Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T10:44:52.916Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Canine separation-related behaviour at six months of age: Dog, owner and early-life risk factors identified using the ‘Generation Pup’ longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2024

Fiona C. Dale
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Science and Ethics, Royal Veterinary College, North Mymms AL9 7TA, UK
Charlotte C. Burn*
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Science and Ethics, Royal Veterinary College, North Mymms AL9 7TA, UK
Jane Murray
Affiliation:
Dogs Trust, London EC1V 7RQ, UK
Rachel Casey
Affiliation:
Dogs Trust, London EC1V 7RQ, UK
*
Corresponding author: Charlotte Burn; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Separation-related behaviours (SRBs) in dogs (Canis familiaris) often indicate poor welfare. Understanding SRB risk factors can aid prevention strategies. We investigated whether early-life experiences and dog-owner interactions affect SRB development. Using a longitudinal study, we conducted exploratory analyses of associations between potential risk factors and SRB occurrence in six month old puppies (n = 145). Dogs were less likely to develop SRBs if owners reported that, at ≤ 16 weeks old, puppies were restricted to crates/rooms overnight and had ≥ 9 h of sleep per night. Puppies with poor house-training at ≤ 16 weeks were more likely to show SRBs, as were those trained using dog treats or novel kibble versus other rewards. Puppies whose owners used more punishment/aversive techniques when responding to ‘bad’ behaviour had increased odds of SRBs at six months versus other puppies. Puppies whose owners reported ‘fussing’ over their dogs at six months in response to ‘bad’ behaviour upon their return, versus those whose owners responded in other ways, were six times more likely to display SRBs. Other factors, including dog breed, sex and source, showed no significant association with SRB occurrence. Thus, SRB development might be prevented by enabling sleep for ≥ 9 h in early life, providing enclosed space overnight, refraining from aversive training of puppies generally, and avoiding fussing over puppies in response to unwanted behaviour following separation. These recommendations derive from correlational longitudinal study results, so analysis of interventional data is required for confirmation regarding effective prevention strategies.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© Royal Veterinary College, 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Canine separation-related behaviours (SRBs) can be associated with poor canine welfare, but the factors leading to their development are little understood, making prevention potentially difficult. These behaviours occur when owners are absent, typically starting within a few minutes of owner departure (Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010). The most commonly reported SRBs are destructive behaviours, such as chewing and scratching, often near the site of owner departure, and vocalisations such as barking, howling and whining (Lund & Jørgensen Reference Lund and Jørgensen1999). Dogs (Canis familiaris) may also urinate, defaecate, or display repetitive behaviours such as spinning or pacing (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006; Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010). Behaviours relating to dogs being left alone are commonly reported as being problematic for dog owners and are often included as leading reasons for behavioural referrals (Bamberger & Houpt Reference Bamberger and Houpt2006; Storengen et al. Reference Storengen, Boge, Strøm, Løberg and Lingaas2014; Siracusa et al. Reference Siracusa, Provoost and Reisner2017). There is a range of terminology used to describe separation behaviours in the literature (Ogata Reference Ogata2016; de Assis et al. Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020), including SRBs, separation anxiety, separation-related disorders, separation-related distress and separation-related problems (e.g. Takeuchi et al. Reference Takeuchi, Ogata, Houpt and Scarlett2001; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006; Konok et al. Reference Konok, Dóka and Miklósi2011; de Assis et al. Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020). Here, we refer to problematic canine behaviours when owners are absent as SRBs, so as not to make assumptions regarding the severity of such behaviours or the underlying emotions, which could include frustration, fear, anxiety or boredom (McCrave Reference McCrave1991; Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010a; Burn Reference Burn2017; de Assis et al. Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020) or categories described in de Assis et al. (Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020) such as panic and desire.

The prevalence of SRBs has been reported from 6 to 55% (Bradshaw et al. Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002; Soares et al. Reference Soares, Pereira and Paixão2010; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016; Dinwoodie et al. Reference Dinwoodie, Dwyer, Zottola, Gleason and Dodman2019; Salonen et al. Reference Salonen, Sulkama, Mikkola, Puurunen, Hakanen, Tiira, Araujo and Lohi2020; Harvey et al. Reference Harvey, Christley, Giragosian, Mead, Murray, Samet, Upjohn and Casey2022; Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024). The wide range in prevalence estimates is likely to be partly due to the difficulty in observing behaviour that, by definition, occurs when no humans are present. Prevalence estimations from owner-report (e.g. Tiira et al. Reference Tiira, Sulkama and Lohi2016; Dinwoodie et al. Reference Dinwoodie, Dwyer, Zottola, Gleason and Dodman2019; Salonen et al. Reference Salonen, Sulkama, Mikkola, Puurunen, Hakanen, Tiira, Araujo and Lohi2020) are often based on what owners could hear upon their approach to the household immediately prior to their return, neighbour reports, or visible damage to the household or other items. Therefore, SRBs displayed during owner absence, without witness or evidence, may be missed from owner-report which may mean prevalence measured in this way is underestimated. Furthermore, subtle SRBs observable via video, such as panting, lip-licking (Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010), salivation, repetitive behaviour (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006), or depression-like behaviours (Podberscek et al. Reference Podberscek, Hsu and Serpell1999; Landsberg et al. Reference Landsberg, Melese, Sherman, Neilson, Zimmerman and Clarke2008; Soares et al. Reference Soares, Pereira and Paixão2010), may go unnoticed by owners or may not be attributed to problems with being left alone. The prevalence of SRBs may therefore be higher than reported, meaning more dogs than estimated may be experiencing SRBs, potentially on a daily basis, when their owners are absent.

Risk factors for SRB development have been researched to some extent, but almost exclusively in adult dogs rather than in puppies. Risk factors can include characteristics of the dogs themselves, their owners, or their environment and early experiences.

Dog characteristics as risk factors

Several dog characteristics have been found to be associated with SRBs, but without entirely consistent findings. For example, previous research has suggested that mixed breed dogs are more likely to display SRBs than pure breed dogs (McCrave Reference McCrave1991; Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001) or that specific breeds, such as Cocker Spaniels (Storengen et al. Reference Storengen, Boge, Strøm, Løberg and Lingaas2014) or Golden Retrievers (van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018), may be more likely to express specific SRBs than other breeds. However, such variations may be due to methodological differences such as case definitions (owner-reported separation anxiety vs diagnosis of separation anxiety at a veterinary clinic), or differing breed compositions of the sample populations (Sherman & Mills Reference Sherman and Mills2008), leading to sampling bias which may have influenced findings.

Similarly, previous work has suggested that male dogs are significantly more likely to exhibit SRBs than female dogs (Bradshaw et al. Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002; McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008), but this finding is by no means universal, with some finding no association between the sex of the dog and SRBs (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001; Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Brooks, Basse, Burman, Paul, Blackwell and Casey2010b; Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010). Neutered dogs have been found to be more likely to show SRBs than intact dogs (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001; McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008). Neutering might affect males and females differently, however, as a questionnaire-based study reported that neutered male dogs were more likely to show SRBs than intact male dogs, while neutered female dogs had lower SRB scores than intact female dogs (van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018). While the effect of neutering on SRB occurrence requires further clarification, the direction of causation should be considered; owners of dogs with SRBs may be more likely to have their dogs neutered (van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018), as some veterinarians may suggest neutering male dogs to control problematic or undesirable behaviours (Salman et al. Reference Salman, Hutchison, Ruch-Gallie, Kogan, New, Kass and Scarlett2000).

Owner characteristics and behaviour as risk factors

Owner experience may be related to the occurrence of SRBs. Dogs belonging to first-time owners were, perhaps surprisingly, less likely to show separation-related defaecation than those belonging to more experienced owners (Jagoe & Serpell Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996). In terms of owner attitudes, owners who were self-reportedly ‘attachment avoidant’ (owners who were classed as dismissive or less sensitive to their dogs’ needs according to a human attachment questionnaire; Collins Reference Collins1996; Konok et al. Reference Konok, Kosztolányi, Rainer, Mutschler, Halsband and Miklósi2015) had dogs with increased SRBs, possibly because these owners may be less responsive or less consistent in their interactions with their dogs (Konok et al. Reference Konok, Kosztolányi, Rainer, Mutschler, Halsband and Miklósi2015).

Owner approach to training and discipline may additionally be associated with likelihood of SRBs. Less separation-related toileting has been reported with increasing formality of owner-reported obedience training (absent, informal, or formal) (Jagoe & Serpell Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996). However, results are mixed, with González-Martínez et al. (Reference González-Martínez, Martínez, Rosado, Luño, Santamarina, Suárez, Camino, de la Cruz and Diéguez2019) finding no association between puppy class attendance and SRBs (González-Martínez et al. Reference González-Martínez, Martínez, Rosado, Luño, Santamarina, Suárez, Camino, de la Cruz and Diéguez2019). Training techniques are generally categorised as either positive or negative, and as either reinforcement- or aversive/punishment-based (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Bolster, Richards, Loftus and Casey2012). Many owners tend to use a mixture of two or more of these methods at a time leading to some referring to training techniques as being reward-based (positive reinforcement and negative punishment) or aversive-based (positive punishment or negative reinforcement) (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Bolster, Richards, Loftus and Casey2012). Aversive training methods have been associated with increased fear and anxiety (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008; Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Bubna-Littitz, Bartels, Futschik and Troxler2010), and more owner-reported behavioural problems (Mills Reference Mills1997; Hiby et al. Reference Hiby, Rooney and Bradshaw2004; Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Bubna-Littitz, Bartels, Futschik and Troxler2010; Casey et al. Reference Casey, Loftus, Bolster, Richards and Blackwell2014; Bräm Dubé et al. Reference Bräm Dubé, Asher, Würbel, Riemer and Melotti2020). In terms of SRBs, one C-BARQ (Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire) study found that dogs had worse SRB scores if their owners engaged in more aversive training (Hsu & Serpell Reference Hsu and Serpell2003). However, a smaller questionnaire-based study found no association between SRB occurrence and training methods (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008), indicating a need for clarification, especially if owner training approach can help to prevent this welfare problem. Much of the advice for treating SRBs includes not punishing undesired behaviour upon owner return (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006), but its inclusion in advice packages has been questioned, as the advice was not found to contribute to the reduction of pre-existing SRBs in an experimental study of eight dogs (Butler et al. Reference Butler, Sargisson and Elliffe2011). Whilst there are differences between studies in the efficacy of stopping punishment for treating SRBs, it is widely considered to be important to avoid exacerbating anxiety through fear of owner return as well as departure, and is also part of wider evidence that punishment-based techniques negatively impact upon dog welfare and are ethically unacceptable (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008; Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Bubna-Littitz, Bartels, Futschik and Troxler2010).

In addition to advice on punishment, owners are often advised to avoid excessive greeting behaviours upon owner-dog reunion (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006, Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016). There is support for this advice as McGreevy and Masters (Reference McGreevy and Masters2008) found that dogs belonging to owners who played with them within the first 30 min of returning had higher odds of displaying SRBs, suggesting owner interactions upon owner return influence SRBs (McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008). However, the direction of causation is unclear; it is possible that owners of dogs with SRBs perceive their dogs’ SRBs and respond by greeting them effusively or playing with them to comfort them.

Previous research has pointed to the general influence on dog welfare of factors such as sleeping routines, crates and training (Jagoe & Serpell Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996; Podberscek et al. Reference Podberscek, Hsu and Serpell1999; Sherman & Mills Reference Sherman and Mills2008). Research has suggested that allowing dogs to sleep in a bedroom or on the owner’s bed could potentially exacerbate behavioural problems (Podberscek et al. Reference Podberscek, Hsu and Serpell1999). Similarly, Jagoe and Serpell’s (Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996) questionnaire-based study found that separation-related toileting was more likely to occur when dogs slept in the same room as their owner. Also, a descriptive study found that just over half of a population of dogs diagnosed with separation anxiety at a behaviour clinic slept with their owners, although no comparison with dogs without SRBs was made (Storengen et al. Reference Storengen, Boge, Strøm, Løberg and Lingaas2014). In contrast, one previous study reported no association between SRBs and allowing dogs to sleep on the owner’s bed (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001). Learning more about whether, and in what context, puppies come across such experiences during the first few months in new homes, may illuminate more about their influence of later SRB occurrence. Similarly, while owner training approaches and attitudes may influence behaviour, information regarding owner approaches to their dogs at times other than training may shed more light upon how owner interactions influence dog behaviour long-term.

Finally, the home environment of dogs may affect SRB outcome, with dogs belonging to single adults being more likely to show ‘separation anxiety’ (defined in the paper as when dogs ‘become upset when separated from their owner’) than those in multi-person households (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001). Recent research from Brand and colleagues (Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024), on a cohort population of dogs purchased during the pandemic, found that dogs who had had another dog in the house when they were a puppy were less likely to have SRBs at 21 months of age than those without another dog in the house as a puppy. This suggests that growing up with another dog in the house may help prevent puppies from developing SRBs (Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024); however, this was a novel finding, and the protective effect may vary considerably depending on the relationship between the dogs in question. Similarly, changes in the household have been associated with separation anxiety (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001), with increases in household members and owner job changes resulting in a greater likelihood of higher SRB scores (McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008).

Early life experiences and environment as risk factors

Early life experiences can have long-term effects on behaviour and temperament (Appleby et al. Reference Appleby, Bradshaw and Casey2002). Early experiences are thought to affect the ability of dogs to cope with stress (Foyer et al. Reference Foyer, Wilsson, Wright and Jensen2013), and that early traumatic experiences of separation or unpleasant experiences can affect later behaviour (Appleby & Pluijmakers Reference Appleby and Pluijmakers2003). Dogs have a sensitive period of behavioural development and emotional responses. If puppies do not receive appropriate or adequate socialisation during this time, it can result in future behavioural problems. This time-period, known as the ‘socialisation period’, occurs when puppies are 3–12 weeks of age (Scott & Fuller Reference Scott and Fuller1965). Tiira and Lohi (Reference Tiira and Lohi2015) found that dogs reported by their owners to be ‘fearful’ had had poorer maternal care and fewer socialisation experiences when they were puppies (Tiira & Lohi Reference Tiira and Lohi2015), but they did not assess SRBs specifically.

Perhaps relatedly, the provenance of puppies can influence their behaviour long-term. For example, evidence from questionnaire responses have indicated that dogs from pet shops not only had overall worse behaviour and were more aggressive and fearful, but also showed more SRBs than dogs from non-commercial breeders (McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Serpell, Duffy, Masaoud and Dohoo2013; Pirrone et al. Reference Pirrone, Pierantoni, Pastorino and Albertini2016). Similarly, high levels of SRBs were reported in dogs acquired from shelters or rescue centres (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008; van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018) and pet shops (McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Serpell, Duffy, Masaoud and Dohoo2013; van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018), while dogs acquired from friends and family may have lower risks of developing SRBs (McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008).

There appears to relatively little research into the potential effect of early life experiences on later SRBs specifically. As mentioned previously, early research identified that puppies become distressed when separated from their mothers or littermates, or when placed in a strange environment, and that puppies were more distressed if they had not experienced being separated before 9–12 weeks of age (Elliot & Scott Reference Elliot and Scott1961). This implies that early experiences of being briefly left alone may be helpful to prevent SRBs later on, and puppies may develop issues with being left alone if they have not been habituated to short periods of isolation from a young age (Appleby et al. Reference Appleby, Bradshaw and Casey2002). The influence of breeder environment was investigated in a study which found that dogs obtained from breeders categorised as ‘less responsible’ by the authors due to their rearing practices, had higher levels of behavioural problems, including SRBs (Gray et al. Reference Gray, Butler, Douglas and Serpell2016), although the exact definition of what constituted ‘responsible’ was not clear.

Programmes to increase socialisation at an early age seem to be effective at reducing SRBs. For example, Vaterlaws-Whiteside and Hartmann (Reference Vaterlaws-Whiteside and Hartmann2017) implemented a socialisation programme with litters of puppies at an assistance dog breeding centre. Results indicated that with increased dog-handler, one-on-one time over the first six weeks of life puppies showed better SRB scores (i.e. less evidence of SRB) derived from questionnaires administered to dog handlers after eight months (Vaterlaws-Whiteside & Hartmann Reference Vaterlaws-Whiteside and Hartmann2017). However, in another questionnaire study, whilst general fearfulness was increased in dogs who had sub-optimal early experiences (e.g. poor maternal care and socialisation), lower daily exercise was the only factor that significantly increased SRBs specifically (Tiira & Lohi Reference Tiira and Lohi2015).

Aims of the study

We aimed to explore early life risk factors for the development of SRBs, with a view towards making recommendations for future research and prevention strategies. Using a longitudinal questionnaire study of pet dogs living in the UK and Republic of Ireland (ROI), we carried out an exploratory risk factor analysis to investigate and quantify associations between dog and owner factors and specific environmental and dog experiences in early life (up to six months of age) and their effect on owner-reported SRB occurrence at six months of age.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Data used in this analysis were collected as part of ‘Generation Pup’, an ongoing large, longitudinal questionnaire-based project, primarily investigating risk factors for dog health and behaviour. Owners living in the UK or the ROI, aged 16 years or older, with puppies of any breed or cross breed are asked to sign up to the study providing that their puppy is aged under 16 weeks at the time of recruitment, or up to 21 weeks of age at registration if entering the UK or ROI through quarantine. Further details on the methodology related to the project (e.g. time-points for surveys, length of time surveys were available for) can be found elsewhere (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021). Owners answer questionnaires on their dogs’ health and behaviour throughout their dogs’ lives. Owners can elect to join the Generation Pup project, and awareness of the project is promoted in a number of ways, including through printed flyers at veterinary practices, electronic advertisements on social media, press articles, dog training classes (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021) and stalls at public events such as Discover Dogs. If an owner had more than one puppy enrolled in the study, one puppy was selected at random to avoid clustering at the level of the household.

Ethical approval and considerations

All data used for risk factor analyses were obtained from the ‘Generation Pup’ database. Puppies included in the study were born between March 2017 and November 2018. The ‘Generation Pup’ project received ethical approval from the Social Science Ethical Review Board at the Royal Veterinary College (URN SR2017-1116), the University of Bristol Animal Welfare Ethical Research Board (UIN/18/052) and the Dogs Trust Ethical Review Board (ERB009). All animals included in the study were dogs belonging to owners who voluntarily signed up to ‘Generation Pup’ to answer questionnaires regarding their dog’s health and behaviour. ‘Generation Pup’ owners are directed to contact a clinical animal behaviourist via specific websites (www.asab.org.uk and www.apbc.org.uk) if they are concerned about their dogs’ behaviour but are advised that ‘Generation Pup’ researchers cannot provide direct help or advice regarding their dogs’ behaviour.

Data collection

Data for the current study were comprised from selected owner responses to ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires completed upon signing up to the study (‘About Me’, ‘About My Puppy’, and ‘About My Household’), at ‘Settling In’, 12 weeks, 16 weeks, and six months (Table 1) (questionnaire files are available as supplementary material in Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021). Owners answered questions regarding the early life of their puppies (≤ 16 weeks) by filling out one to three questionnaires depending on puppy age at registration (Table 1). The timings of the questionnaires used in the present study are shown in Table 1 (for further details, see Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021).

Table 1. List of ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires used for the present study and the corresponding age that dogs reached when owners received each questionnaire

* Questionnaires from which ‘≤ 16 weeks’ time-point data were extracted. Data from the latest time-point available for each question were used.

Depending on when owners acquired their puppies, owners completed differing combinations of the ‘Settling In’, 12-week and 16-week questionnaires, each of which contained similar, overlapping questions (Table 1; Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021). This meant that equivalent questions across the three questionnaires needed to be condensed down to create only one response per dog. Therefore, for each dog, only the latest available response from the three questionnaires was taken. This created a new ‘≤ 16 weeks’ time-point for some variables.

Collection of risk factor data

Information about early life factors and owner-dog interactions was gained from questions across the ‘About Me’, ‘About My Puppy’, ‘About My Household’, ≤ 16 weeks (see above/Table 1 footnote), and six-month ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires (Table 1). Most questions, but not all, were compulsory (throughout all time-points). Questions were presented with multiple choice or ‘tick one box’ options and/or as free-text options and were either completed online or on paper copies. Each survey included questions supporting more than one study, so the overall aim of the ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires of exploring potential risk factors for dog health and behavioural problems throughout dogs’ lives (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021) was communicated to respondents, rather than the specific aim of the current study. This may have minimised response bias towards answers that owners believed might align with preventing SRBs. However, because each survey took an estimated 20 to 30 min to complete (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021), to motivate respondents to complete later sections about SRBs, respondents were told that their answers would ‘help us make recommendations that can be used to help dogs adjust to being left alone in the future’. Certain questions required the owner to report how they had responded to behaviour in the last seven days only, to reduce recall bias. Responses regarding puppy factors (e.g. source, breed, sex, and age joined household), owner factors (age, education, experience with dogs), experiential factors (sleep, house-training, experiences) and owner responses to training and behaviour across the early questionnaires, at ≤ 16 weeks and at six months were considered as potential risk factors (see Table S1; Supplementary material).

Six-month questionnaire: SRB case definition

The six-month ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaire was used to identify which dogs did, or did not, perform SRBs at six months of age, based on owner-report of dog behaviour. It included a section entitled ‘My puppy’s day’ which comprised questions on dog responses to several common situations including when the puppy was left alone without human company (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Kinsman, Lord, Da Costa, Woodward, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Tasker, Knowles and Casey2021). Relevant questions which were used to define the dependent variable for analysis included those on how dogs behaved (Table 2): when the owner was relaxing with their dog; when the dog was left alone without human company; when the owner was not paying attention to their dog; and when the owner was separated from the dog by a baby gate or door. Dog behaviour when left alone was split into two questions: how the dog behaved when left alone without human company; and how the dog behaved upon owner return following a period of separation (Table 2, Scenario 2 question 1 & 2). Owners were presented with a list of possible behaviour responses to these scenarios and, for each behaviour, owners were asked to tick one of four options: ‘Definitely yes’, ‘Definitely no’, ‘I don’t know’, or ‘Not applicable’. For each question ‘Other (please specify)’ was included as a possible response where owners could fill in a free-text response. The wording of the relevant questions used to create the case definition, and the presented scenarios are listed in Table 2.

Table 2. Scenarios, question wording and potential responses from the ‘Generation Pup’ six-month questionnaire used to create the case definition for SRB occurrence in puppies at six months of age. Three scenarios were presented to the owners, as follows. Scenario 1: Owner relaxing with dog; Scenario 2: Dog left alone without human company (split over two questions); and Scenario 3: Owner not paying attention to dog.

SRB: Separation-related behaviours.

Missing data

Some dogs had missing values for different variables meaning the sample size available varied for each explanatory variable. ‘Prefer not to answer’ responses were treated as missing data as appropriate for relevant questions.

Data processing

Data recategorisation for analysis

Variables were combined as necessary to allow comparisons between categories. Adjacent or similar categories were combined where possible if one or both contained low numbers of respondents (categories with less than ten individuals) and if combining categories made logical sense. For example, at univariable analysis only five individual breeds were compared to unknown crossbreeds, known crossbreeds and other pure breeds due to low sample size in other breed categories. Further condensed categories were then created depending on results of initial univariable analyses to allow variables to be investigated further. For example, breed data were further condensed into crossbreeds, Labrador Retrievers, and other pure breeds as there was no evidence of significant associations between other pure breeds and SRBs. The new categories derived from the original questionnaire data are described in Tables S1 and S2 in the Supplementary material.

Owners could choose from a series of potential responses for correcting behaviour they would like to discourage in a number of different scenarios (when the owner was not giving the dog attention, when separated by a baby gate, relaxing at home with the owner, and when the owner returned to the household) ranging from telling the dog off to cuddling them (Table 3), in any combination. Comparison of the four different types of training (positive punishment, negative reinforcement, positive reinforcement, negative punishment) (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Bolster, Richards, Loftus and Casey2012) was thus challenging (categorisation summarised in Table 3). Therefore, for simplicity, owners who indicated they used two or more positive punishment techniques (referred to here as aversive techniques) were compared to owners who used fewer than two positive punishment techniques. Owners were categorised this way to try and prevent including owners who had, for example, shouted at their dog once, with owners who generally used aversive techniques when interacting with their dog.

Table 3. Categorisation of owner responses to questions regarding correcting their puppy for ‘bad’ behaviours in different scenarios (when owner not giving the dog attention, separated by a baby gate or door, relaxing at home with the owner, when owner returned to the household) from the ‘Generation Pup’ six-month questionnaire according to training categories as described in Blackwell et al. (Reference Blackwell, Bolster, Richards, Loftus and Casey2012)

Categories within variables were also recategorised to prevent multicollinearity as identified by inflated standard errors during initial univariable analysis. For example, at ≤ 16 weeks owners were asked what techniques they used to reward their puppy during training (Table S1; Supplementary material). Owners were able to tick any of the following in any combination: verbal praise, physical praise, a toy, a ball, a clicker, some of his/her normal dog food, a novel dog food (‘a dog food other than his/her own kibble/biscuit’), dog food treats, human food, none/no other rewards are used, training has not started or other. For the purposes of analyses, these responses were categorised into owners who ticked dog treats or novel kibble, or owners who did not tick either of these options following initial univariable analysis of the binary reward categories. This was carried out to try and capture differences in approaches to reward techniques by combining those categories which had reached threshold significance in initial univariable analysis screening. Miscellaneous responses were categorised into one of the existing categories where possible; otherwise, responses were coded as ‘other’.

Case definition

Vocal, destructive (Bradshaw et al. Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002), repetitive behaviours (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006) and inappropriate toileting (McCrave Reference McCrave1991) are commonly reported canine SRBs, so these behaviour categories were used to define cases. To create a binary case/control outcome variable for logistic regression analysis, ‘Generation Pup’ six-month questionnaire responses were used to categorise dogs as ‘cases’ if they were reported to show one or more of the following behaviours when left alone without human company:

  • • Spun or turned in a circle;

  • • Paced/ran back and forth;

  • • Barked;

  • • Howled;

  • • Pined, whined, or cried;

  • • Scratched/damaged around the door or entrance in to and out of the house;

  • • Scratched/damaged furniture;

  • • Scratched/damaged skirting boards;

  • • Chewed/damaged other things which belong to him/her (for example his/her bed);

  • • Chewed/damaged items that belong to people in the household;

  • • Weed inside the house or where he/she should not;

  • • Pooed inside the house or where he/she should not.

Dogs displaying these behaviours when people were in the household, e.g. not giving the dog attention, separated by a baby gate, relaxing at home, were excluded as cases. Cases were thus classed as dogs who were reported to display any one or more of the SRBs listed above, but only when the dog was left alone, but not when the owners remained within the home.

Free-text responses were recoded and allocated to pre-existing categories where appropriate, and missing data and ambiguous cases/controls were excluded (e.g. when an owner provided information regarding their puppy’s behaviour when left alone in the car, rather than when left alone in the home).

Control definition

Control dogs were those who did not show any of the 12 behaviours listed above when left alone (indicative of SRB) or separated at home by a door or baby gate (an ambiguous situation where dogs may be separated from owners and may or may not be able to see/be aware of the owners’ presence in the household), but they could display any of these behaviours in the other two contexts: when the owner was relaxing or not paying attention to the dog.

Statistical analysis

Univariable logistic regression analysis

The SRB case definition (case; control) was used as the dependent variable in analyses. Univariable logistic regression analysis was conducted for each potential risk factor from the ‘About My Puppy’, ‘About My Household’, ‘About Me’, ‘Settling In’, ≤ 16-week, and six-month ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires. Non-modifiable factors, including dog sex, dog breed, and owner age were also investigated as potential risk factors. Explanatory variables which were liberally associated with SRBs (P ≤ 0.200) were taken forwards for multivariable analysis (Table S2; Supplementary material).

It should be noted that, while research has suggested particularly high levels of reported SRBs in dogs purchased during the COVID-19 pandemic (often known as ‘Pandemic Puppies’) (Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024), potentially due to reduced time left alone during national lockdowns (Harvey et al. Reference Harvey, Christley, Giragosian, Mead, Murray, Samet, Upjohn and Casey2022), data collection for this study was conducted prior to this time-period and therefore this effect was not investigated during analyses.

Multivariable logistic regression model selection

A multivariable logistic regression model, with the SRB response variable, was built using a forward stepwise model selection process. Variables were added to the model in order of P-value obtained in univariable analyses in ascending order. After each variable of interest was added to the model, an online Chi-squared calculator (Social Science Statistics 2020) was used to calculate whether the difference in Chi-squared values and degrees of freedom suggested the model was significantly improved by the addition of that variable; if significant, the variable would be retained in the model. Variables were added to the model until all variables were statistically significant (P < 0.050) and/or it was determined that the model fit was improved with the inclusion of a particular variable according to the Chi-squared calculator (Social Science Statistics 2020).

All analyses were conducted in the IBM SPSS® Statistics package (Version 26).

Results

Description of the sample

There were data for dogs belonging to a total of 1,987 respondents included in the original six-month dataset downloaded from ‘Generation Pup’. Dogs were excluded if they not been left alone without human company at six months (162 dogs), if key data were missing (1,218 dogs), or if data were ambiguous across response options for the case definition (e.g. ‘I don’t know’ or ‘Not applicable’ across all answer options) and if they did not fit the case definitions for either the cases or controls (462 dogs). Of the resulting 145 dogs included in the final model, there were 108 pure breed dogs (74.5%) and 37 cross breed dogs (25.5%). The most common breed was the Labrador Retriever, and the most common Kennel Club group for purebreds was gundog. There were slightly more male (52.4%) than female dogs. The mean (± SD) age at acquisition, excluding homebred puppies, was 61.9 (± 12.5) days; median = 58 days, equating to approximately eight weeks and six days old. Demographics of both dogs and owners are shown in Table 4.

Table 4. Demographics of dogs (n = 145) and owners included in the final multivariable model investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study ‘Generation Pup’. Categories without a natural scale are ordered from most to least common, followed by miscellaneous categories

SRB: Separation-related behaviours.

a Owner ticked the ‘Other’ option from the list of answers and provided further information. Examples of answers included information not relevant to the question, for example ‘Currently also have an adult dog’, or ‘Currently have 6 dogs’.

Almost one-third of owners were aged between 55 and 64 years, with the next most common age group being 35–44 years (Table 4). Most owners (73.1%) reported that they had owned at least one previous dog as an adult, whilst 22.0% reported that this was the first dog they had owned as an adult.

Description of separation experiences

At ≤ 16 weeks, most dogs (110; 75.9%) had been left alone during the day, eight (5.5%) had been left during both the day and overnight, and three dogs (2.1%) had been left alone overnight only. In contrast, 22 dogs (15.2%) had not been left alone without human company at all. Most dogs had been left for a maximum of 1–6 h (Figure 1).

Figure 1. The longest continuous duration for which dogs (n = 145) were left alone at 16 weeks and under in a study investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study, ‘Generation Pup’.

At the six-month time-point, all dogs had experienced being alone: 133 dogs (91.7%) had been left without human company during the day only, eight (5.5%) had been left alone both during the day and overnight, and four (2.8%) had been left alone overnight only. As at the ≤ 16-week time point, most dogs were left alone for a maximum of 1–6 h (Figure 2).

Figure 2. The longest continuous duration for which dogs (n = 145) were left alone in a study investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study, ‘Generation Pup’. Dogs were left during the day only (orange), overnight only (grey) and during the day and overnight (blue).

Sixty-eight of the puppies (46.9%) displayed SRBs at six months of age according to the case definition. Table 5 shows the number of dogs performing each SRB, only when left alone. The most commonly reported SRB was pacing (14.5%), followed by whining (7.6%) and spinning/circling (6.9%).

Table 5. Number of dogs (n = 145) performing each canine SRB of interest at six months of age (only when left alone) using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study Generation Pup. The SRBs are arranged from most to least commonly reported.

SRB: Separation-related behaviours.

Multivariable analysis

The final model consisted of eight explanatory variables, explaining 0.526–0.702 of the total variation (Cox & Snell R Square to Nagelkerke R Square values). The final model is shown in Table 6 (univariable results are available in Table S2; Supplementary material).

Table 6. Multivariable logistic regression model for risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study ‘Generation Pup’ (n = 145). Significance threshold: P < 0.050

SRB: Separation-related behaviours.

a Aversive techniques: Tell him/her off, physically reprimand/correct him/her (for example smack or tap on nose or bottom), distract him/her (for example with a rattled can/loud noise/water spray), physically manipulate him/her (for example hold still, put into a sit, or push off if he/she jumps up), hold him/her still.

b Time-out methods: Move away from him/her, don’t give him/her treats that would normally be given at this time, ignore him/her, move him/her to another part of the house.

Dogs had significantly lower odds of SRBs if they had experienced being in an enclosed space at night at ≤ 16 weeks compared to dogs who had not been in an enclosed space at this time-point. Those who had 9+ h of sleep at night at ≤ 16 weeks had significantly decreased odds of showing SRBs at six months, compared with those who slept a maximum of 6–8 h at this time. Dogs were more likely to show SRBs if they were not yet fully house-trained at ≤ 16 weeks. The odds of SRBs were also increased if owners used special kibble or treats during training at ≤ 16 weeks (Table 6) and were greatly increased if they used two or more aversive training techniques, or time-out methods (negative punishment) at six months across different scenarios (when their puppy was receiving no attention and relaxing, respectively; Table 6). Owners who ‘fussed’ over (greeted/stroked/cuddled/played with) their dogs at six months in response to ‘bad’ behaviour when returning to the household also saw increased odds of their dogs showing SRBs at six months (Table 6).

Additionally, dogs belonging to younger owners (aged 16 to 34 years) had increased odds of SRBs compared to owners aged 35 years and older. No other owner or dog characteristics reached statistical significance in the final model.

Discussion

This is the first study, to the authors’ knowledge, that has explored the potential effects of early life risk factors, dog-owner interactions, and general dog and owner risk factors, for SRBs at six months of age. The results suggest that overnight conditions in the early lives of puppies and owner interactions with their puppies are important factors influencing SRB development. More specifically, experience of being in an enclosed space overnight and increased sleep at ≤ 16 weeks appear to serve as a protective function against SRB development at six months of age. Conversely, younger owners, poor house-training progression, and aversive approaches to training used at this time may increase the likelihood of SRBs. Positive and negative punishment across different situations at six months of age greatly increased a dog’s likelihood of SRBs, as did fussing over dogs upon reunion.

We identified an SRB prevalence of 46.9%, which is higher than previously reported in some owner-report studies (Bradshaw et al. Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002; Dinwoodie et al. Reference Dinwoodie, Dwyer, Zottola, Gleason and Dodman2019; Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024), but comparable to other estimates (Soares et al. Reference Soares, Pereira and Paixão2010; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016). Differences in populations and ages of dogs, as well as methodologies and case definitions are likely to explain varying prevalence estimates. For example, this study specifically relates to puppies at six months of age, and excluded ambiguous dogs based on stringent case: control definitions. In a questionnaire study, such as this, we might expect to have underestimated SRB prevalence through owners being unaware of SRBs that generate no audible or physical evidence, but it is notable that the most commonly reported SRBs here included covert repetitive behaviours (pacing and spinning). In the six-month ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaire, one of the questions regarding SRBs (Table 2) referred to behaviours the dogs showed upon owner return rather when the owner had been absent. This means the case definition encapsulates both SRBs and behaviours displayed upon initial owner return in response to owner absence, so perhaps the pacing and spinning occurred once owners returned, rather than when they were absent. It is also possible that owners participating in the ‘Generation Pup’ survey are especially vigilant to their dogs’ behaviour generally and may own cameras to observe their dogs when they are alone, for example.

The results apply to SRBs at six months of age only and relate only to the overt behaviours reported to occur during separation, rather than to the dogs’ underlying welfare states, so caution should be exercised in not over-interpreting the findings. Also, the exploratory nature of the study means that many potential risk factors were tested, so it is possible that some results are Type I (falsely significant) (Bender & Lange Reference Bender and Lange2001) and Type II errors (false negative) due to a small sample size and low statistical power (Mascha & Vetter Reference Mascha and Vetter2018). Nevertheless, the findings of this study could help inform future research in this area. The potential risk factors identified in the present study fall into broad themes and will be discussed in more detail accordingly: enclosed spaces and sleep, house-training, training approaches, owner responses upon reunion, and other remaining factors.

Enclosed spaces and sleep

In the present study, puppies with experience of being in an enclosed space overnight at ≤ 16 weeks had decreased odds of developing SRBs, compared with puppies with no experience of being in an enclosed space. While a number of sources suggest that providing dogs with a crate or safe place during separation may help to reduce anxiety around owner departure (e.g. Tuber et al. Reference Tuber, Miller, Caris, Halter, Linden and Hennessy1999; Houpt et al. Reference Houpt, Goodwin, Uchida, Baranyiová, Fatjó and Kakuma2007), others have found that crated dogs show more yawning and lip-licking during separation than non-crated dogs (Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010) and emphasise the importance of ensuring dogs are not forced into enclosed spaces as punishment for bad behaviour (Schwartz Reference Schwartz2003). There is little previous evidence to suggest that experience of crates or enclosed spaces, particularly overnight, may be protective against SRBs. However, there is some evidence suggesting that other sleeping experiences may contribute to SRB expression. For example, studies have suggested that allowing dogs to sleep in a bedroom or on the owner’s bed could potentially exacerbate SRBs (Podberscek et al. Reference Podberscek, Hsu and Serpell1999), such as separation-related urination and defaecation (Jagoe & Serpell Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996). However, Flannigan and Dodman found no association between SRBs and allowing dogs to sleep on the owner’s bed (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001). Whilst previous research is slightly ambiguous, our results lend weight towards overnight enclosed spaces being associated with reduced SRB development. The result is correlational, and whether dogs sleep alone or with owners has had associations with other putative diagnoses (e.g. Hoppe et al. Reference Hoppe, Bininda-Emonds and Gansloßer2017). Recommendations need to be treated with caution, and treatment plans should always be based on the specific diagnosis and treatment plan for individual dogs.

The direction of causation underlying the association between overnight enclosure use and reduced SRBs requires further investigation. One explanation could be that, even if many owners initially attempt to enclose their dogs overnight, this is only successful for those dogs who are less distressed by separation; owners may quickly give up if their dogs show signs of stress. If this is the case, then recommending owners to place puppies in enclosed spaces, such as crates, overnight would not necessarily be constructive. Palestrini and colleagues (Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010), in their video study, found that crated dogs showed increased yawning and lip-licking behaviour than those dogs not confined to a crate, and other studies found no association between owner-reported separation anxiety and whether dogs had been crated (Cannas et al. Reference Cannas, Frank, Minero, Godbout and Palestrini2010; Herron et al. Reference Herron, Lord and Husseini2014).

Alternatively, overnight use of enclosed spaces could be helpful in reducing SRB risk. Overnight experience of spatial restriction may prepare puppies for being alone for longer periods during the day when they are older. Enclosed spaces also enable a degree of separation, albeit with dogs potentially being able to see/hear their owners nearby, thereby creating a step between being with the owner and being entirely alone when owners are absent from the home. Crating puppies, or offering them an alternative enclosed space, if trained with positive reinforcement from an early age, may help to create a safe ‘den’ for them (Houpt et al. Reference Houpt, Goodwin, Uchida, Baranyiová, Fatjó and Kakuma2007). Puppies may learn to relax when partially separated from owners in such enclosed places, and associate them with going to sleep, so they may be less distressed at being fully separated from their owner when this occurs later. Our data did not include information on whether or not owners were nearby while puppies were enclosed, nor whether the breeders had carried out sleep or crate training before the puppies went to their homes, but these factors could be important in determining whether enclosed spaces help to reduce SRBs.

We also found that puppies were less likely to develop SRBs if their owners reported that the puppies slept ≥ 9 h at night at ≤ 16 weeks, compared to those who had only 6–8 h of sleep at this time. Dogs have been reported to sleep between 7.7 and 16 h (for a review, see Campbell & Tobler Reference Campbell and Tobler1984) and puppies tend to spend the majority of their time sleeping or resting (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Minero, Cannas and Palestrini2007; Cannas et al. Reference Cannas, Frank, Minero, Godbout and Palestrini2010). However, Kinsman and colleagues (Reference Kinsman, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Casey, Knowles, Tasker, Woodward, Da Costa and Murray2020) reported that puppies at 16 weeks of age slept on average 7 h, while dogs at 12 months slept an average of 7.3 h (Kinsman et al. Reference Kinsman, Owczarczak-Garstecka, Casey, Knowles, Tasker, Woodward, Da Costa and Murray2020). These data and those of the present study are based upon owner-report so we cannot be sure of their accuracy and whether they are representative of the true sleeping habits of dogs. The likelihood that any over- /under-estimates of sleep are associated with SRB occurrence is likely to be low; any similar research in future could incorporate actual sleep data to increase the validity and accuracy of results.

Our result aligns with previous research showing that dogs without SRBs tended to sleep and rest more than dogs diagnosed with separation anxiety (Scaglia et al. Reference Scaglia, Cannas, Minero, Frank, Bassi and Palestrini2013). The direction of causality could be either way, with dogs who happen to be less anxious (and thus arguably less likely to perform SRBs) sleeping better, or alternatively with dogs who are enabled to sleep for longer being more resilient to challenges including separation. In humans and rodents, those with disturbed sleep are less likely to be able to cope with stressful environments (Meerlo et al. Reference Meerlo, Sgoifo and Suchecki2008). Encouraging more sleep and relaxation at early ages, for example, by establishing clear routines of activity and rest for puppies during the day (Dogs Trust 2021), could potentially decrease the likelihood of dogs displaying SRBs as they age; further research could investigate this over a longer time-scale.

House-training

Puppies whose house-training was still ongoing at ≤ 16 weeks, with ≥ 4 accidents per week, had increased odds of displaying SRBs at six months, compared with puppies who had 0–3 accidents per week at that time-point. House soiling is commonly reported in dogs with separation problems (McCrave Reference McCrave1991) and thought to be a sign of general anxiety. Accordingly, toileting was included in the case definition, which could have meant that this result was inevitable or circular. However, the case definition specified that SRBs had to only occur when the puppy was left alone and not in scenarios when the owner was present. There were very few dogs for whom toileting was their only SRB; just 6/68 cases in the study only urinated and/or defaecated without other SRBs when left alone. Another consideration is that the ‘Generation Pup’ question regarded accidents and house-training in general, and the context of these accidents was not specified. Owners may have included separation-related soiling in their estimation of toileting accidents. Thus, this result may rather represent a correlation such that dogs with separation-related toileting at ≤ 16 weeks were also likely to show SRBs at six months. Inappropriate elimination can also occur for many reasons such as medical problems, excitement, anxiety, fear or marking behaviour (McCrave Reference McCrave1991), which should be considered.

Training approaches

Owners who used ≥ 2 aversive techniques when trying to discourage unwanted behaviours when relaxing at home or when no one was paying attention to their dog, when their dogs were six months of age, had greatly increased odds of their dogs displaying SRBs compared to those who used ≤ 1 aversive training technique in these scenarios. This is in general agreement with the existing literature regarding the relationship between aversive training styles and problem behaviours overall (Hiby et al. Reference Hiby, Rooney and Bradshaw2004; Bräm Dubé et al. Reference Bräm Dubé, Asher, Würbel, Riemer and Melotti2020). However, previous results on whether aversive techniques affect SRBs specifically are mixed. For instance, a study that compared dogs trained using reward-based or aversive techniques during the Ainsworth Strange Situation Test, did not find any difference between the two types of training for SRBs, such as whining or exit-directed behaviours (Vieira de Castro et al. Reference Vieira de Castro, Barrett, de Sousa and Olsson2019). Stopping punishment of SRBs upon owner return, among other interventions, did effectively reduce SRB development in newly adopted rescue dogs (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016), but some authors have suggested that stopping punishment may not play a large role in reducing SRBs (Butler et al. Reference Butler, Sargisson and Elliffe2011; Herron et al. 2014). Intervention programmes also often introduce multiple interventions at once, making interpretation of the efficacy of individual elements challenging (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006).

We found that the use of aversive techniques and punishment upon owner return (as opposed to when relaxing or withholding attention) was not significantly associated with SRBs in the final model, despite reaching threshold significance in univariable analyses. While this result could be further investigated with a different dataset, this may suggest that punishment after separation does not necessarily affect SRBs’ outcomes, as suggested by Butler et al. (Reference Butler, Sargisson and Elliffe2011). Perhaps owner interactions at other times, such as relaxing and no attention, affect SRB presentation via more general effects on dog anxiety levels. In addition, logically, punishment on return would most likely lead to anxiety about owner return rather than influencing anxiety about owner absence.

We also found that puppies of owners who used ‘time-outs’ when correcting their puppy when the owner was relaxing, also showed increased odds of developing SRBs. Our definition of ‘time-outs’ included negative punishment methods where owner affection or attention was removed (Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Bubna-Littitz, Bartels, Futschik and Troxler2010; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Bolster, Richards, Loftus and Casey2012). These techniques are sometimes favoured by owners because they perceive them as kinder or less likely to cause fear, than punishment. However, in the human literature, disciplinary techniques involving ignoring or rejecting children are generally considered to be controversial (Dadds & Tully Reference Dadds and Tully2019) and potentially harmful for child mental health when used in the extreme (Arslan Reference Arslan2017). In dogs, a questionnaire-based study found that negative punishment increased the number of reported behaviour problems, but only where dogs scored highly for a ‘canine sensory processing sensitivity’ personality trait: a trait suggested to occur when dogs experience emotions strongly, making them more susceptible to stress (Bräm Dubé et al. Reference Bräm Dubé, Asher, Würbel, Riemer and Melotti2020). This is a relatively new concept, but potentially the dogs reacting to negative punishment in the present study, may be particularly sensitive to this type of training, highlighting individuality of response. Blackwell and colleagues (2008) found that 52% of owners in their study reported using negative punishment in combination with positive punishment and/or positive reinforcement, but they found no association between training type and SRBs (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008). We found no effect of positive reinforcement on SRBs in the present study, which may suggest a greater impact of aversive interactions on puppy behaviour; however most owners in the study used positive reinforcement, so we may not have had sufficient variation to detect an effect.

These results concerning negative punishment and time-outs, conflict with many SRB behavioural modification plans which often suggest ignoring the dogs or only interacting with the dog upon returning to the house when the dog is calm (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2006; Butler et al. Reference Butler, Sargisson and Elliffe2011). Similarly, owners may use crates/kennels as punishment or ‘time-out’ areas (Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals [RSPCA] 2014). Puppies may acquire negative associations with the crate where they are meant to sleep and relax, and if the dog has problems with separation and with being enclosed, this may exacerbate their distress (Schwartz Reference Schwartz2003; Sherman & Mills Reference Sherman and Mills2008; Todd Reference Todd2018). Given the findings discussed earlier that experience of enclosed space at ≤ 16 weeks overnight was associated with decreased odds of SRBs at six months, it may be worth noting in preventative advice for owners to only use crates in association with positive reinforcement.

The type of positive reinforcer used may also be important, because puppies at ≤ 16 weeks who were rewarded with novel dog food treats when being trained to carry out a new behaviour had increased odds of displaying SRBs at six months of age. If this result is not a Type I error, it is unexpected and conflicts with previous research suggesting that reward-based training, including using food treats, is associated with lower numbers of owner-reported problem behaviours, compared with mixed or punishment-based training techniques (Hiby et al. Reference Hiby, Rooney and Bradshaw2004).

One explanation could be that, if owners perceive that their dogs are anxious or depressed, they may attempt to ‘cheer them up’ with novel dog treats. This could mean that provision of these rewards is a response to SRB-prone dogs, rather than a cause of SRB development. Alternatively, it is possible that there are different ‘types’ of owners: those who buy high value treats or ‘sweets’ for their dogs and those who just use normal dog food as treats. This may be linked to other owner behaviours that could influence SRBs. The link between treat-giving and problem behaviours could then be further investigated, as treat-giving is often seen by owners as being related to the dog-owner bond (Linder & Mueller Reference Linder and Mueller2014; White et al. Reference White, Ward, Pink, Craigon and Millar2016). Finally, preventing SRBs can be viewed under the general strategy of managing interactions with dogs and controlling owner attention in response to how a dog behaves. Owners who are very focused on ‘training’ with treats to specifically train behaviours may focus on this rather than on the use of their attention as an important reinforcer (Chrysafis et al. Reference Chrysafis, Payne and Salazar2023). Only using food as a reinforcer in training may not be as effective as owner attention. In summary, the unexpected nature of this result, and the variety of possible explanations for it, means that we would not necessarily recommend that owners refrain from using novel dog treats as rewards.

Owner responses upon returning home

We found that puppies whose owners responded to behaviour they would like to discourage when returning to their puppies at six months, by fussing over them, had significantly increased odds of displaying SRBs at six months. Fussing over puppies in response to behaviour owners would like to encourage upon owner return was not retained in the final model but was significant at univariable analysis. Consistent with our finding, some studies recommend that owners ignore their dog before departure and after returning to the household and to only initiate contact when the dog is relaxed (King et al. Reference King, Simpson, Overall, Appleby, Pageat, Ross, Chaurand, Heath, Beata, Weiss, Muller, Paris, Bataille, Parker, Petit and Wren2000; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016). Owners are also often advised to avoid excessive greeting behaviour upon returning to their dog (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016). McGreevy and Masters (Reference McGreevy and Masters2008) found that dogs of owners who played with them in the first 30 min of returning were more likely to display SRBs. The authors suggest the dogs may be anticipating the play behaviour upon owner return and become aroused accordingly (McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008). Remaining calm during departure and arrival may prevent anxious responses from being reinforced at this time (Teixeira & Hall Reference Teixeira and Hall2021) and may make coming and going generally less emotive and meaningful for the dog. Despite conflicting evidence from Teixeira and Hall (Reference Teixeira and Hall2021) who found no evidence of high arousal/excited greetings or reunions leading to increased SRBs in rescue dogs, our study appears to support the theory that calm interactions may help prevent SRBs. However, implementing this may be challenging for owners who may be inclined to greet their dog enthusiastically upon their return, meaning owner compliance to this type of advice is often poor (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Casey and Bradshaw2016).

Despite the theory, because the SRB outcome variable and the ‘fussing’ result were both at six months of age and the data are correlational, it is possible that the direction of causation may be in either direction. Owners may have been more likely to fuss over their dogs to reassure them upon return to the household, if the owners were already aware that their dogs were showing SRBs. Moreover, Konok et al. (Reference Konok, Dóka and Miklósi2011) found that dogs who were distressed during separation tests were more likely to be ‘happy’ and show increased greeting behaviour towards their owners when reunited. Again, this means that it is difficult to make a recommendation, because avoiding fussing over puppies when greeting them may be a response to, rather than a cause of, SRBs.

Other factors

We found that dogs belonging to younger owners (aged 16–34) had increased odds of displaying SRBs at six months compared to owners aged ≥ 35 years. While Bennett and Rohlf (Reference Bennett and Rohlf2007) found conflicting results, similar findings were reported by Konok et al. (Reference Konok, Kosztolányi, Rainer, Mutschler, Halsband and Miklósi2015), and Brand and colleagues (Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024) who found increased odds of dogs having SRBs if their owners were aged 25 to 34 years of age compared to if owners were 45 to 54 years old (Bennett & Rohlf Reference Bennett and Rohlf2007; Konok et al. Reference Konok, Kosztolányi, Rainer, Mutschler, Halsband and Miklósi2015; Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024). Owner age is likely to be a proxy for an unmeasured variable (e.g. owner routine), but replication of the study would be needed to establish any further association.

We did not find significant associations between SRBs and many previously identified risk factors. For example, previous studies have suggested that mixed breed dogs were more likely to display SRBs than pure breed dogs (Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001). However, in the present study, breed was only significant at univariable analysis, with increased odds in mixed breed dogs and decreased odds in Labrador Retrievers (see Supplementary material) but did not contribute to the final model. Bradshaw et al. (Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002) similarly found that SRB occurrence was not significantly associated with breed in a cross-sectional questionnaire study of dog walkers comparing SRB occurrence in mixed breed and pedigree dog breeds. Similarly, we found no effect of whether puppies attended puppy classes at ≤ 16 weeks, but rather the ways in which owners trained and interacted with their puppies seemed to influence SRB occurrence at six months. This has been previously reported (e.g. González-Martínez et al. Reference González-Martínez, Martínez, Rosado, Luño, Santamarina, Suárez, Camino, de la Cruz and Diéguez2019) but contrasts with others (Jagoe & Serpell Reference Jagoe and Serpell1996; Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001).

We also found no effect of maximum time left alone at six months. This factor was positively associated with SRB occurrence at 16 weeks at the univariable stage of analysis, but did not make the final model. It is recommended that owners do not leave their dogs > 4 h (Dogs Trust 2005) and previous research has suggested that dogs left for > 4 h show increased greeting behaviour, shaking and lip-licking upon owner arrival (Rehn & Keeling Reference Rehn and Keeling2011). Interestingly, a recent paper reporting the results of a cohort study found that dogs aged 21 months of age had reduced odds of SRB if dogs had been left alone for > 4 h at 21 months compared to those who had never been left alone; however, these dogs were purchased during the 2020 pandemic and results may have been affected by owners increasing the amount of time their dogs were left alone compared to during UK lockdowns (Brand et al. Reference Brand, O’Neill, Belshaw, Dale, Merritt, Clover, Tay, Pegram and Packer2024). Repetition of the present study with higher statistical power may allow the influence of both the pandemic and this variable to be investigated further as evidence on the influence of the maximum time left alone at an early age would be helpful in guiding new puppy owners on how to approach leaving their puppies.

Investigation of risk factors for SRBs in dogs older than six months may be beneficial, as research has suggested that owners tend to report canine behavioural problems at ages older than this. Indeed, one study noted that owners tended to report behavioural problems to veterinary practices when dogs were aged an average of 3.7 years (Bamberger & Houpt Reference Bamberger and Houpt2006). Furthermore, Lund and colleagues (Reference Lund, Agger and Vestergaard1996) found that all behavioural problems reported in their study occurred between 18 and 33 months of age. Despite this, the present study provides evidence that signs of SRBs can occur at an early stage in a puppy’s life and emphasises the importance of effective management from an early age in order to prevent the development of these problems. Further analyses of data from the ‘Generation Pup’ cohort could investigate SRBs in these dogs at a later age to learn more about the development of SRBs over time and the long-term influence of early life factors and owner interactions.

The sample size was relatively small in this study compared to other studies (e.g. approximately 200–700: Flannigan & Dodman Reference Flannigan and Dodman2001; McGreevy & Masters Reference McGreevy and Masters2008; van Rooy et al. Reference van Rooy, Thomson, McGreevy and Wade2018), resulting in low statistical power. Due to the multifactorial nature of separation problems in dogs it may be that there are many other factors that influence SRB occurrence at six months which were not covered in the present study. While we should be careful not to over-interpret the current findings, they can help prompt further research into the potential influence of early life experiences and owner responses towards their dogs on future development of SRBs using larger sample sizes. Future replication of the present study could also consider alternative explanations for SRBs and rule these out by considering the medical history of the dogs involved in the study (McCrave Reference McCrave1991). This was not conducted for the present study, but this information is available for ‘Generation Pup’ dogs whose owners have consented to permit access to their dog’s veterinary records; this could be an avenue to explore in future ‘Generation Pup’ studies. It is also important to note that due to the fact that SRBs can occur for a number of underlying reasons (e.g. frustration, fear, anxiety or boredom, see McCrave Reference McCrave1991; Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010a; Burn Reference Burn2017; de Assis et al. Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020), the group of behaviours may be considered as a syndrome (a group of behaviours which regularly occur together in a context with more than one underlying cause) meaning treatment can be challenging (de Assis et al. Reference de Assis, Matos, Pike, Burman and Mills2020). There is a range of terminology and proposed case definitions for SRBs, and it is important that each individual case has a diagnosis to inform the treatment plan for that dog.

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

This study identified potential early-life risk factors for SRBs in dogs at six months of age. SRBs are a complicated problem in dogs which can have serious welfare implications, and early identification is important in preventing their development. It will be important that any reductions in SRB development are associated with genuine improvements to canine welfare during separation, rather than merely being reductions of the behavioural manifestations of separation-related distress.

