Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:29:59.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of onset density in preschool children: Implications for development of phonological awareness and phonological representation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2009

JUDITH G. FOY*
Affiliation:
Loyola Marymount University
VIRGINIA A. MANN
Affiliation:
University of California–Irvine
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Judith G. Foy, Department of Psychology, Loyola Marymount University, University Hall 4741, Los Angeles, CA 90045. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Neighborhood density influences adult performance on several word processing tasks. Some studies show age-related effects of density on children's performance, reflecting a developmental restructuring of the mental lexicon from holistic into segmental representations that may play a role in phonological awareness. To further investigate density effects and their implications for development of phonological awareness, we compared performance on dense and sparse onset words. We adapted these materials to three phonological awareness tests that were pretested on adults then administered to preschool children who were expected to vary in phonological awareness skills. For both the adults and the children who passed a phonological awareness screening task, dense onset neighborhoods were associated with slower reaction times and increased errors. A separate comparison of word repetition by the children who passed and who did not pass the phoneme awareness screening failed to provide evidence that lexical restructuring was a sufficient condition for the attainment of phonological awareness. Both groups of children more accurately repeated words from high onset density neighborhoods, regardless of the level of their phonological awareness. Thus, we find no evidence of either age- or ability-driven effects in children's performance, contradictory to a view that the attainment of phoneme awareness relates to developmental changes in the segmental representation of words in dense neighborhoods.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 255259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, R. W. (1998). Proto-literate knowledge: Antecedents and influences on phonological awareness and literacy. In Hulme, C. & Joshi, R. M. (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 153175). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. A. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading & Writing, 5, 6985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B., & Liberman, A. M. (1999). Meaninglessness, productivity and reading: Some observations about the relation between the alphabet and speech. In Oakhill, J. & Beard, R. (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 157173). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Young children's sensivity to probabilistic phototactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 183213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the mental lexicon. In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 6, pp. 535555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2003). Phonological neighbourhood density effects in a rhyme awareness task in 5-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 695710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Gliga, T. (2004). Common neural basis for phoneme processing in infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 13751387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 3660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2001). Does strength of phonological representations predict phonological awareness in preschool children? Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 301325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2003). Home literacy environment and phonological awareness: Differential effects for phoneme awareness and rhyme awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 5988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2004). Effects of phonotactic probability on phoneme awareness in adult readers. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego.Google Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2006). Changes in letter sound knowledge are associated with development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 143161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 468492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C. S., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 259273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The children's test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 563582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehtonen, A., & Treiman, R. (2007). Adults' knowledge of phoneme–letter relationships is phonology-based and flexible. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 95114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, J. S. (1992). A computational analysis of young children's lexicons: Research on spoken language processing (Tech. Rep. 8). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon (Rep. No. 6). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, V. A. (1998). Language problems: A key to early reading problems. In Wong, B. Y. L. (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 163201). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., & Foy, J. G. (2003). Speech development, phonological awareness, and letter knowledge in preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 149173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, V. A., & Foy, J. G. (2007). Speech production patterns and phonological awareness in preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 5174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V. A., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of german and american children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattys, S. L., Jusczyk, P. W., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Phonotactic and prosodic effects on word segmentation. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 465494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a series of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. T. (1975). Encoding sensitivities to phonological markedness and transitional probability: Evidence from spoonerisms. Human Communication Research, 1, 353363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munson, B., Swenson, C. L., & Manthei, S. (2005). Lexical and phonological organization in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 108124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, C. A., Zhang, Y., Nie, H., & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing the alphabetic reading. Cognition, 24, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1994). Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 579595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., & Bourassa, D. (2000). Children's written and oral spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 183204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1981). Phonology and the development of the lexicon: Evidence from children's errors. Journal of Child Language, 8, 239264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. (2002a). Influence of onset density on spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 270278.Google ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S. (2002b). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28, 735747.Google ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S., Armbruster, B., & Chu, S. (2004). Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: Effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 30, 514529.Google ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: Levels of processing in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 9, 325329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 274408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., Luce, P. A., Charles-Luce, J., & Kemmerer, D. (1997). Phonotactics and syllable stress: Implications for the processing of spoken nonsense words. Language and Speech, 40, 4762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S., & Sommers, M. S. (2003). The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 31, 491504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C. (2005). Speech perception in childhood. In Pisoni, D. B. & Remez, R. E. (Eds.), Handbook of speech perception (pp. 449468). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing, 16, 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C., Stavrinos, D., & Imai, S. (2005). Speech learning and the development of spoken word recognition. Paper presented at the ASA Workshop on Second Language Speech Learning, Vancouver, BC.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Williams, K. T. (1997). The Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. (1999). What time may tell: Towards a new conceptualization of developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 69, 328.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. M. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery—Revised, form H. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar