Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T10:25:22.704Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal behaviour and peripartum levels of oestradiol and progesterone show little difference in Merino ewes selected for calm or nervous temperament under indoor housing conditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2010

S. Bickell
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
P. Poindron
Affiliation:
INRA, UMR85 Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, F-37380 Nouzilly, France CNRS, UMR6175 Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, F-37380 Nouzilly, France Université François Rabelais de Tours, F-37041 Tours, France Haras Nationaux, F-37380 Nouzilly, France
R. Nowak
Affiliation:
INRA, UMR85 Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, F-37380 Nouzilly, France CNRS, UMR6175 Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, F-37380 Nouzilly, France Université François Rabelais de Tours, F-37041 Tours, France Haras Nationaux, F-37380 Nouzilly, France
D. Ferguson
Affiliation:
CSIRO Livestock Industries, Livestock Welfare, Locked Bag 1 Armidale, NSW 2350, Australia
M. Blackberry
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
D. Blache*
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
*
Get access

Abstract

Temperament influences the expression of maternal behaviour in sheep under experimental field conditions. We investigated whether maternal behaviour between ewes selected for a calm or nervous temperament is independent from environmental conditions. In addition, the level of maternal behaviour expressed by mothers is correlated with the concentration of hormones during the peripartum period. Therefore, we investigated whether the selection for temperament had resulted in hormonal differences between the two lines with regard to the hormones that could be involved in the onset of maternal behaviour. Oestradiol, progesterone and cortisol concentrations from 4 days before parturition to 24 h after parturition were determined from blood samples collected from 10 calm and 12 nervous ewes. Behavioural interactions between ewe and lamb were also recorded for 2 h starting at parturition. Mothers of both temperament lines showed adequate maternal behaviour under the controlled conditions of the study. Therefore, the results of the study do not suggest that selection for a calm or nervous temperament has profoundly affected the intrinsic ability of mothers to display adequate maternal behaviour. Hormonal differences between the two temperament lines were generally small and their possible influence on the display of maternal behaviour in the two temperament lines would have to be demonstrated.

Type
Full Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Animal Consortium 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beausoleil, NJ, Blache, D, Stafford, KJ, Mellor, DJ, Noble, ADL 2008. Exploring the basis of divergent selection for ‘temperament’ in domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109, 261274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickell, S, Poindron, P, Nowak, R, Chadwick, A, Ferguson, D, Blache, D 2009. Genotype rather than non-genetic behavioural transmission determines the temperament of Merino lambs. Animal Welfare 18, 459466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blache, D, Ferguson, D 2005. Genetic estimates for temperament traits in sheep breeds. Meat and Livestock Australia, Sydney.Google Scholar
Broom, DM, Johnson, KD 1993. Stress and animal welfare. Chapman and Hall, London, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, CM, Lawrence, AB 1999. Does the behaviour of the neonate influence the expression of maternal behaviour in sheep? Behaviour 136, 367389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, CM, Gilbert, CL, Lawrence, AB 2004. Prepartum plasma estradiol and postpartum cortisol, but not oxytocin, are associated with interindividual and breed differences in the expression of maternal behaviour in sheep. Hormones and Behavior 46, 529543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, CM, McLean, KA, Deans, LA, Chirnside, J, Calvert, SK, Lawrence, AB 1998. Vocalisations between mother and young in sheep: effects of breed and maternal experience. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58, 105119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, AL, Hutson, GD 1990. Changes in heart-rate, plasma-cortisol and haematocrit of sheep during a shearing procedure. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 26, 91101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendrick, KM, Keverne, EB 1991. Importance of progesterone and estrogen priming for the induction of maternal behavior by vaginocervical stimulation in sheep: effects of maternal experience. Physiology and Behavior 49, 745750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendrick, KM, Da Costa, APC, Broad, KD, Ohkura, S, Guevara, R, Levy, F, Keverne, EB 1997. Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Research Bulletin 44, 383395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiley, M 1972. The vocalisations of ungulates, their causation and function. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 31, 171222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meurisse, M, Gonzalez, A, Delsol, G, Caba, M, Levy, F, Poindron, P 2005. Estradiol receptor-[alpha] expression in hypothalamic and limbic regions of ewes is influenced by physiological state and maternal experience. Hormones and Behavior 48, 3443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, PM 1999. Maternal behaviour and rearing ability of Merino ewes can be improved by strategic feed supplementation during late pregnancy and selection for calm temperament. PhD thesis, The University of Western Australia.Google Scholar
Murphy, PM, Purvis, IW, Lindsay, DR, Le Neindre, P, Orgeur, P, Poindron, P 1994. Measures of temperament are highly repeatable in Merino sheep and some are related to maternal behaviour. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 20, 247250.Google Scholar
Nowak, R, Poindron, P, Sèbe, F, Hart, KW, Chadwick, A, Blache, D 2006. Divergent selection on temperament affects vocal and locomotor activity in isolated newborn lambs. In Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production. Short Communication, Perth, WA, 45pp.Google Scholar
Poindron, P, Levy, F 1990. Physiological, sensory and experiential determinants of maternal behaviour in sheep. In Mammalian parenting: biochemical, neurobiological and behavioral determinants (ed. NA Krasnegor and RS Bridges), pp. 133156. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Poindron, P, Levy, F, Krehbiel, D 1988. Genital, olfactory, and endocrine interactions in the development of maternal behaviour in the parturient ewe. Psychoneuroendocrinology 13, 99125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pryce, CR, Abbott, AH, Hodges, JK, Martin, RD 1988. Maternal behaviour is related to prepartum urinary estradiol levels in the red-bellied tamarin monkeys. Physiology and Behavior 44, 717726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, SL, Panesar, S, Steiner, M, Fleming, AS 2004. The effects of adrenalectomy and corticosterone replacement on maternal behavior in the postpartum rat. Hormones and Behavior 46, 411419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roussel, S, Hemsworth, PH, Leruste, H, White, C, Duvaux-Ponter, C, Nowak, R, Boissy, A 2006. Repeated transport and isolation during pregnancy in ewes: effects on the reactivity to humans and to their offspring after lambing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 97, 172189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sèbe, F, Duboscq, J, Aubin, T, Ligout, SPoindron, P 2010. Early vocal recognition of mother by lambs: contribution of low- and high-frequency vocalization. Animal Behaviour 79, 10551066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sèbe, F, Nowak, R, Poindron, P, Aubin, T 2007. Establishment of vocal communication and discrimination between ewes and their lamb in the first two days after parturition. Developmental Psychobiology 49, 375386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shillito, EE 1972. Vocalisation in sheep. Journal of Physiology 226 (suppl.), 4546.Google ScholarPubMed
Tu, MT, Lupien, SJ, Walker, CD 2005. Measuring stress responses in postpartum mothers; perspectives from studies in human and animal populations. Stress 8, 1934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M 2005. The potential influence of maternal stress on development and mental health of the offspring. Brain Behavior and Immunity 19, 296308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed