Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:31:44.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

I’m a Survivor: Political Dynamics in Bureaucratic Elites’ Partisan Identification

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2024

BENNY GEYS*
Affiliation:
BI Norwegian Business School, Norway
PER LÆGREID*
Affiliation:
University of Bergen, Norway
ZUZANA MURDOCH*
Affiliation:
University of Bergen, Norway
SUSAN WEBB YACKEE*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
*
Benny Geys, Professor, Department of Economics, BI Norwegian Business School (Campus Bergen), Norway, [email protected].
Per Lægreid, Professor Emeritus, Department of Government, University of Bergen, Norway, [email protected].
Zuzana Murdoch, Professor, Department of Government, University of Bergen, Norway, [email protected].
Corresponding author: Susan Webb Yackee, Professor, La Follette School of Public Affairs, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States, [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article challenges the common assumption that the partisan identification of bureaucratic elites is fixed over time. Building on principal-agent and organization theory, we hypothesize that bureaucratic elites may respond to political turnover by adjusting their partisan identification toward that of their (new) elected principals. We test this prediction using data from the American State Administrators Project (ASAP) over the 1964–2008 period, which allows us to study the same US agency leaders (N=951 individuals) before and after partisan shifts in their agency’s elected principals. We find significant evidence that agency leaders remaining in office following a shift in the party in power on average reorient their partisan identity in response to such turnover events. These adjustments are stronger for agency leaders directly appointed by, or in more frequent contact with, their elected principals. Our results suggest a malleability of partisanship seldom attributed to bureaucratic elites in public and academic discourse.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NC
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

INTRODUCTION

The (in)stability of individuals’ partisan identification is among the most prominent and persistent debates within the study of American politics (Key Reference Key1966; Kollman and Jackson Reference Kollman and Jackson2021; Lazarsfeld, Berelson, and Gaudet Reference Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet1944; MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989). Going back to the pioneering work of Campbell et al. (Reference Campbell, Converse, Miller and Stokes1960), some scholars treat partisan identification as a near-constant social identity that “stabilizes partisan preferences over a lifetime” (Tucker, Montgomery, and Smith Reference Tucker, Montgomery and Smith2019, 310). Others challenge this presumption of (near-)stability and theorize that “claimed identities can change (…) over time, as people have new experiences and encounter new contexts” (Egan Reference Egan2020, 701). To date, this debate concentrates on (un)stable partisanship within the mass public. Yet, understanding the (non-)adaptive nature of partisanship among bureaucratic elites also holds important implications for our understanding of politics and public policy (Clinton and Lewis Reference Clinton and Lewis2008; Kappe and Schuster Reference Kappe and Schuster2022; Nixon Reference Nixon2004; Reference Nixon2023; Richardson, Clinton, and Lewis Reference Richardson, Clinton and Lewis2018). Public bureaucracies are, after all, deeply involved in policy formulation, development, and implementation (Aberbach, Putnam, and Rockman Reference Aberbach, Putnam and Rockman1981; Key Reference Key and White1942; Murdoch, Connolly, and Kassim Reference Murdoch, Connolly and Kassim2018; Waldo Reference Waldo1952). As we note below, most of the bureaucratic politics literature tacitly assumes that bureaucratic elites do not change their party identification over time. This article is, to the best of our knowledge, the first to challenge this assumption.

More specifically, we investigate whether and how bureaucratic elites respond to a political turnover event, which we define as a change in the party holding power in the bureaucrat’s political environment. Previous work shows that political turnover can have substantial effects on staff turnover in public organizations (Aberbach, Putnam, and Rockman Reference Aberbach, Putnam and Rockman1981; Bach and Veit Reference Bach and Veit2018; Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021; Christensen, Klemmensen, and Ostrup Reference Christensen, Klemmensen and Ostrup2014; Colonnelli, Prem, and Teso Reference Colonnelli, Prem and Teso2020; Mayntz and Derlien Reference Mayntz and Derlien1989; Spenkuch, Teso, and Xu Reference Spenkuch, Teso and Xu2023). Although staff turnover is not the only possible response to political turnover, much less attention has been awarded to other coping strategies available to public employees (Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021; Golden Reference Golden2000; Scholz Reference Scholz1986). This article addresses this persistent gap by investigating a previously unexplored coping strategy, which we refer to as a “malleability of partisanship”: that is, the ability to reorient one’s partisan identification after a political turnover event.

We hypothesize that bureaucratic elites may respond to political turnover by modifying their partisanship toward that of their new political principals through a process of “adaptive adjustment” (Bendor and Moe Reference Bendor and Moe1985; March and Olsen Reference March and Olsen1975; Moe Reference Moe1985; Yackee Reference Yackee2023). We theorize several mechanisms that can bring about this response and furthermore maintain that the strength of this adaptive process differs depending on how closely bureaucratic elites’ world is intertwined with that of politicians. Building on insights from organization theory and psychology, two moderating factors are included as a central part of our theorizing: the nature of bureaucratic elites’ appointment process (Bach and Veit Reference Bach and Veit2018; Ban, Park, and You Reference Ban, Park and You2023; Mayntz and Derlien Reference Mayntz and Derlien1989) and contact patterns between bureaucratic elites and politicians (Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021; Burkhardt Reference Burkhardt1994; Christensen and Ostrup Reference Christensen and Ostrup2018; Egeberg and Trondal Reference Egeberg and Trondal2011; Kiecolt Reference Kiecolt1988).

Our empirical analysis relies on data from the American State Administrators Project (ASAP), which is a survey of all state agency leaders across all US states fielded ten times over the 1964–2008 period (Yackee and Yackee Reference Yackee and Yackee2021). This dataset holds four benefits for our analysis. First, it provides data from the perspective of bureaucratic elites (Kertzer and Renshon Reference Kertzer and Renshon2022). Second, it contains information about US state agency leaders’ partisanship (our dependent variable), as well as their appointment process and contact patterns (our moderators). The ASAP also allows for the integration of other data based on the ASAP survey year and US state where a respondent is employed, including political turnover events (our main independent variable). Third, the 45-year timeframe offers substantial variation across time and space in the party in power at the US state level. Since political turnover events derive from state-wide elections and are exogenous to the bureaucratic elites under analysis, we can exploit them to identify our main relationship of interest (i.e., political dynamics in bureaucratic elites’ partisanship). Finally, although each ASAP wave is an independent cross-sectional study, we implement an innovative methodology to extract a panel dataset that allows following the same state agency leaders (951 individuals) before and after partisan shifts in their agency’s elected principals (i.e., either the governor or state legislature). This longitudinal dimension at the individual level enables the estimation of first-difference regression models that accommodate unobserved individual-level heterogeneity (Wooldridge Reference Wooldridge2010). Thus, we leverage a (quasi-)natural experimental design to derive stronger causal inferences than is feasible via cross-sectional analyses (Baekgaard, Herd, and Moynihan Reference Baekgaard, Herd and Moynihan2023; Foos and Bischof Reference Foos and Bischof2022).

Our findings indicate that US state agency leaders who remain in office after a change in the party in power on average display a tendency to reorient their partisan leaning toward their new political principals. Importantly, we do not find a similar response to partisan turnover in the US presidency or in the balance of partisan support among state-level voters. This strengthens our inference that US state agency leaders adjust their partisan leaning toward that of their direct political principals, rather than toward general ideological shifts in the country or state. Furthermore, in line with theoretical expectations, the observed adaptive adjustment is more prominent among those appointed by, or in more frequent contact with, their elected principals. Overall, our findings show that bureaucratic elites are sensitive to changes in political power particularly when their position is closely intertwined with the world of politicians.

As mentioned, our results are conditional on retaining one’s position after a political turnover event (henceforth designated as “survivors”). Yet, such survivors are a pivotal group to study as they constitute the majority of those in state agency leader positions. For instance, 58% of ASAP respondents “survived” at least one partisan shift in the governor’s office. Thus, the modal state agency leader is a survivor. Given this, a critical insight from our analysis is that, on average, “surviving” state-level bureaucratic elites meaningfully adjust their partisanship in line with changing political dynamics in their state. Whether this is a good thing arguably depends on one’s normative standpoint. Some may desire bureaucratic elites to offer advice untainted by partisan pressures or inhibitions, while others may appreciate a degree of responsiveness or “serial loyalty” toward elected officials. Either way, our findings require that we revisit the persistent characterization of bureaucratic elites as notable mainly for their “disconnection and fossilization” (Lodge and Hood Reference Lodge and Hood2003, 135; Moynihan and Ingraham Reference Moynihan and Ingraham2010; Rainey and Steinbauer Reference Rainey and Steinbauer1999).

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES

In this section, we first build on the adaptive model of bureaucratic politics developed by Bendor and Moe (Reference Bendor and Moe1985) and Moe (Reference Moe1985) to argue that bureaucratic elites may—for several reasons—adjust their partisan identification following political turnover. Then, we turn to organization theory and organizational psychology to derive two scope conditions under which such adaptive adjustments are particularly (un)likely to arise.

Bendor and Moe (Reference Bendor and Moe1985) set out a general framework to study the interdependent decisions of political and administrative actors. This framework takes from principal-agent models that the relationship between politicians and administrators is hierarchical and that both sets of actors choose among available alternatives based on a self-interested calculation of expected costs and benefits (Moe Reference Moe1985). Yet, crucially, all participants in the system are assumed to be boundedly rational (Simon Reference Simon1957). They act on incomplete information and are not aware of all alternatives and their consequences. The latter assumption allows for dynamic processes whereby actors “adapt in simple ways to their environments, moving in directions that appear to promise them greater utility” (Bendor and Moe Reference Bendor and Moe1985, 756). This becomes reflected in agents’ “adaptive adjustment” to signals arriving from the broader (political) environment (Bendor and Moe Reference Bendor and Moe1985; Moe Reference Moe1985, 1094; Yackee Reference Yackee2023). In a similar spirit, March and Olsen (Reference March and Olsen1975, 147) build on a “view of limited rationality” to argue that “individuals in organizations modify their understanding in a way that is intendedly adaptive.”

Applying this general framework to our setting, we maintain that any adaptive adjustment of bureaucratic elites’ partisan leaning following a change in the party in power can come about for (at least) three reasons. First, politicians are often argued to prefer bureaucrats who better match their own ideological stance and policy preferences (the ally principle; Bendor, Glazer, and Hammond Reference Bendor, Glazer and Hammond2001; Palus and Yacke Reference Palus and Yackee2016; Reference Palus and Yackee2022). The reason is that such ideological alignment helps mitigate problems and inefficiencies related to task delegation (Bendor, Glazer, and Hammond Reference Bendor, Glazer and Hammond2001; Fiva et al. Reference Fiva, Geys, Heggedal and Sørensen2021; Spenkuch, Teso, and Xu Reference Spenkuch, Teso and Xu2023; Toral Reference Toral2024). When politicians surround themselves with allies via politically motivated hiring, firing, promotion, and remuneration decisions (Bach and Veit Reference Bach and Veit2018; Colonnelli, Prem, and Teso Reference Colonnelli, Prem and Teso2020; Fiva et al. Reference Fiva, Geys, Heggedal and Sørensen2021; Mayntz and Derlien Reference Mayntz and Derlien1989), any perceived lack of alignment can prove costly for bureaucratic elites. This gives them an instrumental motivation to “move far in accommodating political executive demand” (Christensen, Klemmensen, and Ostrup Reference Christensen, Klemmensen and Ostrup2014, 215). In our setting, we argue that this could include reorienting their partisan leaning following a change in the party in power.

Second, recent research in organizational psychology argues that leaders can produce both short- and long-term changes in subordinates by influencing—through their rhetoric, actions, and characteristics—the relative salience of different aspects within subordinates’ claimed identities (Epitropaki, Kark, and Mainemelis Reference Epitropaki, Kark, Mainemelis and Lord2017; Geys et al. Reference Geys, Connolly, Kassim and Murdoch2020). The core idea is that the “refinement of one’s identity is an ongoing and central aspect of organizational membership that depends, in part, on the relationship with one’s supervisor” (Lord, Gatti, and Chui Reference Lord, Gatti and Chui2016, 125). As illustrated by Geys et al. (Reference Geys, Connolly, Kassim and Murdoch2020, 555), leadership successions can then “invoke observable shifts in individuals’ attitudes” by affecting subordinates’ sense-making within organizations (Weick Reference Weick1995). In a similar vein, the arrival of new political principals with a different partisan orientation can be expected to trigger an adaptive adjustment in bureaucratic elites’ partisan identification.

Finally, most humans share a basic need of belonging (Baumeister and Leary Reference Baumeister and Leary1995; Van Ryzin Reference Van Ryzin2021). This makes them keen to conform to the opinions of significant others—such as elected principals in the case of bureaucratic elites—“for the purpose of altering (shaping) positively the evaluations or attributions of relevant others” (Liden and Mitchell Reference Liden and Mitchell1988, 572). A closely related psychological argument relates to human’s need to limit feelings of “cognitive dissonance” (Festinger Reference Festinger1957) when, for instance, discord arises between the partisan leaning of bureaucratic elites and the arrival of new political principals. Since it is impossible for bureaucratic elites to adjust the partisan leaning of a new party in power, adjusting their own partisan identification is possible to mitigate any sense of dissonance (Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen Reference Acharya, Blackwell and Sen2018; Homola, Pereira, and Tavits Reference Homola, Pereira and Tavits2020).

Common to all three lines of argument is that political turnover can prompt shifts in bureaucratic elites’ partisan identification toward that of their new elected principals. The ensuing “politicized identity shifting” (Egan Reference Egan2020, 714) reflects an ideological pragmatism that constitutes a “fundamental attribute of an adaptive decision-maker” (Bendor and Moe Reference Bendor and Moe1985, 764; March and Olsen Reference March and Olsen1975; Scholz Reference Scholz1986). This leads to the first hypothesis:

H1: Bureaucratic elites adjust their partisan identification in the same direction as the shift in partisanship of their elected political principals.

Naturally, the response of bureaucratic elites to political leadership succession may differ depending on how closely their world is intertwined with that of politicians. Building on organization theory and organizational psychology, we assert that appointment processes and contact patterns constitute two key moderating factors.

First, the organization theory approach to public administration emphasizes that organizational features of public bureaucracies shape civil servants’ role perceptions and opinions (Christensen, Lægreid, and Røvik Reference Christensen, Lægreid and Arne Røvik2021; Egeberg Reference Egeberg1999; March and Olsen Reference March and Olsen1984). One of these features relates to staff members’ appointment procedures. Appointments requiring explicit political consent or being in the purview of politicians—as in the so-called spoils system (Ferguson Reference Ferguson, Gray, Hanson and Kousser2018)—are more likely to guide bureaucratic elites’ role perceptions and opinions toward those of their political principals compared to recruitment via, for instance, a civil service or merit-based appointment (Bach and Veit Reference Bach and Veit2018; Ban, Park, and You Reference Ban, Park and You2023; Mayntz and Derlien Reference Mayntz and Derlien1989). The reason is that the influence of politicians over appointment procedures strengthens the instrumental motivation of pragmatic bureaucratic elites to adapt to their elected principals. As a result, we hypothesize that bureaucratic elites appointed by their political principals show a higher level of adaptive adjustment to political turnover:

H2: Adaptive adjustments in bureaucratic elites’ partisan identification are stronger when their appointment requires explicit political approval.

Second, it is well known from research in social and organizational psychology that individuals’ attitudes are formed and developed through interactions with significant others (Burkhardt Reference Burkhardt1994; Kiecolt Reference Kiecolt1988). Organization theory scholarship likewise holds that direct and personal contacts enabled via organizational structures form individuals’ attitudes and behavior by conveying information, expectations, and feedback (Christensen and Lægreid Reference Christensen and Lægreid2003; Egeberg and Trondal Reference Egeberg and Trondal2011). Building on this line of argument, Christensen and Ostrup (Reference Christensen and Ostrup2018) highlight that close politician–bureaucratic interactions induce higher levels of political responsiveness among public employees. Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis (Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021, 462) make a similar argument by stating that adjustments by public employees toward (the preferences of) political principals are stronger among those “most likely to interact with political appointees.” In our setting, this would imply that:

H3: Adaptive adjustments in bureaucratic elites’ partisan identification are stronger when they have more frequent contact with their political principals.

INSTITUTIONAL SETTING AND DATA

Institutional Setting

The 50 US state governments hold legislative, executive, and judicial authority within the state’s boundaries. Every state has a bicameral legislative system with elected lower and upper chambers (except the unicameral Nebraska), and the executive branch of government is headed by a directly elected governor. Elections for all these political bodies are partisan in nature (except the legislative elections in Nebraska) and are usually dominated by the Republican and Democratic parties. US states have wide-ranging authority in, among others, education, healthcare, economic development, infrastructure, emergency management, security, and environmental policy. In each of these areas, state-level public agencies play a key role in the determination and implementation of public policy.

Our empirical analysis focuses on the top administrative positions in US state-level public agencies. Considerable variation in appointment procedures for such positions exists across as well as within US states. There are five main mechanisms. First, some leaders—such as lieutenant governors, secretaries of state, and state treasurers—are popularly elected in some states but not in others (Folke, Hirano, and Snyder Reference Folke, Hirano and Snyder2011). Second, the governor directly appoints numerous agency leaders: a power that is considered more significant when the legislature is not required to consent to the appointment (Ferguson Reference Ferguson, Gray, Hanson and Kousser2018). This appointment style comes the closest to the so-called spoils system whereby the governor can use political appointments to the top positions across the bureaucracy to reward loyalty.

Third, some agency leaders are appointed by other department heads—such as a state’s Secretary of the Department of Transportation appointing a state’s Highway Administrator. In these cases, as Bowling (Reference Bowling, Gray, Hanson and Kousser2018) states, the department head is likely to be a political appointee of the governor, which makes these subordinate agency leaders one step removed from the governor’s influence. Fourth, some states have a considerable number of independent boards or commissions whose leaders are sometimes chosen by the governor and sometimes not (Folke, Hirano, and Snyder Reference Folke, Hirano and Snyder2011). Fifth, a number of agency leaders are appointed via a merit-based system (Bowling Reference Bowling, Gray, Hanson and Kousser2018). Table 1 provides an overview of the relative prevalence of these appointment methods in our data, which we exploit to operationalize the level of political influence over appointment processes when testing hypothesis H2.

Table 1. Appointment Procedures for US State Agency Leaders

Note: The table shows the share of respondents in the dataset(s) who obtained their agency leadership position via a given appointment procedure. The ASAP dataset includes 11,592 responses from 10,399 unique respondents, while our panel dataset contains 2,081 responses from 951 unique respondents.

Data

Our dataset combines two main data sources. The first is the ASAP dataset, which brings together surveys conducted among US state agency leaders in 1964, 1968, 1974, 1978, 1984, 1988, 1994, 1998, 2004, and 2008. Each wave includes respondents from all 50 American states and covers agencies across all functional areas of government (Brudney and Wright Reference Brudney and Wright2002; Yackee and Yackee Reference Yackee and Yackee2021). Crucially, the ASAP surveys include detailed individual background characteristics, including respondents’ partisan self-identification and their contacts with political elites. Survey respondents were guaranteed full confidentiality. While this rules out any attempt to personally identify the respondents (see also below), it considerably strengthens the integrity and reliability of the dataset (Kertzer and Renshon Reference Kertzer and Renshon2022). Respondent confidentiality is indeed well known to mitigate incentives to obfuscate or mischaracterize attitudes and self-reported behavior—even in response to highly sensitive questions (Cohen and Cassell Reference Cohen and Cassell2023; Ong and Weiss Reference Ong and Weiss2000; Robertson et al. Reference Robertson, Tran, Lewark and Epstein2018).

Our second data source yields information on the political party in power in US states over the period of 1964–2008. We collected information on the partisan affiliation of the state governor (Democrat, Republican, or other), as well as the seat distribution in the state’s lower and upper legislative chambers.Footnote 1 The latter allows us to define variables indicating whether there is a Democratic or Republican majority in the lower and upper chambers, as well as the partisanship of the state legislature as a whole (i.e., Democratic majority, Republican majority, or a split branch legislature). Both data sources are integrated based on the ASAP survey year and US state where a respondent is employed.

Each ASAP survey constitutes an independent cross-sectional study, and there are no individual identifiers to link respondents across waves (Yackee and Yackee Reference Yackee and Yackee2021). To innovate with these repeated cross-sectional data, we employ an empirical strategy pioneered by Geys (Reference Geys2023) and Murdoch et al. (Reference Murdoch, Connolly, Geys and Kassim2019). It allows us to link ASAP respondents across two consecutive survey waves when they have the same sex, race, US state of birth, US state of employment, and highest education level, as well as reporting the same education and occupation for both parents. We additionally verify that these respondents’ age and length of employment increase with the correct number of years between consecutive waves (i.e., four or six years) and that their religion, career history, training and work experience, and agency characteristics are consistent across both survey waves (whenever available). Although two ASAP respondents may share several of these background characteristics, the probability of sharing all of them simultaneously is vanishingly small.

We extensively validate our data to ensure that we are correctly retrieving panel respondents. We first do so by estimating the number of false positives (i.e., incorrectly designating two distinct respondents as the same “individual”) and false negatives (i.e., failing to link the same individual across survey waves) likely to arise via a simulation exercise using a setting with realistic data characteristics. We thereby simulate 1,000 datasets that replicate the main characteristics of the ASAP dataset in terms of the number, detail, and intercorrelation of included variables (full procedural details in Supplementary Material B), and we apply Geys’ (Reference Geys2023) methodology to these computer-generated datasets. Our simulations show that the high level of detail available in the ASAP surveys allows us to correctly identify more than 99% of repeat respondents (i.e., there are very few false negatives). We can also expect to match respondents inaccurately very rarely across surveys (i.e., fewer than 1% false positives) (see Supplementary Figure B.1). Even so, all potential matches uncovered across consecutive ASAP surveys were verified by two independent researchers and retained in the final sample only when both agreed. As a result, we can be confident that the ASAP respondents matched across two consecutive survey waves are the same individual.Footnote 2

The procedure reveals 951 unique respondents that appear in more than one ASAP survey (9.1% of the 10,399 unique respondents in the ASAP database). Their 2,081 survey responses form the dataset for our analysis, and Supplementary Table A.1 presents their descriptive statistics.Footnote 3 The final column in this table presents the p-value of formal balancing tests evaluating how our sample relates to the set of respondents appearing only once in the ASAP dataset. The results indicate mostly small differences, even though many are statistically significant at conventional levels. This suggests that entry into our panel dataset is not a random event, limiting the potential to generalize any findings to the general population of state agency leaders. Nonetheless, high internal validity—which is critical for hypothesis-testing—can still be achieved when a (quasi-)natural experiment divides the respondent sample into treated and untreated groups in a random manner (Geys et al. Reference Geys, Connolly, Kassim and Murdoch2020; Jilke, Van de Walle, and Kim Reference Jilke, Van de Walle and Kim2016). In our setting, this requires that there should exist no significant differences between the state agency leaders who are “treated” and “untreated” with partisan turnover of their elected principals. Supplementary Table A.2 illustrates this is the case—with the sole exception of respondents’ state of employment. Yet, this simply reflects that certain US states are more likely to witness political turnover (i.e., swing states) relative to others (i.e., safe states), and we return to it below.

EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

Model Specification

To evaluate whether, when, and how bureaucratic elites adjust their partisan leaning following a partisan change in their elected principals, we estimate the following first-difference regression model (with subscript i for individuals):

(1) $$ \Delta {PartyID}_i=\alpha +{\beta}_1\Delta {GovParty}_i+{\varepsilon}_i $$

Our dependent variable $ \Delta {PartyID}_i $ —captures the change in respondents’ self-reported partisan leaning from one survey wave to the next. It is based on the question: “Generally speaking, do you consider yourself to be a Democrat, a Republican, or an Independent? If independent, which party are you closer to?”Footnote 4 We use this question to derive two closely related variables. Shift Party ID exploits only the three main answer categories (coded 1 for Democrat, 2 for Independent, and 3 for Republican) and therefore ranges from -2 (move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 2 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). Shift Party ID (detailed) considers the partisan leaning of Independents using a five-point scale with 1 for Democrat, 2 for Independent leaning Democrat, 3 for Independent, 4 for Independent leaning Republican, and 5 for Republican. It therefore ranges from -4 (move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 4 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). In both cases, respondents who do not change their partisan leaning receive value 0. Given the ordinal character of these variables, we estimate Equation 1 using ordered logistic regressions.

Supplementary Figure A.1 illustrates the magnitude of individual-level shifts in self-reported partisan leaning over time. In line with Nixon’s (Reference Nixon2023) recent work on the dynamics of bureaucratic preferences, we find that stability is the norm. Most respondents (83.6%) do not change between survey waves when looking only at the three main answer options. Yet, 14.5% change from either Democrat or Republican to Independent, or from Independent to Democrat or Republican. The remaining 1.9% shifts between the two major parties (distributed equally across the 45-year period). When including more fine-grained information about Independents, 69.6% claim the same partisan identification in both survey waves. In this case, a considerable number of shifts occur within the Independent category as well as in both directions between Democrat/Republican and Independent (28.5%).Footnote 5

The main independent variable $ \Delta {GovParty}_i $ —captures a change in the party in power from one survey wave to the next. We operationalize this in two ways. First, we look at the partisan affiliation of the state governor. As we differentiate between Democratic (coded 1), Independent (coded 2), and Republican (coded 3) governors, this variable ranges from -2 to 2 in the same way as Shift Party ID. Footnote 6 Second, an alternative operationalization looks at changes in the distribution of power within the state legislature (i.e., lower and upper chambers). Across the 45-year period of our analysis, every US state changes the partisan affiliation of its governor and state legislature at least once. Since such changes derive from state-wide elections, they are exogenous to the bureaucratic elites under analysis. This allows us to identify how the same US agency leaders respond to partisan turnover of their elected principals.Footnote 7

For our evaluation of H2, we exploit the diversity of appointment procedures for agency leaders within and across US states (see Table 1). We expect $ {\beta}_1 $ to be larger when the governor has direct influence over the appointment of a state agency leader (i.e., top three rows of Table 1) compared to the situation where appointments do not require gubernatorial consent (i.e., bottom four rows of Table 1). For H3, we exploit information from the ASAP surveys about the (self-reported) contact pattern of state agency leaders. The question is: “On average, how often do you personally have phone or face-to-face contacts with the following persons during the course of carrying out your official duties?” and respondents answer on a five-point scale (1 daily; 2 weekly; 3 monthly; 4 less than monthly; 5 never). Following Geuijen et al. (Reference Geuijen, ‘t Hart, Princen and Yesilkagit2008) and Vantaggiato et al. (Reference Vantaggiato, Murdoch, Kassim, Geys and Connolly2024), we compare those reporting at least monthly contacts with the governor and governor’s staff versus those reporting less than monthly contacts. We do the same for contacts with state legislators and their staff. We measure this within the second survey observation to ensure the contact pattern relates to the elected principal after any shift in power, and we expect $ {\beta}_1 $ to be larger for those with more frequent contacts. While we split the sample using these moderating variables in our main analysis (thus estimating Equation 1 separately using the respective subsamples), we show that our findings are qualitatively similar when using encompassing models with fully specified interaction terms.

Causal Identification

Using a (quasi-)experimental design based on exogenous election-induced political turnover allows us to take advantage of the longitudinal dimension of the ASAP data to explore dynamic relationships at the individual level. The first-difference regression models also mitigate concerns regarding omitted variable bias by controlling for all time-invariant aspects of respondents—such as innate personality characteristics, education level, or US states of birth and employment (Wooldridge Reference Wooldridge2010).

That said, causal interpretation of $ {\beta}_1 $ requires that agency leaders exposed and not exposed to political turnover are not developing differently in terms of partisan identification prior to a turnover “treatment” (the parallel pre-trends assumption). We assess this in two ways. First, although we usually only observe the same individual for two consecutive ASAP waves (85.5% of our sample), a small subset can be followed for three or more waves (N = 138 individuals). Hence, we can assess how these respondents’ partisan identification develops prior to a shift in their agency’s political principals. Specifically, we extend Equation 1 with a forward lag of $ \Delta {GovParty}_i $ and expect its coefficient estimate to be 0 (Schmidheiny and Siegloch Reference Schmidheiny and Siegloch2023). Supplementary Table A.3 shows that this is the case. While there is a significant effect of contemporaneous changes in the governor’s partisan affiliation (we return to this key finding below), the coefficient of changes four/six years ahead in time is close to zero and statistically insignificant. Second, since the test above is based on a small subsample, we also investigate historical developments in political turnover in Supplementary Figure A.2. This again returns evidence against pre-trend concerns.

Main Findings

Table 2 contains our main findings. Panel I reports results using Shift Party ID as the dependent variable (which treats Independents as a unified group), whereas Panel II allows Independents to have a partisan leaning by using Shift Party ID (detailed) as the dependent variable. Columns (1) to (3) look at the effect of changes in the governor’s partisan affiliation. Column (1) includes the full sample of panel respondents, while column (2) excludes respondents whose party identification moved from Democrat to Republican or vice versa between two survey waves (N = 21 individuals). Column (3) excludes all respondents who self-report as Independent at the initial point of observation, which implies a focus on shifts from Democrat/Republican partisanship toward Independent status. Finally, column (4) turns to shifts in the balance of power in the state legislature and again uses the full sample of panel respondents.

Table 2. Main Results on Shifts in (Self-Reported) Partisan Identification

Note: The table reports coefficient estimates obtained from ordered logistic regression models. The dependent variable in Panel I is Shift Party ID, which represents the shift in respondents’ partisan identification between two consecutive survey waves. It is derived from partisanship variables coded 1 for Democrat, 2 for Independent, and 3 for Republican and therefore ranges from -2 (i.e., move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 2 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). The dependent variable in Panel II is Shift Party ID (detailed), which is derived from partisanship variables coded 1 for Democrat, 2 for Independent leaning Democrat, 3 for Independent, 4 for Independent leaning Republican, and 5 for Republican. It thus ranges from -4 (i.e., move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 4 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). The independent variable Shift Governor party has the same construction and range as Shift Party ID. Since there are only very few Independent state governors (0.56% of the sample), these are excluded throughout the analysis. The independent variable Shift legislative party in power refers to the party holding the majority in the state legislature (coded 1 for Democrat, 2 for a hung legislature, and 3 for Republican). All models include a full set of survey wave fixed effects. Standard errors clustered at the individual level are shown in parentheses. Variations in N are due to differences in the number of missing values across variables. ***p<0.01; **p<0.05; *p<0.1.

All point estimates in Table 2 are positive and statistically significant at the 90% confidence level or better. A positive point estimate implies that US state agency leaders who remain in office after a shift in the party in power on average adjust their self-reported partisan leaning in line with their new political principals—meaning toward the left (right) if power shifts away from a Republican (Democratic) governor/legislature. These findings are robust to excluding the few respondents whose party identification moved from Democrat to Republican or vice versa (column 2). Interestingly, our point estimates marginally increase when excluding respondents who self-identify as Independent at the initial point of observation (column 3). This suggests that our findings may be predominantly driven by individuals who shift from overt Democrat/Republican partisanship toward Independent status.

Since our data contain a subset of agency leaders who respond to more than two ASAP waves (see note 3), one might worry that their repeated responses are driving our results. This is not the case. Auxiliary results show that including only the first two waves of each respondent provides similar results (Supplementary Table A.4). We also verified that our results are robust when excluding observations with six-year gaps between survey waves (Supplementary Table A.5). This restriction avoids potential complications from multiple gubernatorial elections between subsequent survey waves, as well as the increased difficulty in identifying panel respondents when the time between survey waves increases (Geys Reference Geys2023). Qualitatively similar results furthermore arise when looking at the upper and lower chambers of the state legislature separately—albeit with substantively and statistically stronger effects for partisan turnover in the lower chamber (column [1]–[2] in Supplementary Table A.6).

As the point estimates in Table 2 are hard to interpret, we re-ran the model in column (2) of Panel I using a multinomial logit model in order to calculate predicted probabilities for shifts in surviving agency leaders’ partisan self-identification (full details in Supplementary Table A.15). Figure 1 displays these predicted probabilities on the y-axis (with 95% confidence intervals) depending on the change (or not) in the governor’s partisan affiliation (on the x-axis). This offers a better sense of the estimated effect sizes. The full line represents the probability of respondents’ partisan identification moving toward the left, while the dashed line gives the probability of a move toward the right. Figure 1 shows that in the absence of a shift in the governor’s partisan affiliation (value 0 on the x-axis), agency leaders who remain in their position are equally likely to shift their partisan identification toward the left (6.7%) or right (7.5%). However, when the governor shifts toward the left, the probability that survivors also shift to the left increases to 9.8%, while the probability that they shift toward the right drops to 4.6% (difference statistically significant at p = 0.08). Similarly, a shift in the governor’s party toward the right increases the probability that surviving agency leaders shift to the right (12.0%) and decreases shifts toward the left (4.5%) (difference statistically significant at p = 0.01).Footnote 8

Figure 1. Predicted Probabilities from Multinomial Logit Models

Note: The figure displays the predicted probability of a change in respondents’ partisan identification (on the Y-axis) following a change (or not) in the governor’s partisan affiliation (on the X-axis). Shift Governor party ranges from -2 (i.e., move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 2 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). The full (dashed) line represents the probability of respondents’ partisan identification moving toward the left (right). The whiskers capture 95% confidence intervals. Predictions are based on a multinomial logit model; full details are provided in Supplementary Table A.15.

Overall, the results in Table 2 and Figure 1 indicate that surviving state agency leaders become notably more likely to alter their partisan identification when the party in power moves away from them between survey waves. The broadly symmetric nature of the observed shifts suggests that there is no partisan bias in these adaptive adjustments. This is confirmed in Supplementary Table A.7. The absolute value of the point estimates for leftward and rightward shifts are never statistically significantly distinct at conventional levels, which also validates our linear specification in Equation 1 and Table 2.

Taken together, our results thus far provide convincing support for H1 and strongly suggest a malleability of partisanship that is not often attributed to bureaucratic elites. Even so, one might wonder how deeply this malleability extends and whether it also becomes reflected in a broader attitudinal (or even behavioral) transformation. Thus, Supplementary Table A.8 extends our analysis using shifts in respondents’ placement on a seven-point liberal-conservative scale as a dependent variable.Footnote 9 The results indicate that surviving agency leaders’ liberal/conservative self-placement shifts to the right (left) when the governor shifts to the right (left), much like we observe for their partisan self-identification. Although these movements are weaker than those in partisanship—as would be expected given the smaller sample size and the fact that party identification and ideology are not identical constructs (Shor and McCarty Reference Shor and McCarty2011)—the pattern of these auxiliary results is consistent with our main findings. They also suggest that the partisan malleability observed in Table 2 and Figure 1 may extend to ideological change over time, although this would require further analysis in future research.

In an ideal world, we would also extend our analyses to behavioral change. Yet, we know of no dataset that provides that type of information at the individual level across all states and across time.Footnote 10 Still, we were able to verify that our main results on partisan self-identification are not picking up a general pattern in the data. To confirm this, as a placebo check, we analyzed more objective questions about respondents’ weekly hours worked and yearly salary. The results in Supplementary Table A.9 confirm that responses to these variables are unaffected by shifts in the party in power, as would be expected.

We should reiterate that our results apply to respondents retaining their position after political turnover (otherwise they drop out of the ASAP surveys, and, hence, our sample). This is important since less malleable individuals could be more likely to leave the organization (either voluntarily or by force). The ASAP data show that the share of state agency leader “survivors” lies around 58%.Footnote 11 Hence, our analysis is germane to most state agency leaders (even though we caution against generalizing our findings to the remaining 42% of state agency leaders, who, as we suggest above, might hold different preferences and levels of malleability).

Heterogeneous Effects

In Table 3, we investigate the hypothesized moderating roles of bureaucrats’ appointment process (H2) and contact patterns (H3). Columns (1) and (2) differentiate between respondents whose appointment was with or without explicit governor consent (see Table 1). Columns (3) and (4) differentiate between respondents based on their contact frequency with the governor and/or their staff at the current point of measurement (i.e., at least monthly contact versus less than monthly), while columns (5) and (6) do the same for contacts with the state legislature and/or their staff. Panels I and II have Shift Party ID and Shift Party ID (detailed), respectively, as dependent variable. In all cases, we use the full sample of respondents.

Table 3. Heterogeneity by Nature of Appointment and Contact Frequency

Note: The table reports coefficient estimates obtained from ordered logistic regression models. For details on the dependent and independent variables, see note to Table 2. Columns (1) and (2) differentiate between respondents whose appointment was with or without governor consent. Columns (3)–(6) differentiate between respondents based on their contact frequency with the governor/legislature and/or their staff at the current point of measurement (i.e., at least monthly contact versus less than monthly). All models include a full set of survey wave fixed effects. Standard errors clustered at the individual level are shown in parentheses. Variations in N are due to differences in the number of missing values across variables. ***p<0.01; **p<0.05; *p<0.1.

