Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T01:16:16.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of phonemic coding strategies for serial recall

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 1999

Susan Nittrouer*
Affiliation:
Boys Town National Research Hospital
Marnie E. Miller
Affiliation:
Boys Town National Research Hospital
*
Susan Nittrouer, Boys Town National Research Hospital, 555 North 30th Street, Omaha, NE 68131. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined differences between adults and children and between normal and poor readers in the use of phonemic coding strategies for storing words in working memory. In the first experiment, adults, 11-year-olds, and 8-year-olds (categorized as normal or poor readers) recalled eight-item strings of rhyming and nonrhyming words. A developmental decrease in errors was observed for adults, 11-year-olds, and normal-reading 8-year-olds that reflected an improvement in the phonemic coding of items in working memory, but no difference was found between normal- and poor-reading 8-year-olds in the use of phonemic coding strategies. A second experiment with shorter lists and more children supported the latter finding. The results were interpreted as demonstrating that the ability to access syllable-internal phonemic structure is a necessary precursor to the development of phonemic coding strategies for working memory, but that the use of that structure for storing words in working memory is a skill that develops independently and later than the ability to access phonemic structure.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362365.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of shortterm memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575589.Google Scholar
Bar-Shalom, E. G., Crain, S., & Shankweiler, D. P. (1993). A comparison of comprehension and production abilities of good and poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 197227.Google Scholar
Brady, S., Shankweiler, D., & Mann, V. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 345367.Google Scholar
Brady, S., Poggie, E., & Rapala, M. M. (1989). Speech repetition abilities in children who differ in reading skill. Language and Speech, 32, 109122.Google Scholar
Byrne, B. (1981). Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: Is a weak phonetic memory code responsible? Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 201212.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., & Dodd, B. (1980). Hearing by eye. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 8599.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., Dodd, B., & Brasher, J. (1983). The sources of visual recency: Movement and language in serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 571587.Google Scholar
Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429432.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1989). Why poor readers misunderstand spoken sentences. In Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.), Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 133– 165). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S., & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Modularity and learning to read. In Mattingly, I. G. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (Eds.), Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception (pp. 375– 392). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Shankweiler, D., Macaruso, P., & Bar-Shalom, E. (1990). Working memory and sentence comprehension: Investigations of children with reading disorder. In Vallar, G. & Shallice, T. (Eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory (pp. 477508). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (1974). Acoustic memory and perception of speech. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 4160.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ferguson, G. A. (1981). Statistical analysis in psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Hall, J. W., Wilson, K. P., Humphreys, M. S., Tinzmann, M. B., & Bowyer, P. M. (1983). Phonemic- similarity effects in good vs. poor readers. Memory and Cognition, 11, 520527.Google Scholar
Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). The Wide Range Achievement Test–Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.Google Scholar
Katz, R. B., Shankweiler, D. P., & Liberman, I. Y. (1981). Memory for item order and phonetic recoding in the beginning reader. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 474484.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., Cowin, E., & Schoenheimer, J. (1989). Phonological processing, language comprehension, and reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 7689.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 592599.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995a). Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language- impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 355378.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995b). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.Google Scholar
Nunnally, J. M., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw- Hill.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 17531778.Google Scholar
Pratt, A. C., & Brady, S. (1988). Relation of phonological awareness to reading disability in children and adults. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 319323.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1975). Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Salame, P., & Baddeley, A. (1986). Phonological factors in STM: Similarity and the unattended speech effect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 24, 263265.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D. P., Liberman, I. Y., Mark, L. S., Fowler, C. A., & Fischer, F. W. (1979). The speech code and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 531545.Google Scholar
Smith, S. T., Macaruso, P., Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1989). Syntactic comprehension in young poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 429454.Google Scholar
Smith, S. T., Mann, V. A., & Shankweiler, D. (1986). Spoken language comprehension by good and poor readers: A study with the Token Test. Cortex, 22, 627632.Google Scholar
Spoehr, K. T., & Corin, W. J. (1978). The stimulus suffix effect as a memory coding phenomenon. Memory and Cognition, 6, 583589.Google Scholar
Spring, C., & Perry, L. (1983). Naming speed and serial recall in poor and adequate readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 141145.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175190.Google Scholar
Stein, C. L., Cairns, H. S., & Zurif, E. B. (1984). Sentence comprehension limitations related to syntactic deficits in reading-disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 305322.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (1995). Errors in short-term memory for speech: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 11971206.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Danis, C. (1988). Short-term memory errors for spoken syllables are affected by the linguistic structure of the syllables. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 145152.Google Scholar
Vogel, S. A. (1975). Syntactic abilities in normal and dyslexic children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K. (1986). Phonological processing abilities and reading: Implications for disabled readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19, 623630.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar