Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T17:24:12.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shedding ultraviolet light on welfare in laboratory rodents: suggestions for further research and refinement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

DB SØrensen*
Affiliation:
University of Copenhagen, Department of Veterinary Disease Biology, Thorvaldsensvej 57, 1871 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The welfare of laboratory rats and mice is sought to be optimised through adjustment of a variety of environmental factors, including light intensity and photoperiodicity. However, the fact that rodents are able to perceive ultraviolet (UV) light tends to be ignored. The importance of being able — as a rodent — to utilise this part of the visual spectrum has not been studied in great detail, but suggestions, based on the evolutionary success of this trait, indicate that the deprivation of ultraviolet light in mice and rats could perhaps impact negatively on the welfare of these animals. Further research into the importance of having a UV light source available to rats and mice should be encouraged.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2014 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Arakawa, H, Blanchard, DC, Arakawa, K, Dunlap, C and Blanchard, RJ 2008 Scent marking behavior as an odorant com-munication in mice. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review 32: 12361248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chavez, AE, Bozinovic, F, Peichl, L and Palacios, AG 2003 Retinal spectral sensitivity, fur coloration, and urine reflectance in the genus Octodon (Rodentia): implications for visual ecology. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 44: 22902296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.02-0670Google ScholarPubMed
Desjardins, C, Maruniak, JA and Bronson, FH 1973 Social rank in house mice: differentiation revealed by ultraviolet visuali-sation of urinary marking patterns. Science 182: 939941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.182.4115.939CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosling, LM, Atkinson, NW, Dunn, S and Collins, SA 1996 The response of subordinate male mice to scent marks varies in relation to their own competitive ability. Animal Behaviour 52:11851191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honkavaara, J, Aberg, H and Viitala, J 2008 Do house mice use UV cues when foraging? Journal of Ethology 26: 339345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10164-007-0068-6Google Scholar
Humphries, RE, Robertson, DHL, Beynon, RJ and Hurst, JL 1999 Unravelling the chemical basis of competitive scent marking in house mice. Animal Behaviour 58: 11771190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1999.1252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurst, JL, Fang, J and Barnard, CJ 1993 The role of substrate odours in maintaining social tolerance between male house mice, Mus musculus domesticus. Animal Behaviour 45: 9971006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, GH 1992 Ultraviolet vision in vertebrates. American Zoologist 32: 544554Google Scholar
Jacobs, GH, Fenwick, JA and Williams, GA 2001 Cone-based vision of rats for ultraviolet and visible lights. Journal of Experimental Biology 204: 24392446Google ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, GH, Neitz, J and Deegan, JF 1991 Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to Ultraviolet-Light. Nature 353: 655656. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/353655a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, GH and Williams, GA 2007 Contributions of the mouse UV photopigment to the ERG and to vision. Documenta Ophthalmologica 115: 137144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10633-007-9055-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacey, JC, Beynon, RJ and Hurst, JL 2007 The importance of exposure to other male scents in determining competitive behav-iour among inbred male mice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 104: 130142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.026Google Scholar
Melin, AD, Moritz, GL, Fosbury, RAE, Kawamura, S and Dominy, NJ 2012 Why aye-ayes see blue. American Journal of Primatology 74: 185192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.21996Google ScholarPubMed
Nevison, CM, Barnard, CJ, Beynon, RJ and Hurst, JL 2000 The consequences of inbreeding for recognizing competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267: 687694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortin-Martinez, A, Jimenez-Lopez, M, Nadal-Nicolas, FM, Salinas-Navarro, M, Alarcon-Martinez, L, Sauve, Y, Villegas-Perez, MP, Vidal-Sanz, M and Agudo-Barriuso, M 2010 Automated quantification and topographical distribution of the whole population of S- and L-cones in adult albino and pig-mented rats. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 51: 31713183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.09-4861Google ScholarPubMed
Solomon, SG and Lennie, P 2007 The machinery of colour vision. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 276286. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tovee, MJ 1995 Ultra-Violet photoreceptors in the animal king-dom: their distribution and function. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 10: 455460. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(00)89179-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Oosterhout, F, Fisher, SP, van Diepen, HC, Watson, TS, Houben, T, VanderLeest, HT, Thompson, S, Peirson, SN, Foster, RG and Meijer, JH 2012 Ultraviolet light provides a major input to non-image-forming light detection in mice. Current Biology 22: 13971402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.032Google Scholar