Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T22:32:20.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Temperament Development to 30 Months of Age in Discordant Twin Pairs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

M. L. Riese*
Affiliation:
Louisville Twin Study, University of Louisville School of Medicine, Louisville, Kentucky
*
Louisville Twin Study, Child Development Unit, Health Sciences Center, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky 40292, U.S.A.

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Twins within pairs often have different weights at birth. A difference of 15% or greater is defined as discordance for weight and is considered to place one or both infants at risk. Temperament differences had been found in the neonatal period for full-term discordant cotwins, but not for preterm discordant cotwins, suggesting that continued gestation for discordant twins was a risk variable for early behavior. 30 pairs of fullterm and 17 pairs of preterm discordant pairs were followed at 6, 9, 12, 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Group differences were observed for the longitudinal maintenance of cotwin discordance in physical measures, with preterm cotwins becoming more like each other. In laboratory assessments, temperament differences no longer were observed between the larger and smaller cotwins. Questionnaires indicated that mothers generally did not differentiate between their larger and smaller cotwin children in temperament ratings, except for ratings of mood for the fullterm pairs. Thus, emotionality was the only temperament dimension that differentiated between the fullterm discordant twins both in the neonatal period and at later ages. In the main, it was concluded that the fullterm discordant twins overcame the adverse in-utero influences on early behavioral development.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Society for Twin Studies 1996

References

REFERENCES

1. Babson, SG, Phillips, DS (1973): Growth and development of twins dissimilar in size at birth. N. Engl J Med 289:937940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Carey, WB, McDevitt, SC (1978): Stability and change in individual temperament diagnosis from infancy to early childhood. American Academy of Child Psychiatry 17:331337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Field, T, Waiden, T, Widmayer, S, et al (1982): The early development of preterm discordant twin pairs: bigger is not always better. In Lipsitt, LP, Field, TM (eds.): “Infant Behavior and Development: Perinatal Risk and Newborn Behavior”. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 153163.Google Scholar
4. Fujikura, T, Froehlich, LA (1974): Mental and motor development in monozygotic cotwins with dissimilar birthweights. Pediatrics 53:884889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5. Fullard, W, McDevitt, SG, Carey, WB (1984): Assessing temperament in one- to three-year-old children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 9:205217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Matheny, AP Jr, Brown, AM (1971): The behavior of twins: effects of birthweight and birth sequence. Child Dev 42:251257.Google Scholar
7. Matheny, AP Jr, Riese, ML, Wilson, RS (1985): Rudiments of infant temperament: newborn to nine months. Developmental Psychology 21:486494.Google Scholar
8. Patterson, RM, Wood, RC (1990): What is twin birthweight discordance? Am J Perinatol 7:217219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Riese, ML (1983): Behavioral patterns in full-term and preterm twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae: Twin Research 32:209220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Riese, ML (1987a): Longitudinal assessment of temperament from birth to 2 years: a comparison of full-term and preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development 10:347363.Google Scholar
11. Riese, ML (1987b): Temperament stability between the neonatal period and 24 months. Developmental Psychology 23:216222.Google Scholar
12. Riese, ML (1994a): Neonatal temperament in fullterm twin pairs discordant for birth weight. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: 15: 343347.Google Scholar
13. Riese, ML (1994b): Discordant twin pairs: The relation between gestational age and neonatal temperament differences in cotwins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae: Twin Research 43:165173.Google Scholar
14. Rothbart, MK, Derryberry, D (1981): “Development of individual differences in temperament”. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L. (eds.): Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp 3786): Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
15. Wilson, RS, Matheny, AP Jr (1983): Assessment of temperament in infant twins. Developmental Psychology 19:172183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar