Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T21:15:02.853Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention-getting skills of deaf children using American Sign Language in a preschool classroom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 January 2014

AMY M. LIEBERMAN*
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Amy M. Lieberman, Center for Research on Language, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive No. 0108, La Jolla, CA 92093-0108. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Visual attention is a necessary prerequisite to successful communication in sign language. The current study investigated the development of attention-getting skills in deaf native-signing children during interactions with peers and teachers. Seven deaf children (aged 21–39 months) and five adults were videotaped during classroom activities for approximately 30 hr. Interactions were analyzed in depth to determine how children obtained and maintained attention. Contrary to previous reports, children were found to possess a high level of communicative competence from an early age. Analysis of peer interactions revealed that children used a range of behaviors to obtain attention with peers, including taps, waves, objects, and signs. Initiations were successful approximately 65% of the time. Children followed up failed initiation attempts by repeating the initiation, using a new initiation, or terminating the interaction. Older children engaged in longer and more complex interactions than younger children. Children's early exposure to and proficiency in American Sign Language is proposed as a likely mechanism that facilitated their communicative competence.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, D., & Reilly, J. (2002). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Normative data for American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7, 83106.Google Scholar
Antia, S. D. (1982). Social interaction of partially mainstreamed hearing-impaired children. American Annals of the Deaf, 127, 1825.Google Scholar
Antia, S. D., & Dittillo, D. A. (1998). A comparison of the peer social behavior of children who are deaf/hard of hearing and hearing. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 19, 110.Google Scholar
Antia, S. D., & Kreimeyer, K., H. (2003). Peer interactions of deaf and hard of hearing children. In Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education: Vol. 1 (pp. 164176). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baker, C. (1977). Regulators and turn-taking in American Sign Language discourse. In Friedman, L. (Ed.), On the other hand: New perspectives on American Sign Language (pp. 215236). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bergeson, T. R., Pisoni, D. B., & Davis, R. A. O. (2003). A longitudinal study of audiovisual speech perception by children with hearing loss who have cochlear implants. Volta Review, 103, 347370.Google ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coates, J., & Sutton-Spence, R. (2001). Turn-taking patterns in deaf conversation. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 5, 507529.Google Scholar
Courtin, C. (2000). The impact of sign language on the cognitive development of deaf children: The case of theories of mind. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 266276.Google Scholar
Deluzio, J., & Girolametto, L. (2011). Peer interactions of preschool children with and without hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 11971210.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, 1189.Google Scholar
Gertner, B. L., Rice, M. L., & Hadley, P. A. (1994). Influence of communicative competence on peer preferences in a preschool classroom Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 913923.Google Scholar
Griffin, A., Solit, G., & Bodner-Johnson, B. (1991). Gallaudet University Child Development Center's model integration program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6, 5156.Google Scholar
Guralnick, M. J. (1981). Peer influences on the development of communicative competence. In Strain, P. S. (Ed.), The utilization of classroom peers as behavior change agents (pp. 3168). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Harris, M., Clibbens, J., Chasin, J., & Tibbitts, R. (1989). The social context of early sign language development. First Language, 9, 8197.Google Scholar
Howes, D., & Clements, D. (1994). Adult socialization of children's play in child care. In Goelman, H. & Jacobs, E. V. (Eds.), Children's play in child care settings (pp. 2036). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keating, E., & Mirus, G. (2003). Examining interactions across language modalities, Deaf children and hearing peers at school. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 34, 115135.Google Scholar
Koester, L. S., Karkowski, A. M., & Traci, M. A. (1998). How do deaf and hearing mothers regain eye contact when their infants look away? American Annals of the Deaf, 143, 513.Google Scholar
Lederberg, A. R. (1991). Social interaction among deaf preschoolers: The effects of language ability and age. American Annals of the Deaf, 136, 5359.Google Scholar
Lieberman, A. M., Hatrak, M., & Mayberry, R. I. (2014). Learning to look for language. Development of joint attention in young deaf children. Language Learning and Development, 10, 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy-Shiff, R., & Hoffman, M. A. (1985). Social behavior of hearing-impaired and normally-hearing preschoolers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 111118.Google Scholar
McKee, R. L., Johnson, K., & Marbury, N. (1991). Attention-getting strategies of deaf children at the dinner table. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 2, 239268.Google Scholar
McKirdy, L. S., & Blank, M. (1982). Dialogue in deaf and hearing preschoolers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 25, 487.Google Scholar
Meadow-Orlans, K. P., Spencer, P., & Koester, L. (2004). The world of deaf infants: A longitudinal study. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Messenheimer-Young, T., & Kretschmer, R. (1994). “Can I Play?” A hearing-impaired preschooler's requests to access maintained social interaction. Volta Review, 96, 518.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard-of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4, 138163.Google Scholar
Mohay, H. (2000). Language in sight: Mothers’ strategies for making language visually accessible to deaf children. In Spencer, P. E., Erting, C. J., & Marschark, M. (Eds.), The deaf child in the family and at school: Essays in honor of Kathryn P. Meadow-Orlans (pp. 151166). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mohay, H., Milton, L., Hindmarsh, G., & Ganley, K. (1998). Deaf mothers as communication models for hearing families with deaf children. In Weisel, A. (Ed.), Issues unresolved: New perspectives on language and deaf education (pp. 7687). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 603613.Google Scholar
Padden, C., & Humphries, T. (1988). Deaf in America: Voices from a culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, H. S., Lollis, S. P., & Elliott, C. (1982). Toddler-peer communication. In Rubin, K. H. & Ross, H. S. (Eds.), Peer relationships and social skills in childhood (pp. 7398). New York: Springer–Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleper, D. R. (1995). Read it again and again . . . and again. Perspectives in Education and Deafness, 14 (2), 1619.Google Scholar
Singleton, J. L., & Crume, P. K. (2010, July). Socializing visual engagement in early childhood deaf education. Poster session presented at the 21st International Congress on the Education of the Deaf, Vancouver, BC, Canada.Google Scholar
Spencer, P. E., & Deyo, D. A. (1993). Cognitive and social aspects of deaf children's play. In Marschark, M. & Clark, M. D. (Eds.), Psychological perspective on deafness (pp. 6591). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spencer, P. E., Koester, L. S., & Meadow-Orlans, K. P. (1994). Communicative interactions of deaf and hearing children in a day care center: An exploratory study. American Annals of the Deaf, 139, 512518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, P., & Lederberg, A. (1997). Different modes, different models: Communication and language of young deaf children and their mothers. In Adamson, L. and Romski, M. (Eds.), Communication and language: Discoveries from atypical development (pp. 203230). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Swisher, M. V. (2000). Learning to converse: How deaf mothers support the development of attention and conversational skills in their young deaf children. In Spencer, P., Erting, C. J., & Marschark, M. (Eds.), The deaf child in the family and at school (pp. 2140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Vandell, D. L., & George, L. B. (1981). Social interaction in hearing and deaf preschoolers: Successes and failures in initiations. Child Development, 52, 627635.Google Scholar
Van der Lem, G. J., & Timmerman, G. E. (1995). Joint picture book reading in signs. An interaction process between parent and deaf child. In Nelson, K. E. & Reger, Z. (Eds.), Children's language (Vol. 8, pp. 171180). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Weisel, A., Most, R., & Efron, C. (2005). Initiations of social interactions by young hearing impaired preschoolers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10, 161170.Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. B., & Petitto, L. A. (1981). How to know a conversation when you see one: Discourse structure in American Sign Language conversations. Journal of the National Student Speech–Language–Hearing Association, 9, 6681.Google Scholar