Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T21:35:18.296Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Quality of life: the heart of the matter

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

JK Kirkwood*
Affiliation:
Universities Federation for Animal Welfare and Humane Slaughter Association, The Old School, Brewhouse Hill, Wheathampstead, Hertfordshire AL4 8AN, UK
*
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

It is believed widely, and with good reason, that some other members of the animal kingdom, like us, have feelings (associated with brain states induced by various sensory inputs and cognitive processes) which can be pleasant or unpleasant. Associated with the strengthening scientific foundations for this belief, there has been growing consensus around the world that we have a moral responsibility, in all of our dealings and interactions with sentient animals, to take account of their feelings. This has led to widespread re-evaluation, in recent years, of the nature of our interactions with other animals. However, assessment of the feelings of animals — the quality of their lives — remains a great challenge for veterinarians and others involved with their management. The fundamental difficulty is that whilst judgements about management or treatment often have to be made on the basis of our inferences of how they feel (ie of the feelings they consciously experience), a subjective step cannot be avoided in making these inferences. We cannot know how other animals feel but can only infer this based on our knowledge of the animal and on our own experiences of feelings. This inevitable ‘gap’ in objective deductions about feelings is often wide enough that people can reach radically different conclusions when judging an animal's quality of life. Opinions thus often differ regarding the point at which it becomes kinder to euthanase an animal than not to do so, the point at which it becomes kinder not to undertake a potentially painful therapeutic intervention than to do so, and where the balance lies when animal welfare costs are being ‘weighed’ against some benefit of their use for humans (eg as laboratory, farm or companion animals). The aim of this meeting is to discuss if and how science has helped in developing reasoned approaches to these dilemmas, and to consider the need for further research, education, and policy development.

Type
Introduction
Copyright
© 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Appleby, MC and Hughes, BO (eds) 1997 Animal Welfare. CAB International: Wallingford, UKGoogle Scholar
CAWC 2003 The welfare of non-domesticated animals kept for companionship. Companion Animal Welfare Council, UK. www.cawc.org.ukGoogle Scholar
CAWC 2006 Breeding and welfare in companion animals. Companion Animal Welfare Council, UK. www.cawc.org.ukGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P 1996 The Engine of Reason, The Seat of the Soul. MIT Press: Cambridge, Massachusetts, USAGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A 1999 The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. William Heinemann: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R 1976 The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Edelman, GM and Tonini, G 2000 Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination. Penguin Press: London, UKGoogle Scholar
FAO 2006 FAOSTAT Database Results from http://faostat.fao.org/faostat/collections?version=ext&hasbulk=0&subset=agriculture. Updated 24 April 2006. Accessed 15 July 2006Google Scholar
Independent Working Group on Snares 2005 Report of the Independent Working Group on Snares. Defra: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Kirkwood, JK 2004 The importance of animal welfare. In: Perry GC (ed) The Welfare of the Laying Hen pp 1-7. Proceedings of the World's Poultry Science Association Symposium on the Welfare of the Laying Hen, July 2003, Bristol, UK. CAB International: Wallingford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkwood, JK 2006 The distribution of sentience in the animal kingdom. In: Turner, J and D'Silva, J (eds) Animals, Ethics and Trade: The Challenge of Animal Sentience pp 1226. Proceedings of the CIWF Trust Conference, March 2005, London, UK. Earthscan: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Kirkwood, JK and Hubrecht, R 2001 Consciousness, cognition and animal welfare. Animal Welfare 10 (Suppl): S5-S17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C 2004 The Quest for Consciousness: A Neurobiological Approach. Roberts & Company: Englewood, Colorado, USAGoogle Scholar
Nordenfelt, L 2006 Animal and Human Health and Welfare: A Comparative Philosophical Analysis. CAB International: Wallingford, UKGoogle Scholar
Rollin, BE 1989 The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain and Science. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Rolls, ET 1999 The Brain and Emotion. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Ryder, RD 2000 Animal Revolution: Changing Attitudes Towards Speciesism. Berg: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Sainsbury, AW, Bennett, PM and Kirkwood, JK 1995 Welfare of free-living wild animals in Europe: harm caused by human activities. Animal Welfare 4: 183206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
US Census Bureau 2006 US and world population clocks. http://www.census.gov/main/www/popclock.html. Accessed 15 July 2006Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L 1997 Consciousness Lost and Found. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Zoos Forum 2001 The ethical review process. In: The Zoos Forum Handbook. www.defra.gov.uk/wildife-countryside/gwd/zoosforum/handbook/index.htm.Google Scholar
Zoos Forum 2006 Animal welfare and welfare assessment in zoos. In: The Zoos Forum Handbook. www.defra.gov.uk/wildife-countryside/gwd/zoosforum/handbook/index.htm.Google Scholar