Poor house-training progress at ≤ 16 weeks increased a puppy’s likelihood of showing problems with being left alone at six months. Providing puppies with positive experiences of being left alone and enclosed overnight from an early age, as well as encouraging sufficient sleep and rest, could help reduce the chance of puppies developing issues with being left alone at six months of age and potentially SRB that continues as the dog ages.

Many of our findings were associated with how owners responded to unfavourable dog behaviour across different contexts, but generally suggest that aversive techniques during dog-owner interactions and fussing during reunion may lead to an increased risk of SRBs at six months. We recommend that owners avoid both positive and negative punishment techniques during the general handling of puppies, as well as effusively greeting dogs upon returning to the household after separation.

Caution should be exercised in not over-interpreting these exploratory results, and efforts should be made to replicate with a larger sample. However, future research could further investigate the factors detailed here and in older dogs, and the effects of owner interactions in different scenarios and training approaches in earlier life to allow further understanding of the complicated nature of canine SRBs. We recommend replicating this study, with the addition of video footage (Palestrini et al. Reference Palestrini, Minero, Cannas, Rossi and Frank2010) to potentially see the effect of time left alone, which would additionally allow subtler SRBs, such as pacing, spinning and trembling, to be detected with greater certainty than in questionnaire-based studies. Furthermore, repetition of the study could further investigate the potential influence of puppy provenance and the pandemic on SRBs in the long-term. These results are promising and may ultimately improve animal welfare and contribute to positive change regarding how owners approach responding to puppy ‘problem behaviours’, especially as the results reported here indicate that owner interactions may play a part in this behavioural problem.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.56.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the ‘Generation Pup’ team for their support, as well as the study participants for completing surveys and for their commitment to the ‘Generation Pup’ project. The authors are also very grateful to the Dogs Trust for funding the first three years of FD’s PhD, during which the study was conducted, and Deborah Wells and Dominic Mellor for their comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Competing interest

The study was funded by the Dogs Trust, an animal welfare charity interested in identifying and reducing factors having a negative impact on dog welfare. JM and RC are employed by the Dogs Trust. The Dogs Trust did not influence findings or reporting.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: RC, FD, CB, JM; Data curation: FD, JM; Formal analysis: FD; Funding acquisition: RC, CB; Investigation: FD, CB, RC; Methodology: FD, CB, JM, RC; Project administration: FD, CB, JM, RC; Resources: CB, JM, RC; Supervision: CB, JM, RC; Writing – original draft: FD; Writing – review & editing: FD, CB, JM, RC.

