Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:59:00.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stereotypic swaying and serum Cortisol concentrations in three captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

ML Wilson*
Affiliation:
Lincoln Park Zoo, 2001 N Clark Street, Chicago, IL 60614, USA Center for Conservation and Behavior, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia GA 30332, USA
MA Bloomsmith
Affiliation:
Center for Conservation and Behavior, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia GA 30332, USA
TL Maple
Affiliation:
Center for Conservation and Behavior, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia GA 30332, USA Zoo Atlanta, 800 Cherokee Avenue SE, Atlanta, Georgia GA 30315-1440, USA
*
* Correspondence: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The behaviour and serum Cortisol concentrations of three captive female African elephants (Loxodonta africana) were studied to determine whether their stereotypic swaying was more prevalent before regularly scheduled events in the elephants’ routine, and whether the elephants that exhibited more stereotyped swaying had lower mean serum cortisol concentrations. Behavioural data were collected during hour-long observations balanced across three periods, and during 15-min observations prior to the elephants being moved to different portions of their enclosure. Observational data were collected using instantaneous focal sampling of behaviours every 30s. Serum cortisol measures were obtained through weekly blood withdrawal from the elephants’ ears. Of the three elephants, two exhibited stereotyped swaying, which accounted for a mean of 0.4% of the scans during the hour-long observations and a mean of 18% of the scans prior to the elephants being moved between different parts of the enclosure. Swaying was highly variable among the individual elephants during both categories of observations. Additionally, both elephants swayed more prior to moving in the afternoon than prior to moving in the morning. Analyses of serum cortisol concentrations indicated that each elephant had a different mean cortisol level, which did not clearly correspond with the expression of swaying. The findings indicate that a rigidly scheduled management event may elicit stereotyped swaying in the studied elephants. Future research should document the behavioural and physiological effects of an altered management routine to improve captive elephant welfare.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Altmann, J 1974 Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 48: 227267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, JL and Hemsworth, PH 1990 The validity of physiological and behavioural measures of animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 25: 177187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkson, G 1967 Abnormal stereotyped motor acts. In: Zubin J and Hunt HF (eds) Comparative Psychopathology: Animal and Human pp 76-94. Green & Stratton: New York, UsaGoogle Scholar
Bettinger, T and Laudenslager, M 1998 Cortisol in mammals: what can it tell us? In: Proceedings of the Conservation Research Consortium. 4-6 June 1998. Riverbanks Zoo and Garden: Columbia, SC, USAGoogle Scholar
Bettinger, T, Larry, M, Goldstein, M and Laudenslager, M 1997 Plasma Cortisol concentrations and behavioral traits of two female Asian elephants. In: American Zoological and Aquarium Association Annual Conference Proceedings pp 88-90. 13-17 September 1997. Albuquerque Biological Parle Albuquerque, NM, USAGoogle Scholar
Bildsoe, M, Heller, KE and Jeppesen, LJ 1991 Effects of immobility stress and food restriction on stereotypies in low and high stereotyping female ranch mink. Behavioural Processes 25: 179189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloomsmith, MA and Lambeth, SP 1995 Effects of predictable versus unpredictable feeding schedules on chimpanzee behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44: 6574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Lambeth, SP and Stoinski, TS 2001 The behavioral effects of meal predictability on chimpanzee behavior. American Journal of Primatology 54 (Suppl I): 96Google Scholar
Brockett, RC, Stoinski, TS, Black, J, Markowitz, T and Maple, TL 1999 Nocturnal behavior in a group of unchained female African elephants. Zoo Biology 18: 1011093.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, JL, Wemmer, CM and Lehnhardt, J 1995 Urinary cortisol analysis for monitoring adrenal activity in elephants. Zoo Biology 14: 533542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K 1986 Predictability of feeding: its effects on agonistic behavior and growth in grower pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 16: 2538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K 1996 Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild mammals. In: Kleiman, DG, Allen, ME, Thompson, KV and Lumpkin, S (eds) Wild Mammals in Captivity pp 317333. University of Chicago Press: Chicago, USAGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K and Seidensticker, J 1991 Seasonal variation in stereotypic pacing in an American black bear Ursus americanus. Behavioural Processes 25: 155161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Seidensticker, JC and Baldwin, R 1991 Environmental enrichment for zoo bears. Zoo Biology 14: 316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, AS, Mason, WA and Moberg, GP 1988 Differential behavioral and adrenocortical responses to stress among three macaque species. American Journal of Primatology 14: 3752CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, JJ, Odberg, F and Nicol, C 1996 Limitations on the effectiveness of environmental improvement in reducing stereotypic behaviour in bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 48: 237248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockett, CM, Bowers, CL, Sackett, GP and Bowden, DM 1993 Urinary cortisol responses of longtailed macaques to five cage sizes, tethering, sedation, and room change. American Journal of Primatology 30: SS74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dathe, HH, Kuckelkorn, B and Minneman, D 1992 Salivary cortisol assessment for stress detection in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus): a pilot study. Zoo Biology 11: 285289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friend, T H 1999 Behavior of picketed circus elephants. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 62: 7388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friend, TH and Parker, ML 1999 The effect of penning versus picketing on stereotypic behavior of circus elephants. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 64: 213255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, TM, Friend, TH, Gardner, JM, Packard, JM, Beaver, B and Bushong, D 2000 Variation in stereotypic behavior related to restraint in circus elephants. Zoo Biology 19: 2092213.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krishnamurthy, R 1994 Vocalizations of captive Francois’ languars linked to stereotypy and possible stress. Folia Primatology 63: 9195CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laudenslager, ML, Boccia, ML, Berger, CL, Ruggles-Gennaro, MM, McFerran, B and Reite, ML 1995 Total cortisol, free cortisol, and growth hormone associated with brief social separation experiences in young macaques. Developmental Psychobiology 28: 199211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P and Bateson, P 1986 Measuring Behavior: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ 1991a Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour 41: 10151037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ 1991b Stereotypies and suffering. Behavioural Processes 25: 103115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moberg, GP 1985 Influence of stress on reproduction: measures of well-being. In: Moberg, GP (ed) Animal Stress pp 2750. American Physiological Society: Bethesda, USACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novak, MA and Suomi, SJ 1988 Psychological well-being of primates in captivity. American Psychologist 43: 765773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redbo 1 1993 Stereotypies and cortisol secretion in heifers subjected to tethering. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38: 213225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, JP 1985 Stereotypies, aggression and the feeding schedules of tethered sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14: 137147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, JP 1993 The ‘coping’ hypothesis of stereotypic behaviour. Animal Behaviour 45: 613615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, J 1995 Keeping circus elephants temporarily in paddocks. Animal Welfare 4: 87101Google Scholar
Schmid, J, Heistermann, M, Gansloßer, U and Hodges, JK 2001 Introduction of foreign female Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) into an existing group: behavioural reactions and changes in cortisol levels. Animal Welfare 10: 357372Google Scholar
Terlouw, EMC, Lawrence, AB, Ladewig, J, DePassille, AM, Rushen, J and Schouten, WGP 1991 Relationship between plasma cortisol and stereotypic activities in pigs. Behavioural Processes 25: 133153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waitt, C and Buchanan-Smith, HM 2001 What time is feeding? How delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 75: 7585CrossRefGoogle Scholar