Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T23:54:25.689Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The interpretation of Spanish masculine plural NPs: Are they perceived as uniformly masculine or as a mixture of masculine and feminine?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 November 2022

Alejandro Anaya-Ramírez
Affiliation:
Centro de Investigación en Ciencias Cognitivas, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca, Morelos, Mexico
John Grinstead
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish and Portuguese, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
Melissa Nieves Rivera
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish and Portuguese, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
David Melamed
Affiliation:
Translational Data Analytics Institute, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
Asela Reig-Alamillo*
Affiliation:
Centro de Investigación en Ciencias Cognitivas, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca, Morelos, Mexico
*
*Corresponding autor. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This article investigates whether human masculine plural noun phrases (NPs) in Spanish, which can be interpreted with an exclusively masculine or a mixed-gender meaning, are a case of balanced or unbalanced ambiguity. The results of an experiment using a sentence continuation task with oral stimuli are consistent with the claim that masculine grammatical gender biases listeners toward an exclusively masculine interpretation. The acceptance rate of continuations with the pronoun uno/una referring to a masculine plural antecedent showed that the exclusively masculine meaning of the NP is accessed more frequently and involves a lower cognitive cost than the mixed-gender interpretation. Further, this effect interacts with the stereotypicality of the noun: nouns independently established to carry a masculine stereotype are less likely to be associated with a mixed-gender interpretation. The study also found that the speakers’ attitudes toward nonsexist language predict their acceptance of the mixed-gender interpretation of masculine NPs.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binder, K. S., & Morris, R. K. (1995). Eye movements and lexical ambiguity resolution: Effects of prior encounter and discourse topic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 21(5), 11861196.Google ScholarPubMed
Braun, F., Gottburgsen, A., Sczesny, S., & Stahlberg, D. (1998). Können Geophysiker Frauen sein? Generische Personenbezeichnungen im Deutschen [Can geophysicians be women? Generic terms in German]. Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik, 26(3), 265283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Garnham, A., Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (1996). The use of stereotypical gender information in constructing a mental model: Evidence from English and Spanish. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(3), 639663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, S. A., Kambe, G., & Rayner, K. (2001). The effect of prior disambiguating context on the comprehension of ambiguous words: Evidence from eye movements. In Gorfein, D. S. (Ed.), Decade of behavior. On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity (pp. 2743). American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, S. A., & Keir, J. A. (2004). Violating stereotypes: Eye movements and comprehension processes when text conflicts with world knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 551559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esaulova, Y., Chiara, R., & von Stockhausen, L. (2014). Influences of grammatical and stereotypical gender during Reading: Eye movements in pronominal and noun phrase anaphor resolution, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(7), 781803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, U. (2008). Language policies and in-group favoritism: The malleability of the interpretation of generically intended masculine forms. Social Psychology, 39(2), 103107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, U., Behne, D. M., & Gygax, P. M. (2017). Speech vs. reading comprehension: An explorative study of gender representations in Norwegian. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(7), 795808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, U., Gygax, P. M., & Kuhn, E. A. (2018). Neutralising linguistic sexism: Promising but cumbersome? Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 21(5), 844858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadsby, N., Arnott, W., & Copland, D. A. (2008). An investigation of working memory influences on lexical ambiguity resolution. Neuropsychology, 22(2), 209216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnham, A., Gabriel, U., Sarrasin, O., Gygax, P., & Oakhill, J. (2012). Gender representation in different languages and grammatical marking on pronouns: When beauticians, musicians and mechanics remain men. Discourse Processes, 49(6), 481500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnham, A., Oakhill, J., & Reynolds, D. (2002). Are inferences from stereotyped role names to characters’ gender made elaboratively? Memory & Cognition, 30(3), 439446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnham, A., & Yakovlev, Y. (2015). The interaction of morphological and stereotypical gender information in Russian. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gygax, P., & Gabriel, U. (2008). Can a group of musicians be composed of women? Generic interpretation of French masculine role names in absence and presence of feminine forms. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67(3), 141153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gygax, P., Gabriel, U., Lévy, A., Pool, E., Grivel, M., & Pedrazzini, E. (2012). The masculine form and its competing interpretations in French: When linking grammatically masculine role names to female referents is difficult, Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 395408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gygax, P., Gabriel, U., Sarrasin, O., Oakhill, J., & Garnham, A. (2008) Generically intended, but specifically interpreted: When beauticians, musicians and mechanics are all men. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(3), 464485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irmen, L. (2007). What’s in a (Role) Name? Formal and conceptual aspects of comprehending personal nouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(6), 431456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irmen, L., & Rossberg, N. (2004). Gender markedness of language: The impact of grammatical and nonlinguistic information on the mental representation of person information. Journal of language and Social Psychology, 23(3), 272307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irmen, L., & Schumann, E. (2011). Processing grammatical gender of role nouns: Further evidence from eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(8), 9981014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parks, J. B., & Roberton, M. A. (2000). Development and validation of an instrument to measure attitudes toward sexist/nonsexist language. Sex Roles, 42, 415438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prentice, D. A. (1994). Do language reforms change our way of thinking? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13(1), 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., Hyönä, J., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2010). Activating gender stereotypes during online spoken language processing. Evidence from visual world eye tracking. Experimental Psychology, 57(2), 126133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redl, T., Eerland, A., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2018). The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn ‘his’ across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study. PLOS ONE, 13(10), e0205903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothmund, J., & Scheele, B. (2004). Personenbezeichnungsmodelle auf dem Prüfstand. Lösungsmöglichkeiten für das Genus-Sexus-Problem auf Textebene. [Putting gender-neutral reference terms to the test: Constructive solutions to the problem of grammatical vs. referential gender on the textual level]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 212(1), 4054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sleeman, P., & Ihsane, T. (2016). Gender mismatches in partitive constructions with superlatives in French. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 1(1), 35, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., & Sczesny, S. (2007). Representation of the sexes in language. In Fiedler, K. (Ed.), Social communication. A volume in the series. Frontiers of Social Psychology (pp. 163187). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Stahlberg, D., & Sczesny, S. (2001). Effekte des generischen Maskulinums und alternativer Sprachformen auf den gedanklichen Einbezug von Frauen. Psychologische Rundschau, 52(3), 131140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar