Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:01:09.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2009

CAROLINE BOUCHARD*
Affiliation:
Université du Québec à Montréal
NATACHA TRUDEAU
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal et Hôpital Sainte-Justine
ANN SUTTON
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal et Hôpital Sainte-Justine
MARIE-CLAUDE BOUDREAULT
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal et Hôpital Sainte-Justine
JOANE DENEAULT
Affiliation:
Université du Québec à Rimouski
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Caroline Bouchard, Département d'éducation et pédagogie, Université du Québec à Montréal, C.P. 8888, Succursale Centre-ville, Montréal, Québec H3C 3P8, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The purpose of this article is to examine the language of girls and boys between 8 and 30 months of age, using the Quebec French version of The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. The findings from this parental report measure confirm those of earlier research, which showed the linguistic superiority of girls over boys at a young age. More specifically, the results show that girls produce significantly more words than boys; their utterances contain a greater number of grammatical forms, and are more complex syntactically. On the qualitative level, the data illustrate distinctive characteristics associated with gender in the acquisition of the first 100 words. These findings suggest that caution is necessary when assessing young children to interpret performance in light of factors that may contribute to it, including gender. These results are discussed in light of whether separate normative data are warranted for young boys and girls learning Canadian French.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1996). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), The handbook of child language. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., et al. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21, 85123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigras, N., Drouin, C., & Fournier, C. (2004). Conclusion générale. In Drouin, C., Bigras, N., Fournier, C., Desrosiers, H., & Bernard, S. (Eds.), Grandir en qualité 2003. Enquête québécoise sur la qualité des services de garde éducatifs. Québec: Institut de la statistique du Québec.Google Scholar
Bordens, K. S., & Abbott, B. B. (1996). Research design and methods: A process approach (3rd ed.). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L.R. (2005). Expressive vocabulary in language learners from two ecological settings in three language communities. Infancy, 7, 299316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2005). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24, 267304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boudreault, M.-C., Cabirol, A., Trudeau, N., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sutton, A. (2007). Les Inventaires MacArthur du Développement de la Communication: Validité et données normatives préliminaires. Revue Canadienne d'Orthophonie et d'Audiologie, 31, 2737.Google Scholar
Boudreault, M.-C., & Trudeau, N. (2005). Influence de la scolarité maternelle, du sexe et de la fréquentation d'un milieu de garde sur le développement lexical de l'enfant francophone. Poster presented at the 4th Annual Conference of the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network, Toronto.Google Scholar
Chiland, C. (1997). L'identité sexuée: Clinique et méthodologie. In Coslin, P.-G., Lebovici, S., & Stork, H.-E. (Eds.), Garçons et filles, hommes et femmes—Aspects pluridisciplinaires de l'identité sexuée (pp. 1940). Paris: PUF.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coates, J. (1993). The acquisition of gender-differentiated language. In Coates, J. (Ed.), Women, men and language: A sociolinguistic account of gender differences in language (2nd ed., pp. 143167). London: Longman.Google Scholar
Dolloghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Janosky, J. E., Pitcairn, D. N., et al. (1999). Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 14321443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative development. First Language, 19, 5590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, S., & Thal, D. (2000). Measuring variability in early child language: Don't shoot the messenger. Child Development, 71, 323328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). The MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Frank, I., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Trudeau, N. (1997). Inventaires MacArthur du développement de la communication: Mots et énoncés [Quebec French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Sentences]. Unpublished manuscript, Montréal.Google Scholar
Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3, 206215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gayraud, F., & Kern, S. (in press). De l'origine du phénomène de l'explosion lexicale chez le jeune enfant. Psychologie Française.Google Scholar
Grégoire, J. (2005). Aspects psychométriques de la construction d'épreuves d'évaluation. In Piérart, B. (Ed.), Le langage de l'enfant. Comment l'évaluer? Brussels: De Boeck.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. American Psychologist, 52, 10911102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilaire, G., Kern, S., Viguié, A., Dudognon, P., Langue, J., & Romieu, J. (2001). Le Pédiatre, 36, 182, 713.Google Scholar
Houston-Price, C., Mather, E., & Sakkalou, E. (2007). Discrepancy between parental reports of infants’ receptive vocabulary and infants’ behaviour in a preferential looking task. Journal of Child Language, 34, 701724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, J. S. & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 5369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Intons-Petersons, M. (1988). Children's concept of gender. New York: Ablex.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & Gutienez-Clennen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 20, 523549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kern, S. (2003). Le compte-rendu parental au service de l'évaluation de la production lexicale des enfants français entre 16 et 30 mois. Glossa, 8, 4861.Google Scholar
Kern, S. (2007). Lexicon development in French-speaking infants. First Language, 27, 227250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, D. (1992). Sex differences in the brain. Scientific American, September, 119–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krüger, H. (1998). Les sexes mixtes et la structures des institutions. In Mosconi, N. (Ed.), Égalité des sexes en éducation et formation (pp. 177183). Paris: PUF.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. London: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Maital, S. L., Dromi, E., Sagi, A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory: Language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language, 27, 4367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masiéri, W. (1965). Notions essentielles de statistique et de calcul des probabilités. Paris: Sirey.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F., Sedey, A. L., & Miolo, G. (1995). Validity of parents report measures of vocabulary development for children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 38, 10371044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E., & Booth, C. L. (1995). Toddlers’ language development: Sex differences within social risk. Developmental Psychology, 31, 851865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otto, B. (2005). Language development in early childhood (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice–Hall.Google Scholar
Parisse, C., & LeNormand, M. (2000). How children build their morphosyntax: The case of children. Journal of Child Language, 27, 267292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1978). Langage et éducation. Brussels: Pierre Mardaga Éditeur.Google Scholar
Rubin, A., & Babbie, E. (1997). Research methods for social work (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Swaroop, J., Nanda, P., & Kang, T. K. (2001). Perceptual ability as correlate of age, sex and locale. Psycho-Lingua, 31, 131134.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. (2005). Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: Implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 244276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thodardottir, E., & Ellis-Weismer, S. (1996). Language assessment via parent report: Development of a screening instrument for Icelandic children. First Language, 16, 265285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudeau, N., Bacopoulos-Viau, A., Boudreault, M.-C., Boulanger, J., Marsolais, I., Michaud, A., et al. (2006). Version québécoise de l'IMDC: Normalisation et comparaisons interlinguistiques. Poster presented at the 5th Annual Conference of the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network, Charlottetown, Canada.Google Scholar
Trudeau, N., Boudreault, M.-C., Marsolais, I., Rioux, A. & Tousignant, J. (2005). Validité et fiabilité de la version québécoise de l'Inventaire MacArthur du développement de la communication. Poster presented at the 4th Annual Conference of the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Trudeau, N., Frank, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Une adaptation en français québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. Revue d'Orthophonie et d'Audiologie, 23, 6173.Google Scholar
Trudeau, N., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Frank, I. (1997). Inventaires MacArthur du développement de la communication: Mots et gestes [Quebec French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures]. Unpublished manuscript, Montréal.Google Scholar
Viding, E., Spinath, F. M., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite sex twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45, 315325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed