Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:09:53.271Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain measures which tell us about animal welfare

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

DM Broom*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Veterinary Medicine, University of Cambridge, Madingley Road, Cambridge CB3 OES, UK
AJ Zanella*
Affiliation:
Department of Animal Science, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824, USA
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Studies of the brain inform us about the cognitive abilities of animals and hence affect the extent to which animals of that species are respected. However, they can also tell us how an individual is likely to be perceiving, attending to, evaluating, coping with, enjoying, or disturbed by its environment, and so can give direct information about welfare. In studies of welfare, we are especially interested in how an individual feels. Since this depends upon high-level brain processing, we have to investigate brain function. Brain correlates of preferred social, sexual and parental situations include elevated oxytocin in the para-ventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Abnormal behaviour may have brain correlates, for example, high frequencies of stereotypy are associated with down-regulated μ and κ receptors and dopamine depletion in the frontal cortex. Such results help in evaluating the effects of treatment on welfare. Some brain changes, such as increased glucocorticoid receptors in the frontal lobes or increased activity in the amygdala, may be a sensitive indicator of perceived emergency. Active immunological defences lead to cytokine production in the brain, vagal nerve activity and sickness effects. Some aspects of brain function can be temporarily suppressed, for example, by opioids when there is severe pain, or permanently impaired, for example, in severely impoverished environments or during depression. Coping attempts or environmental impact can lead to injury to the brain, damage to hippocampal neurons, remodelling of dendrites in the hippocampus, or to other brain disorganisation. Brain measures can explain the nature and magnitude of many effects on welfare.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Altemus, M, Redwine, L S, Leong, Y-M, Frye, C A, Porges, S W and Carter, C S 2001 Responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in postpartum women. Psychosomatic Medicine 63: 814821CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackshaw, J K and McVeigh, J F 1984 The behaviour of sows and gilts, housed in stalls, tethers and groups. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 15: 8588Google Scholar
Broom, D M 1983 Stereotypies as animal welfare indicators. In: Smidt, D (ed) Indicators Relevant to Farm Animal Welfare pp 8187. Current Topics in Veterinary Medicine and Animal Science 23. Martinus Nijoff: The Hague, The NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M 1988 The scientific assessment of animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 20: 519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M 1998 Welfare, stress and the evolution of feelings. Advances in the Study of Behavior 27: 371403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M 2001 Coping, stress and welfare. In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 19. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M and Johnson, K G 1993 Stress and Animal Welfare. Kluwer: Dordrecht, The NetherlandsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M and Potter, M J 1984 Factors affecting the occurrence of stereotypies in stall-housed dry sows. In: Unshelm, J, van Putten, G and Zeeb, K (eds) Proceedings of the International Congress on Applied Ethology in Farm Animals pp 229231. KTBL: Darmstadt, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M, Mendl, M T and Zanella, A J 1995 A comparison of the welfare of sows in different housing conditions. Animal Science 61: 369385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, C S 2001 Is there a neurobiology of good welfare? In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 1130. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Carter, C S and Altemus, M 1997 Integrative functions of lactational hormones in social behaviour and stress management. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 807: 164174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronin, G M, Wiepkema, P R and van, Ree JM 1985 Endogenous opioids are involved in abnormal stereotyped behaviours of tethered sows. Neuropeptides 6: 527530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dantzer, R 2001 Can we understand the brain and coping without considering the immune system? In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 101110. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, M 1993 Through Our Eyes Only? W H Freeman: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
de Kloet, E R 1991 Brain corticosteroid receptor balance and homeostatic control. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 12: 95164Google Scholar
Fraser, D, Weary, D M, Pajor, E A and Milligan, B N 1997 A scientific conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Animal Welfare 6: 174186Google Scholar
Hagen, K and Broom, D M 2003 Cattle discriminate between familiar herd members in a learning experiment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82: 1328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, M 2001 How are stress and depression inter-related? In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 271288. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Kendrick, K M, Atkins, K, Hinton, M R, Borad, K D, Fabre-Nys, C and Keverne, B 1995 Facial and vocal discrimination in sheep. Animal Behaviour 49: 16651676CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendrick, K M and Baldwin, B A 1987 Cells in the temporal cortex of sheep can respond preferentially to the sight of faces. Science 236: 448450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendrick, K M, da Costa, A P, Leigh, A E, Hinton, M R and Peirce, J W 2001 Sheep don't forget a face. Nature 414: 165166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laughlin, K and Zanella, A J Modification of the water-maze procedure to examine cognitive processes in nursing and newly weaned pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science: in press.Google Scholar
McBride, S D and Hemmings, A 2001 Nucleus accumbens DI dopamine receptor numbers are significantly higher in horses performing stereotypic behaviour. Proceedings of the Association for Veterinary Teachers and Research Workers Conference, Scarborough 9-12 April 2001.Google Scholar
McEwen, B S 2001 Protecting and damaging effects of stress mediators: lessons learned from the immune system and brain. In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 229248. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Zanella, A J and Broom, D M 1992 Physiological and reproductive correlates of behavioural strategies in female domestic pigs. Animal Behaviour 44: 11071121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, R J 2001 Is there a major stress system in the brain? In: Broom, D M (ed) Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Man pp 111122. Dahlem University Press: Berlin, GermanyGoogle Scholar
Olds, J and Milner, P 1954 Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of the septal area of the rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 47: 419427CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J 1998 Affective Neuroscience. Oxford University Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Siegford, J, Nobis, W, Poletto, R, Ren, X, Coussens, P and Zanella, A J 2003 Development and validation of a porcine brain cDNA library and microarray resource using the hippocampus of early-weaned pigs. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 29: 758.19Google Scholar
Porges, S W 1998 Love: an emergent property of the mammalian autonomic nervous system. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23: 837861CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redwine, L S, Altemus, M, Leong, Y-M and Carter, C S 2001 Lymphocyte responses to stress in post partum women: relationship to vagal tone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2b: 241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reul, J M and de Kloet, E R 1985 Two receptor systems for corticosterone in rat brain: microdistribution and differential occupation. Endocrinology 117: 25052511CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E T 1999 The Brain and Emotion. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R 1992 Stress, the Aging Brain and the Mechanisms of Neuron Death. MIT Press: Cambridge, Massachusetts, USAGoogle Scholar
Spruijt, B M, van den Bas, R and Pijlman, F T A 2001 A concept of welfare based on reward evaluating mechanisms in the brain: anticipating behaviour as an indicator of the state of reward systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 72: 145171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sudheimer, K D, Davis, K K, Bixby, J C, Knox, D and Symonds, L L 2001 An emotion induction paradigm for neuroimaging: International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 27.Google Scholar
Worobec, E K, Duncan, I J H and Widowski, T M 1999 The effects of weaning at 7, 14 and 28 days on piglet behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science b2: 173182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanella, A J, Broom, D M, Hunter, J C and Mendl, M T 1996 Brain opioid receptors in relation to stereotypies, inactivity and housing in sows. Physiology and Behavior 59: 769775CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanella, A J, Brunner, P, Unshelm, J, Mendl, M T and Broom, D M 1998 The relationship between housing and social rank on cortisol, ß-endorphin and dynorphin (1-13) secretion in sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 59: 110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanella, A J and Mendl, M 2000 Behavioural responses to maternal deprivation stress in domestic animals are mediated by glucocorticoid action in hippocampal cells. In: Ramos, E, Pinheiro Machado, L C and Hötzel, M J (eds) Proceedings of the 34th International Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology p 35 (Abstract). Laboratory of Applied Ethology UFSC: Florianopolis, BrazilGoogle Scholar