Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T23:28:02.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Communication patterns between parents and children: Comparing mothers and fathers in different learner contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2014

LARA PIERCE*
Affiliation:
McGill University
FRED GENESEE
Affiliation:
McGill University
KARINE GAUTHIER
Affiliation:
Montreal Children's Hospital
MARIE-EVE DUBOIS
Affiliation:
Concordia University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Lara Pierce, Department of Psychology, McGill University, 1205 Avenue Docteur-Penfield, Montreal, QC H3A 1B1, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Language use and joint attention (JA) strategies were examined during interactions between francophone mothers and fathers and either their birth children (n = 10) or their internationally adopted children from China (n = 8), once when the children were 15 months old and again at 20 months, on average. Results showed that mothers engaged in more JA episodes and tended to talk more with their children than did fathers; however, this was influenced by the language-learning situation of the child. Specifically, the adoptive parents engaged more with their children than did the birth parents, and the behaviors of the internationally adopted mothers and fathers were more similar to each other than to those of the birth parents, arguably to support their children's unique language-learning situation. However, in contrast to a previous study that examined JA with adoptive mothers, the adoptive fathers’ interaction styles with their children at 15 months were not related to children's vocabularies at 20 months as has been observed for mothers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In Golinkoff, R. M.et al. (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 115135). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, V., & Bernstein-Ratner, N. (1987). Patterns of maternal and paternal response to language-learners’ verbalizations. Paper presented at the American Speech and Hearing Association Convention, New Orleans. LA.Google Scholar
Arriaga, R. I., Fenton, L., Cronan, T., & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 209224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1979). Parent, child and reciprocal influences. American Psychologist, 34, 821826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brachfeld-Child, S., Simpson, T., & Izenson, N. (1988). Mothers’ and fathers’ speech to infants in a teaching situation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 9, 173180.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, i–vi, 1143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherry, L., & Lewis, M. (1976). Mothers and two-year-olds: A study of sex-differentiated aspects of verbal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 12, 278282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheskin, A. (1982). The use of language by hearing mothers of deaf children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 15, 145153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G. (1990). Maternal recasts and other contingent replies to language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 262274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G. (1994). Language interaction with atypical language learners. In Gallaway, C. (Ed.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 183196). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Friel-Patti, S. (1983). Mothers’ discourse adjustments to language-impaired and non-language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 360367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. G., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Five-year-olds’ interactions with fathers versus mothers. First Language, 16, 223242. doi:10.1177/014272379601604704CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominey, P. F., & Dodane, C. (2004). Indeterminacy in language acquisition: The role of child directed speech and joint attention. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 121145. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00056-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S. (1992). One parent, one language: An interactional approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brooks.Google Scholar
Frank, I., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Trudeau, N. (1997). Inventaire MacArthur du Développement de La Communication: Mots et énoncés. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Gauthier, K., Genesee, F., Dubois, M. E., & Kasparian, K. (2011). Communication patterns between internationally adopted children and their mothers: Implications for language development. Applied Psycholinguistics. Advance online publication. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000725Google Scholar
Geren, J., Snedeker, J., & Ax, L. (2005). Starting over: A preliminary study of early lexical and syntactic development in internationally adopted preschoolers. Seminars in Speech and Language, 26, 4453. doi:10.1055/s-2005-864215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleason, J. B. (1975). Fathers and other strangers: Men's speech to young children. In Dato, D. P. (Ed.), Georgetown University round table on language and linguistics (pp. 289297). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Glennen, S. (2002). Language development and delay in internationally adopted infants and toddlers: A review. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 11, 333339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glennen, S. (2005, February). New arrivals: speech and language assessment for internationally adopted infants and toddlers within the first months home. Seminars in Speech and Language. doi:10.1055/s-2005-864212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodz, N. S., Goodz, E., & Green, D. (1991). Mothers’ and fathers’ conversations with their young bilingual children. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Harris, S., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. D. (1996). Joint attention and language gains in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100, 608619.Google ScholarPubMed
Henggeler, S. W., & Watson, , , S. M. (1984). Verbal and nonverbal maternal controls in hearing mother–deaf child interaction. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 5, 319329. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(84)90005-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killarney, J., & McCluskey, K. (1981). Parent–infant conversations at age one: Their length, reciprocity, and contingency. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.Google Scholar
Kornhaber, M., & Marcos, H. (2000). Young children's communication with mothers and fathers: Functions and contents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 187210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., & Chapieski, M. L. (1989). Joint attention and infant toy exploration: Effects of Down syndrome and prematurity. Child Development, 60, 103118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyon, M. (1985). The verbal interaction of mothers and their pre-school hearing-impaired children: A preliminary investigation. Journal of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf, 9, 119129.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1990). The Child Language Data Exchange System: An update. Journal of Child Language, 17, 457472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, G., Fors, S., & Wood, S. (1990). Maternal directive behavior revisited. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 94, 398406.Google ScholarPubMed
Malone, M. J., & Guy, R. F. (1982). A comparison of mothers“ and fathers” speech to their 3-year-old sons. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 599608. doi:10.1007/BF01067614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marfo, K. (1991). Early intervention in transition. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Gleason, J. B. (1980). Parent–child interaction and the acquisition of lexical information during play. Developmental Psychology, 16, 404409. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.16.5.404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D. S., Neal, R., et al. (2000). Responding to JA across the 6- to 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 283298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paparella, T., & Kasari, C. (2004). Joint attention skills and language development in special needs populations: Translating research to practice. Infants & Young Children, 17, 269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollock, K., Price, J., & Fulmer, K. (2003). Speech–language acquisition in children adopted from China: A longitudinal investigation of two children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 1, 184193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratner, N. B. (1988). Patterns of parental vocabulary selection in speech to very young children. Journal of Child Language, 15, 481492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimpi, P. M., & Huttenlocher, J. (2007). Redirective labels and early vocabulary development. Journal of Child Language, 34, 845859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoneman, Z., & Brody, G. H. (1981). Two's company, three makes a difference: An Examination of mothers’ and fathers’ speech to their young children. Child Development, 52, 705707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, T. X., & Yang, Y. (2005). Language development of Chinese adoptees 18–35 months old. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 5768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (1998). Gender effects on Mexican-descent parents’ questions and scaffolding during toy play: A sequential analysis. First Language, 18 (53), 129147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences, 10, 6988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. E. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4 (12), 197211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudeau, N., Frank, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1997). Inventaire MacArthur du Développement de la Communication: Mots et gestes. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Tucker, I., Hughes, M., & Glover, M. (1983). Verbal interaction with pre-school hearing-impaired children: A comparison of maternal and paternal language input. Journal of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf, 7, 9098.Google Scholar
Vigil, D. C. (2002). Cultural variations in attention regulation: A comparative analysis of British and Chinese-immigrant populations. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders/Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists, 37, 433458. doi:10.1080/1368282021000007749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vigil, D. C., Tyler, A. A., & Ross, S. (2006). Cultural differences in learning novel words in an attention-following versus attention-directing style. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4, 5970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S. (2006). Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12241237. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/088)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed