Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T05:41:34.622Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Instructive bilingualism: Can bilingual children with specific language impairment rely on one language in learning a second one?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2010

Sharon Armon-Lotem*
Affiliation:
Bar-Ilan University

Extract

Only a decade ago, a very few researchers considered the study of language disorders in bilingual population worth pursuing. It was mostly argued that there were enough challenges in studying bilingualism, and even more challenges in the study of specific language impairment (SLI). So why complicate things and combine the two domains?

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Armon-Lotem, S. (2010). Between L2 and SLI: Inflections and prepositions in the Hebrew of bilingual children with TLD and monolingual children with SLI. Unpublished manuscript, Bar-Ilan University.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Adam, G., Blass, A., Fine, J., Harel, E., Saiegh-Haddad, E., et al. (2010). Verb inflections as indicators of bilingual SLI: Qualitative vs. quantitative measurements. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Danon, G., & Walters, J. (2008). The use of prepositions by bilingual SLI children: The relative contribution of representation and processing. Paper presented at the Generative Assembly on Language Acquisition, Bar-Ilan University, Israel.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Gordishevsky, G., & Walters, J. (2009). Preposition use among Russian–Hebrew bilingual children with specific language impairment. Paper presented at Language, Brain, Communication, Hadassa College, Jerusalem, Israel.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., & Walters, J. (2009). Preposition use among bilingual children with SLI. Paper presented at the Research Conference on Bilingualism and SLI, Jerusalem, Israel.Google Scholar
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 10091018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2007). Cognitive effects of bilingualism: How linguistic experience leads to cognitive change. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 210223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botwinik-Rotem, I. (2004). The category P: Features, projections, interpretation. Doctoral dissertation, University of Orgeon.Google Scholar
Central Bureau of Statistics. (2006). Retrieved from http://www1.cbs.gov.il [in Hebrew]Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Bartke, S., & Göllner, S. (1997). Formal features in impaired grammars: A comparison of English and German SLI children. Journal of Neurolinguistics 10, 151171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crago, M., & Paradis, J. (2003). Two of a kind? Commonalities and variation in languages and language learners. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 97110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
De Avila, E., & Duncan, S. (1980). The language minority child: A psychological, linguistic, and social analysis. In Alatis, J. (Ed.), Current issues in bilingual education. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Dromi, E., Leonard, L., Adam, G., & Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S. (1999). Verb agreement morphology in Hebrew-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 14141431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dromi, E., Leonard, L., & Shteiman, M. (1993). The grammatical morphology of Hebrew speaking children with specific language impairment: Some competing hypotheses. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 760771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., Restrepo, A., & Simón-Cereijido, G. (2006). Evaluating the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12091223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., & Simón-Cereijido, G. (2007). The discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Latino English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 968981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iluz-Cohen, P. (2008). Language proficiency, language control and executive control in bilingual children. Master's thesis, Bar-Ilan University, Israel.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (1999). What do specifically-language impaired and second language children have in common. Child Language Bulletin, 19, 46.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2007). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: Theoretical and applied issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 551564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2008). Tense as a clinical marker in English L2 acquisition with language delay/impairment. In Gavruseva, E. & Haznedar, B. (Eds.), Current trends in child second language acquisition: A generative perspective (pp. 337356). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2000). Tense and temporality: Similarities and differences between language-impaired and second-language children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 834848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2005/2006). Domain-specific versus domain-general theories of the deficit in SLI: Object pronoun acquisition by French–English bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 13, 3362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., & Crago, M. (2007). French–English bilingual children's acquisition of the past tense. In Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S., & Woo, I.-H. (Eds.), BUCLD 31 Proceedings (pp. 497507). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). An organismic process model of Witkin's field-dependence-independence. In Globerson, T. & Zelniker, T. (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child's conception of space. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2004). Growth models of developmental language disorders. In Rice, M. L. & Warren, S. (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T. (2009). Multiple grammars and SLI. Paper presented at the Research Conference on Bilingualism and SLI, Jerusalem, Israel.Google Scholar
Roeper, T., Ramos, E., Seymour, H., & Lamya, A. (2001). Language disorders as a window on universal grammar: An abstract theory of agreement for IP, DP and V-PP. Master's thesis, University of Massachusetts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimon, E. (2008). The use of prepositions by English–Hebrew bilinguals and bilingual children with SLI. Master's thesis, Bar-Ilan University, Israel.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. (2001). Interpretability and language development: A study of verbal and nominal features in normally developing and SLI Greek children. Brain and Language, 77, 432452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watkins, R., & Rice, M. (1991).Verb particle and preposition acquisition in language impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 11301141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, J., Armon-Lotem, S., Gagarina, N., & Remennick, L. (2009). Language acquisition as a window to social integration among Russian language minority children in Germany and Israel. Paper presented at the Migration and Societal Integration Consortium, Jena, Germany.Google Scholar