Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T20:21:11.076Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diving and Underwater Swimming as Enrichment Activities for Captive Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

J R Anderson*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychophysiologie (CNRS URA 1295), Université Louis Pasteur, 7 rue de l'Université, 67000 Strasbourg, France
A Rortais
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychophysiologie (CNRS URA 1295), Université Louis Pasteur, 7 rue de l'Université, 67000 Strasbourg, France
S Guillemein
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychophysiologie (CNRS URA 1295), Université Louis Pasteur, 7 rue de l'Université, 67000 Strasbourg, France
*
Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

In order to assess the environmental enrichment value of a small swimming pool for captive juvenile rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), observations of social and individual behaviours were made during baseline and experimental (pool) conditions. When the pool was available there was less social grooming and cage manipulation, and more play. Most of the monkeys engaged in diving and underwater swimming. The presence of pieces of banana at the bottom of the pool reduced these water-related activities, whereas when raisins were spread along the bottom or when there was no food in the water, there was more diving and less aggression. Certain effects tended to vary with dominance status, but individual differences appeared more important than social status in determining reactions to the water. The provision of a small swimming pool for captive macaques is an effective contribution to improving their welfare.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 1994 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Anderson, J R, Peignot, P and Adelbrecht, C 1992 Task-directed and recreational underwater swimming in captive rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 31(4): 14Google Scholar
Anderson, J R and Visalberghi, E 1990 Towards better conditions for captive nonhuman primates: routines, requirements, and research. In Alleva, E and Laviola, G (eds) Biomedical Experimentation and Laboratory Animals: Hot Behavioural Issues pp 111. ISTISAN Reports: RomeGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J D 1986 Behavior in infancy: exploration and play. In Mitchell, G and Erwin, J (eds) Comparative Primate Biology Vol 2A: Behavior, Conservation, and Ecology pp 295326. Alan R Liss: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Berman, C 1977 Seaside play is a serious business. New Scientist 73: 761763Google Scholar
Bloomstrand, M, Riddle, K, Alford, P and Maple, T L 1986 Objective evaluation of a behavioral enrichment device for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology 5: 293300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, G D and Suomi, S J 1991 Effects of woodchips and buried food on behavior patterns and psychological well-being of captive rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 23: 141151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamove, A S and Anderson, J R 1979 Woodchip litter in macaque groups. Journal of the Institute of Animal Technicians 30: 6974Google Scholar
Fittinghoff, N A and Lindburg, D G 1980 Riverine foraging in east Bornean Macaca fascicularis. In Lindburg D G (ed) The Macaques pp 182214. Van Nostrand Reinhold: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Gautier-Hion, A 1973 Social and ecological features of talapoin monkey - comparisons with sympatric cercopithecines. In Michael, R P and Crook, J H (eds) Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates pp 147170. Academic Press: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, S G and Wrenshall, E 1989 Environmental enrichment for monkeys used in behavioral toxicology studies. In Segal, E F (ed) Housing, Care and Psychological Wellbeing of Captive and Laboratory Primates pp 244254. Noyes Publications: Park Ridge, NJGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W J III 1982 Baboon sleeping site preferences and relationships to primate grouping patterns. American Journal of Primatology 3: 4153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawai, M 1965 Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6: 13Google Scholar
Lindburg, D G 1971 The rhesus monkey in north India: an ecological and behavioral study. In Rosenblum, L A (ed) Primate Behavior Vol 1 pp 1106. Academic Press: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Martin, P and Bateson, P 1986 Measuring Behaviour. Cambridge University Press: CambridgeGoogle Scholar
Mears, C E and Harlow, H F 1975 Play: early and eternal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 72: 18781882CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parks, K A and Novak, M A 1993 Observations of increased activity and tool use in captive rhesus monkeys exposed to troughs of water. American Journal of Primatology 29: 1325Google ScholarPubMed
Poole, T B 1988 Normal and abnormal behaviour in captive primates. Primate Report 22: 312Google Scholar
Roonwal, M L and Mohnot, S M 1977 Primates of South Asia. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E O (ed) 1978 Social Play in Primates. Academic Press: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Suomi, S and Novak, M A 1991 The role of individual differences in promoting psychological well-being in rhesus monkeys. In Novak, M A and Petto, A J (eds) Through the Looking Glass: Issues of Psychological Weil-Being in Captive Nonhuman Primates pp 5056. American Psychological Association: Washington, D CCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood-Gush, DGM and Vestergaard, K 1991 The seeking of novelty and its relation to play. Animal Behaviour 42: 599606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeager, C P 1991 Possible antipredator behavior associated with river crossings by proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus). American Journal of Primatology 24: 6166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zumpe, D and Michael, R P 1986 Dominance index: a simple measure of relative dominance status in primates. American Journal of Primatology 10: 291300CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed