Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T22:03:05.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grammatical morphology in language–impaired children acquiring English or German as their first language: A functional perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Katrin Lidner*
Affiliation:
Universität München
Judith R. Johnston*
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
*
Katrin Lindner, Institut für Deutsche Philologie, Universität München, Schellingstr.3, D-8000 München 40, Germany
Judith R. Johnston, School of Audiology and Speech Sciences, University of British Columbia, 5804 Fairview Crescent, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z3 Canada

Abstract

Fourteen matched pairs of German-speaking and English-speaking language-impaired children, aged 4;5–6;11, were tested for their knowledge of grammatical morphology and expressive vocabulary, using the Grammatical Closure subtest of the ITPA or its German adaptation, Grammatik in the PET, and the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test or the Aktiver Wortschatztest. Children were matched pairwise on the basis of their chronological age, nonverbal IQ, and scores in a sentence imitation and a sentence production task. As predicted, the German-speaking language-impaired children earned higher scores in grammatical morphology and vocabulary than did the English-speaking children. These findings add to a growing body of literature that documents language-specific sensitivity to particular sorts of syntactic devices. They also suggest that the morphological difficulties seen in English-speaking language-impaired children stem in part from the minor functional role played by these forms during the early language learning years.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Angermaier, M. (1974). Psycholinguistischer Enrwicklungstest. Weinheirn/Basel: Beltz.Google Scholar
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). Cross-linguistic study of the development of interpretation strategies. Child Development, 11, 245299.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgermeister, B., Blum, L. B., & Lorge, I. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1988). Normale und gestörte Kindersprache. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekelman, B. L., & Aram, D. M. (1983). Syntactic findings in developmental verbal apraxia. Journal of Communicative Disorders, 16, 237250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, M. F. (1979). The Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Graves, M., & Koziol, S. (1971). Noun plural development in primary grade children. Child Development, 42, 11651173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimm, H., & Schöler, H. (1978). Heidelberger Sprachentwicklungstest. Braunschweig, Germany: Westermann.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R. (1988). Specific language disorders in the child. In Lass, N., McReynolds, L., Northern, J., & Yoder, D. (Eds.), Handbook of speech pathology and audiology (pp. 685715). Philadelphia: Decker.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R., Ammon, M., & Kamhi, A. (1988). Language development chart. Topics in Language Disorders, 8, 4243.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R., & Kamhi, A. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 6586.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R., & Schery, T. (1976). The use of grammatical morphemes by children with communication disorders. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A. (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 239258). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Kaltenbacher, E. (1990). Strategien beim frühkindlichen Syntaxerwerb. Eine Entwicklungsstudie. Tübingen, Germany: Narr.Google Scholar
Kaltenbacher, E., & Kany, W. (1985). Kognitive Verarbeitungsstrategien und Syntaxerwerb bei dysphasischen und sprachunauffälligen Kindern. In Füssenich, I. & Gläss, B. (Eds.), Dysgrammatismus. Theoretische und praktische Probleme bei der interdisziplinären Beschreibung gestörter Kindersprache (pp. 180219). Heidelberg, Germany: HVA Edition Schiefele.Google Scholar
Kiese, C., & Kozielski, P.-M. (1979). Aktiver Wortschatztest für drei- bis sechsjährige Kinder. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Kirk, S., McCarthy, J. J., & Kirk, W. (1968). The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Ability. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development (whole no. 43).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, A. (1985). The acquisition of German. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 1, pp. 141254). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. (1977). Test of Language Development. Primary. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Schaner-Wolles, C. (1989). Plural- vs. Komparativerwerb im Deutschen. Von der Diskrepanz zwischen konzeptueller und morphologischer Entwicklung. In Guenther, H. (Ed.), Experimentelle Studien zur deutschen Flexionsmorphologie (pp. 155186). Hamburg, Germany: Buske.Google Scholar
Schöler, H., Anzer, A., & Illichmann, E. (1986). Flexion und Wortbildung. Ein Vergleich zwischen dysgrammatisch sprechenden und sprachunauffälligen Kindern. Der Sprachheilpädagoge, 18, 6274.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I., & Bever, T. (1982). Children's use of canonical sentence schemas: A cross- linguistic study. Cognition, 12, 229265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, W., & Stern, C. (1928). Die Kindersprache. Eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Tracy, R. (1987). The acquisition of verb placement in German. In Grifiths, P., Local, J., & Mills, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the child language seminar (pp. 8184). York, England: York University Press.Google Scholar
Weist, R. (1983). The word order myth. Journal of Child Language, 10, 97106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wettstein, P. (1983). Logopädischer Sprachverständnistest. Zürich: Heilpädagogisches Seminar.Google Scholar