R. (1983). The development of young children's awareness of the word as a unit of spoken language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 567594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Herriman, M. (1984). Development of metalinguistic abilities: A conceptual overview. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.). (1984). Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
vanKleek, A., & Schuele, C. M. (1987). Precursors to literacy: Normal development. Topics in Language Disorders, 7, 1331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wepman, J. M., & Hass, W. (1969). A spoken word count. Chicago: Language Research Associates.Google Scholar