Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T22:46:56.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Automatization in second language acquisition: What does the coefficient of variation tell us?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2009

JAN H. HULSTIJN*
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam
AMOS VAN GELDEREN
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam
ROB SCHOONEN
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Jan H. Hulstijn, Faculty of Humanities, University of Amsterdam, 134 Spuistraat, Amsterdam 1012 VB, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Segalowitz and Segalowitz distinguish between “speedup” (mean reaction time [RT] and mean standard deviation of responses in an RT task decrease to the same degree) and “automatization” (mean standard deviation decreases more than mean RT). The coefficient of variation, which is the standard deviation divided by the mean RT, decreases in the case of automatization while remaining unchanged in the case of speedup. We present data that are collected in two studies. The first one is a longitudinal study spanning 2 years and comprising four RT tasks, both in second language (L2) English and first language Dutch (N > 200). The second study is an English L2 word training study. Students (N = 41) performed a lexical decision task before and after training. Convincing evidence for automatization was not found in either study. The main problems in testing the Segalowitz and Segalowitz hypothesis is that gains in knowledge itself and gains in processing it cannot be adequately disentangled in the RT tasks currently used, characterized by a speed–accuracy trade-off. Although conceptually skill acquisition can be distinguished from knowledge accumulation, in reality, knowledge accumulation forms part of skill acquisition because, in real L2 learning, exposure to new words goes hand in hand with exposure to words encountered previously.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Acock, A. C. (1997). Working with missing values. Family Science Review, 10, 76102.Google Scholar
Akamatsu, N. (2008). The effects of training on automatization of word recognition in English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., & Cortese, M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning (A travel update). In Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 285375). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 181199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A. (2005). Lexical access in bilingual production. In Kroll, J. F. & de, A. M. B. Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 308325). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Glopper, K., van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., Hulstijn, J., Snellings, P., Stevenson, M., et al. (2004). Project NELSON. Retrieved November 23, 2007, from http://www.scokohnstamminstituut.uva.nl/nelson/index.htmGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Skill acquisition theory. In VanPatten, B. & Williams, J., Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 97113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Oxford: Brooks Cole.Google Scholar
Fukkink, R. G., Hulstijn, J., & Simis, A. (2005). Does training of second-language word recognition skills affect reading comprehension? An experimental study. The Modern Language Journal, 89, 5475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (Eds.). (2008). Second language acquisition: An introductory course (3rd ed.). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, M. (2006). The lexical decision task as a measure of L2 proficiency. EUROSLA Yearbook, 6, 147168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, M., & Myles, M. (Eds.). (2004). Second language learning theories (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Phillips, A., Segalowitz, N., O'Brien, I., & Yamasaki, N. (2004). Semantic priming in a first and second language: Evidence from reaction time variability and event-related brain potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 237262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Gomez, P., & McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. Psychological Review, 111, 159182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. B. (1996). Multiple imputation after 18+ years (with discussion). Journal of the American Statistical Association, 91, 473489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoonen, R., Van Gelderen, A., De Glopper, K., Hulstijn, J., Simis, A., Snellings, P., et al. (2003). First language and second language writing: The role of linguistic fluency, linguistic knowledge and metacognitive knowledge. Language Learning, 53, 165202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoonen, R., Van Gelderen, A., De Glopper, K., Hulstijn, J., Snellings, P., Simis, A., et al. (2002). Linguistic knowledge, metacognitive knowledge and retrieval speed in L1, L2 and EFL writing: A structural equation modeling approach. In Ransdell, S. & Barbier, M.-L. (Eds.), New directions for research in L2 writing (pp. 101122). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoonen, R., Van Gelderen, A., Stoel, R., Hulstijn, J., & De Glopper, K. (2009). Modelling writing development: L1 and EFL writing proficiency in secondary school years. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Schwartz, A. I., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). Language processing in bilingual speakers. In Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 967999). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. (2000). Automaticity and attentional skill in fluent performance. In Riggenbach, H. (Ed.), Perspectives on fluency (pp. 200219). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, N. (2003). Automaticity and second language learning. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 382408). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. S., & Freed, B. F. (2004). Context, contact, and cognition in oral fluency acquisition: Learning Spanish in at home and study abroad contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 173199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. S., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1993). Skilled performance, practice, and the differentiation of speed-up from automatization effects: Evidence from second language word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 369385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. S., Watson, V., & Segalowitz, S. (1995). Vocabulary skill: Single case assessment of automaticity of word recognition in a timed lexical decision task. Second Language Research, 11, 121136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, S. J., Segalowitz, N. S., & Wood, A. G. (1998). Assessing the development of automaticity in second language word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 5367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speyer, R., Wieneke, G. H., & Dejonckere, P. H. (2004). The use of acoustic parameters for the evaluation of voice therapy for dysphonic patients. Acta Acustica, 90, 520527.Google Scholar
Van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., De Glopper, K., Hulstijn, J., Simis, A., Snellings, P., et al. (2004). Linguistic knowledge, processing speed, and metacognitive knowledge in first- and second-language reading comprehension: A componential analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 1930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., De Glopper, K., Hulstijn, J., Snellings, P., Simis, A., et al. (2003) Roles of linguistic knowledge, metacognitive knowledge and processing speed in L3, L2 and L1 reading comprehension: A structural equation modeling approach. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7, 725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., Stoel, R. D., De Glopper, K., & Hulstijn, J. (2007). Development of adolescent reading comprehension in language 1 and language 2: A longitudinal analysis of constituent components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 477491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Brown, S. (2007). On the linear relation between the mean and the standard deviation of a response time distribution. Psychological Review, 114, 830841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willems, M. M., & Oud-de Glas, M. M. B. (1990). Vocabulaire selectie voor het vreemde-talenonderwijs [Selection of vocabulary for foreign language education]. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Instituut voor Toegepaste Sociologie.Google Scholar
Woutersen, M. (1997). Bilingual word perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen.Google Scholar