References

Appleby, D and Pluijmakers, J 2003 Separation anxiety in dogs: The function of homeostasis in its development and treatment. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 33(2): 321344. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-5616(02)00101-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appleby, DL, Bradshaw, JWS and Casey, RA 2002 Relationship between aggressive and avoidance behaviour by dogs and their experience in the first six months of life. Veterinary Record 150(14): 434438. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.150.14.434CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arhant, C, Bubna-Littitz, H, Bartels, A, Futschik, A and Troxler, J 2010 Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 123(3): 131142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arslan, G 2017 Psychological maltreatment, coping strategies, and mental health problems: A brief and effective measure of psychological maltreatment in adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect 68: 96106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.03.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bamberger, M and Houpt, KA 2006 Signalment factors, comorbidity, and trends in behavior diagnoses in dogs: 1,644 cases (1991-2001). Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 229(10): 15911601. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.229.10.1591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bender, R and Lange, S 2001 Adjusting for multiple testing - when and how? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 54(4): 343349. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00314-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, PC and Rohlf, VI 2007 Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102(1): 6584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.03.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, E, Casey, RA and Bradshaw, JWS 2006 Controlled trial of behavioural therapy for separation-related disorders in dogs. Veterinary Record 158(16): 551554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, EJ, Bolster, C, Richards, G, Loftus, BA and Casey, RA 2012 The use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs: estimated prevalence, reasons and risk factors for use, and owner perceived success as compared to other training methods. BMC Veterinary Research 8(1): 93. https://doi.org/10.1186/1746-6148-8-93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, EJ, Casey, RA and Bradshaw, JWS 2016 Efficacy of written behavioral advice for separation-related behavior problems in dogs newly adopted from a rehoming center. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 12: 1319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, EJ, Twells, C, Seawright, A and Casey, RA 2008 The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 3(5): 207217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2007.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, JWS, McPherson, JA, Casey, RA and Larter, IS 2002 Aetiology of separation-related behaviour in domestic dogs. Veterinary Record 151(2): 4346. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.151.2.43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brand, CL, O’Neill, DG, Belshaw, Z, Dale, FC, Merritt, BL, Clover, KN, Tay, M-XM, Pegram, CL and Packer, RMA 2024 Impacts of puppy early life experiences, puppy-purchasing practices, and owner characteristics on owner-reported problem behaviours in a UK pandemic puppies cohort at 21 months of Age. Animals https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14020336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bräm Dubé, M, Asher, L, Würbel, H, Riemer, S and Melotti, L 2020 Parallels in the interactive effect of highly sensitive personality and social factors on behaviour problems in dogs and humans. Scientific Reports 10(1): 5288. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-62094-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burn, C 2017 Bestial boredom: a biological perspective on animal boredom and suggestions for its scientific investigation. Animal Behaviour 130: 141151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R, Sargisson, RJ and Elliffe, D 2011 The efficacy of systematic desensitization for treating the separation-related problem behaviour of domestic dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 129(2–4): 136145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, SS and Tobler, I 1984 Animal sleep: A review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 8(3): 269300. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(84)90054-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannas, S, Frank, D, Minero, M, Godbout, M and Palestrini, C 2010 Puppy behavior when left home alone: Changes during the first few months after adoption. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 5(2): 94100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2009.08.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, RA, Loftus, B, Bolster, C, Richards, GJ and Blackwell, EJ 2014 Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 152: 5263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrysafis, EE, Payne, SW and Salazar, A 2023 Assessing human attention as a reinforcer for dog behavior. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice 23(4): 224237. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000274Google Scholar
Collins, N 1996 Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 810832. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.71.4.810CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, M and Tully, L 2019 What is it to discipline a child: What should it be? A reanalysis of time-out from the perspective of child mental health, attachment, and trauma. American Psychologist 74. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000449CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Assis, LS, Matos, R, Pike, TW, Burman, OHP and Mills, DS 2020 Developing diagnostic frameworks in veterinary behavioral medicine: Disambiguating separation related problems in dogs. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 6: 499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinwoodie, IR, Dwyer, B, Zottola, V, Gleason, D and Dodman, NH 2019 Demographics and comorbidity of behavior problems in dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 32: 6271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.04.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dogs Trust 2005 Do you have time for a dog if you have a full-time job? https://www.dogstrust.org.uk/help-advice/getting-or-buying-a-dog/do-you-have-time-for-a-dog-if-you-have-a-full-time-job (accessed 29 January 2021).Google Scholar
Dogs Trust 2021 Our advice for ensuring you and your puppy get a good night’s rest. https://www.dogstrust.org.uk/latest/2021/ensuring-you-and-your-puppy-get-a-good-night-s-rest (accessed 20 October 2024).Google Scholar
Elliot, O and Scott, JP 1961 The development of emotional distress reactions to separation, in puppies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 99(1): 322. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1961.10534386CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flannigan, G and Dodman, NH 2001 Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 219(4): 460466. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.2001.219.460CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foyer, P, Wilsson, E, Wright, D and Jensen, P 2013 Early experiences modulate stress coping in a population of German shepherd dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 146(1): 7987. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.03.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, D, Minero, M, Cannas, S and Palestrini, C 2007 Puppy behaviours when left home alone: A pilot study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 104(1–2): 6170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González-Martínez, Á, Martínez, MF, Rosado, B, Luño, I, Santamarina, G, Suárez, ML, Camino, F, de la Cruz, LF and Diéguez, FJ 2019 Association between puppy classes and adulthood behavior of the dog. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 32: 3641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.04.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, R, Butler, S, Douglas, C and Serpell, J 2016 Puppies from “puppy farms” show more temperament and behavioural problems than if acquired from other sources. Newcastle University: Newcastle, UK.Google Scholar
Harvey, ND, Christley, RM, Giragosian, K, Mead, R, Murray, JK, Samet, L, Upjohn, MM and Casey, RA 2022 Impact of changes in time left alone on separation-related behaviour in UK pet dogs. Animals 12(4). https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herron, ME, Lord, LK and Husseini, SE 2014 Effects of preadoption counseling on the prevention of separation anxiety in newly adopted shelter dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 9(1): 1321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2013.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiby, EF, Rooney, NJ and Bradshaw, JWS 2004 Dog training methods: Their use, effectiveness and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Universities Federation for Animal Welfare: Wheathampstead, UK.Google Scholar
Hoppe, N, Bininda-Emonds, ORP and Gansloßer, U 2017 Correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-like behavior in domestic dogs: First results from a questionnaire-based study. Veterinary Medicine Open Journal 2: 95131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houpt, KA, Goodwin, D, Uchida, Y, Baranyiová, E, Fatjó, J and Kakuma, Y 2007 Proceedings of a workshop to identify dog welfare issues in the US, Japan, Czech Republic, Spain and the UK. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106(4): 221233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, Y and Serpell, JA 2003 Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 223: 12931300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jagoe, A and Serpell, J 1996 Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47(1): 3142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)01008-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, JN, Simpson, BS, Overall, KL, Appleby, D, Pageat, P, Ross, C, Chaurand, JP, Heath, S, Beata, C, Weiss, AB, Muller, G, Paris, T, Bataille, BG, Parker, J, Petit, S and Wren, J 2000 Treatment of separation anxiety in dogs with clomipramine: results from a prospective, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter clinical trial. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 67(4): 255275. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00127-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsman, R, Owczarczak-Garstecka, S, Casey, R, Knowles, T, Tasker, S, Woodward, J, Da Costa, R and Murray, J 2020 sleep duration and behaviours: A descriptive analysis of a cohort of dogs up to 12 months of age. Animals 10(7). https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konok, V, Dóka, A and Miklósi, Á 2011 The behavior of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) during separation from and reunion with the owner: A questionnaire and an experimental study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135(4): 300308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konok, V, Kosztolányi, A, Rainer, W, Mutschler, B, Halsband, U and Miklósi, Á 2015 Influence of owners’ attachment style and personality on their dogs’ (Canis familiaris) separation-related disorder. PLoS One 10(2): e0118375. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landsberg, GM, Melese, P, Sherman, BL, Neilson, JC, Zimmerman, A and Clarke, TP 2008 Effectiveness of fluoxetine chewable tablets in the treatment of canine separation anxiety. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 3(1): 1219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2007.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linder, D and Mueller, M 2014 Pet obesity management: beyond nutrition. Veterinary Clinics: Small Animal Practice 44(4): 789806.Google ScholarPubMed
Lund, JD, Agger, JF and Vestergaard, KS 1996 Reported behaviour problems in pet dogs in Denmark: age distribution and influence of breed and gender. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 28(1): 3348. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-5877(96)01015-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lund, JD and Jørgensen, MC 1999 Behaviour patterns and time course of activity in dogs with separation problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 63(3): 219236. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00011-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mascha, EJ and Vetter, TR 2018 Significance, errors, power, and sample size: The blocking and tackling of statistics. Anesthesia & Analgesia 126(2).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrave, EA 1991 Diagnostic criteria for separation anxiety in the dog. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 21(2): 247255. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-5616(91)50030-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGreevy, PD and Masters, AM 2008 Risk factors for separation-related distress and feed-related aggression in dogs: Additional findings from a survey of Australian dog owners. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109(2–4): 320328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMillan, FD, Serpell, JA, Duffy, DL, Masaoud, E and Dohoo, IR 2013 Differences in behavioral characteristics between dogs obtained as puppies from pet stores and those obtained from noncommercial breeders. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 242(10): 13591363. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.242.10.1359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meerlo, P, Sgoifo, A and Suchecki, D 2008 Restricted and disrupted sleep: Effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Medicine Reviews 12(3): 197210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M, Brooks, J, Basse, C, Burman, O, Paul, E, Blackwell, E and Casey, R 2010b Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias. Current Biology 20(19): R839R840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP and Paul, ES 2010a An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277(1696): 28952904. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, DS 1997 Using learning theory in animal behavior therapy practice. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 27(3): 617635. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-5616(97)50057-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, JK, Kinsman, RH, Lord, MS, Da Costa, REP, Woodward, JL, Owczarczak-Garstecka, SC, Tasker, S, Knowles, TG and Casey, RA 2021Generation Pup’ – protocol for a longitudinal study of dog behaviour and health. BMC Veterinary Research 17(1): 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-020-02730-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogata, N 2016 Separation anxiety in dogs: What progress has been made in our understanding of the most common behavioral problems in dogs? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 16: 2835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.02.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palestrini, C, Minero, M, Cannas, S, Rossi, E and Frank, D 2010 Video analysis of dogs with separation-related behaviors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 124(1–2): 6167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.01.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pirrone, F, Pierantoni, L, Pastorino, GQ and Albertini, M 2016 Owner-reported aggressive behavior towards familiar people may be a more prominent occurrence in pet shop-traded dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 11: 1317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podberscek, L, Hsu, Y and Serpell, JA 1999 Evaluation of clomipramine as an adjunct to behavioural therapy in the treatment of separation-related problems in dogs. Veterinary Record 145(13): 365. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.145.13.365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rehn, T and Keeling, LJ 2011 The effect of time left alone at home on dog welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 129(2–4): 129135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.11.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RSPCA 2014 Crate Training Guide. RSPCA: Horsham, UK.Google Scholar
Salman, MD, Hutchison, J, Ruch-Gallie, R, Kogan, L, New, JC, Kass, PH and Scarlett, JM 2000 Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12 Shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3(2): 93106. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0302_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salonen, M, Sulkama, S, Mikkola, S, Puurunen, J, Hakanen, E, Tiira, K, Araujo, C and Lohi, H 2020 Prevalence, comorbidity, and breed differences in canine anxiety in 13,700 Finnish pet dogs. Scientific Reports 10(1): 2962. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-59837-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaglia, E, Cannas, S, Minero, M, Frank, D, Bassi, A and Palestrini, C 2013 Video analysis of adult dogs when left home alone. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 8(6): 412417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2013.04.065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S 2003 Separation anxiety syndrome in dogs and cats. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 222(11): 15261532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, JP and Fuller, JL 1965 Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. University of Chicago Press: Chicago: USA.Google Scholar
Sherman, BL and Mills, DS 2008 canine anxieties and phobias: An update on separation anxiety and noise aversions. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 38(5): 10811106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.04.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siracusa, C, Provoost, L and Reisner, IR 2017 Dog- and owner-related risk factors for consideration of euthanasia or rehoming before a referral behavioral consultation and for euthanizing or rehoming the dog after the consultation. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 22: 4656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.09.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soares, GM, Pereira, JT and Paixão, RL 2010 Exploratory study of separation anxiety syndrome in apartment dogs. Ciência Rural 40(3): 548553. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0103-84782010000300008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Social Science Statistics 2020 P-Value from Chi-Square Calculator. https://www.socscistatistics.com/pvalues/chidistribution.aspx (accessed 31 December 2020).Google Scholar
Storengen, LM, Boge, SCK, Strøm, SJ, Løberg, G and Lingaas, F 2014 A descriptive study of 215 dogs diagnosed with separation anxiety. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 159: 8289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takeuchi, Y, Ogata, N, Houpt, KA and Scarlett, JM 2001 Differences in background and outcome of three behavior problems of dogs. Applied Animal Behavioural Science 70(4): 297308. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0168-1591(00)00156-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teixeira, AR and Hall, NJ 2021 Effect of greeting and departure interactions on the development of increased separation-related behaviors in newly adopted adult dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 41: 2232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiira, K and Lohi, H 2015 Early life experiences and exercise associate with canine anxieties. PLoS One 10(11): e0141907. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141907CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiira, K, Sulkama, S and Lohi, H 2016 Prevalence, comorbidity, and behavioral variation in canine anxiety. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 16: 3644. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.06.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, Z 2018 Barriers to the adoption of humane dog training methods. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 25: 2834. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2018.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuber, DS, Miller, DD, Caris, KA, Halter, R, Linden, F and Hennessy, MB 1999 Dogs in animal shelters: Problems, suggestions, and needed expertise. Psychological Science 10(5): 379386. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Rooy, D, Thomson, PC, McGreevy, PD and Wade, CM 2018 Risk factors of separation-related behaviours in Australian retrievers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 209: 7177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaterlaws-Whiteside, H and Hartmann, A 2017 Improving puppy behavior using a new standardized socialization program. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 197: 5561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.08.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieira de Castro, AC, Barrett, J, de Sousa, L and Olsson, IAS 2019 Carrots versus sticks: The relationship between training methods and dog-owner attachment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 219: 104831. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104831CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, GA, Ward, L, Pink, C, Craigon, J and Millar, KM 2016Who’s been a good dog?” – Owner perceptions and motivations for treat giving. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 132: 1419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2016.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. List of ‘Generation Pup’ questionnaires used for the present study and the corresponding age that dogs reached when owners received each questionnaire

Figure 1

Table 2. Scenarios, question wording and potential responses from the ‘Generation Pup’ six-month questionnaire used to create the case definition for SRB occurrence in puppies at six months of age. Three scenarios were presented to the owners, as follows. Scenario 1: Owner relaxing with dog; Scenario 2: Dog left alone without human company (split over two questions); and Scenario 3: Owner not paying attention to dog.

Figure 2

Table 3. Categorisation of owner responses to questions regarding correcting their puppy for ‘bad’ behaviours in different scenarios (when owner not giving the dog attention, separated by a baby gate or door, relaxing at home with the owner, when owner returned to the household) from the ‘Generation Pup’ six-month questionnaire according to training categories as described in Blackwell et al. (2012)

Figure 3

Table 4. Demographics of dogs (n = 145) and owners included in the final multivariable model investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study ‘Generation Pup’. Categories without a natural scale are ordered from most to least common, followed by miscellaneous categories

Figure 4

Figure 1. The longest continuous duration for which dogs (n = 145) were left alone at 16 weeks and under in a study investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study, ‘Generation Pup’.

Figure 5

Figure 2. The longest continuous duration for which dogs (n = 145) were left alone in a study investigating risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study, ‘Generation Pup’. Dogs were left during the day only (orange), overnight only (grey) and during the day and overnight (blue).

Figure 6

Table 5. Number of dogs (n = 145) performing each canine SRB of interest at six months of age (only when left alone) using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study Generation Pup. The SRBs are arranged from most to least commonly reported.

Figure 7

Table 6. Multivariable logistic regression model for risk factors for canine SRBs at six months of age, using owner-reported data from the longitudinal study ‘Generation Pup’ (n = 145). Significance threshold: P < 0.050

Supplementary material: File

Dale et al. supplementary material

Dale et al. supplementary material
Download Dale et al. supplementary material(File)
File 391.6 KB