The results in Table 3 indicate that our point estimates are both statistically and substantively stronger when looking at the subset of respondents directly appointed by, or having more frequent contacts with, their elected principals.Footnote 12 Interestingly, looking separately at those individuals appointed using “other” procedures (N = 132 individuals)—which predominantly captures appointments under civil service (merit) procedures—we find a non-significant point estimate that is even slightly negative ( $ {\beta}_1 $ = -0.032 / -0.097; p > 0.10). As would be expected, civil service appointments benefit the political independence of agency leaders, while increased political influence over appointments induces stronger adjustments to shifts in the party in power. The findings in Table 3 thus are consistent with H2 and H3. Any adaptive adjustments in surviving agency leaders’ partisan allegiance following a change in the party in power are most prominent among those more closely intertwined with the world of politicians.

Beyond our hypothesized effects, one might also wonder whether the observed adaptive adjustments in self-reported partisanship are more common when there is more elite party polarization at the state level. Such polarization might raise the costs of an agency leader’s failing to adjust to a political turnover event. We investigate this proposition with two distinct data sources. First, using the previously described ASAP question tapping respondent ideology on a 7-point liberal-conservative scale, we identify the mean placement by party, state, and year for all ASAP respondents for the available 1994–2008 period. We then calculate the “ideological distance” between respondents from both major parties by state and year as a proxy for polarization. As we show in Supplementary Figure A.3, agency leaders’ adaptive adjustments are indeed more common when there is greater polarization. We return the same conclusion when instead using a state’s elected Members of Congress’ NOMINATE scores (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Poole, Rosenthal, Boche, Rudkin and Sonnet2024) to measure the ideological distance between a state’s political elites. Those results are reported in Supplementary Figure A.4 (full details in Supplementary Table A.16).

Finally, we also considered whether bureaucratic elites’ partisan malleability depends on their task portfolio or on the identifiability of their partisan leaning from participation in primaries or general elections. The former reflects the organization theory perspective that task portfolios shape bureaucratic elites’ actions and political exposure (Ban, Park, and You Reference Ban, Park and You2023; Christensen and Lægreid Reference Christensen, Lægreid and Pollitt2013; Egeberg Reference Egeberg1999). The latter might create differences in the perceived visibility and relevance of partisan identification. Supplementary Figure A.5 shows that respondents more heavily involved in policy development appear somewhat more likely to reorient their partisan leaning toward that of their elected principals after political turnover, although these findings are not very robust across estimations (full details in Supplementary Table A.16). Supplementary Table A.12 reveals that our findings are strongest in states where partisan orientation is identifiable from voters’ electoral activity, consistent with the notion that this might make bureaucratic elites more sensitive to any partisan (non-)alignment with their political principals.

Robustness and Validity Checks

In this section, we briefly discuss the results of several robustness and validity checks. For brevity, we relegate detailed results to the Supplementary Material.

Starting with the dependent variable, our central line of argument implies that agency leaders wish to mitigate political misalignment with their elected principals. In the ASAP surveys, this could be reflected in respondents failing to answer the question on partisan identification. Despite a non-response rate below 2%, we specified a variable equal to 1 whenever a respondent provided a partisan identification in one survey wave but not in the subsequent wave (0 otherwise). This captures individuals opting to self-censor a previously revealed party preference. We then assess whether such self-censoring varies depending on respondents’ (mis)alignment with the state governor in the previous survey wave (“Aligned/Misaligned wave 1”) and whether there was a change in the party of the governor (“Shift/No shift in governor party”). The results are reported in Supplementary Table A.13. While a joint test for differences across all four groups fails to reach statistical significance at conventional levels, the pattern of these findings corresponds to theoretical expectations. Specifically, individuals aligned during their first survey wave who witness a change in governor (and thus become misaligned) are most likely to stop reporting a party preference (2.78%). Individuals aligned during their first survey wave who do not witness a change in governor (and thus remain aligned) are least likely to start censoring their party preference (0.56%).Footnote 13

Turning to our independent variable, one might worry that agency leaders are responding to more general ideological shifts in the country or their state—rather than shifts in their direct political principal(s). To address this, we implement two auxiliary checks. The first defines shifts in the party in power using the partisan affiliation of the US president (coded in the same way as $ \Delta {GovParty}_i $ ). The second looks at the balance of voter support at the state level between the two main parties in the presidential election nearest to an ASAP survey wave (i.e., Republican minus Democratic vote share). Columns (3)–(5) of Supplementary Table A.6 show at best weak effects for these variables when used independently as explanatory variables, while only $ \Delta {GovParty}_i $ provides significant explanatory power in a “horserace” specification including all three types of partisan shifts. Taken together, this strengthens our inference that political turnover at the state level drives the effects observed in Table 2 and Figure 1, rather than general ideological shifts in the country or the state-level population.

Next, despite the extensive validity checks on our panel dataset, one might still be concerned about the influence of false positives on our results. Since false positives by construction ascribe changes in partisan identification to observations where no unique individual (and, hence, no change) exists, we can assess their influence via a counterfactual exercise using only the non-panel respondents in the ASAP dataset (N = 9,448). That is, we can make all of these into “false positives” by randomly assigning them a (counterfactual) change in partisan identification that follows the distribution of such changes observed in our panel dataset (see the left-hand panel of Supplementary Figure A.1).Footnote 14 Repeating this exercise 1,000 times, and subsequently estimating the model in column 2 of Table 2, leads to the results provided in Supplementary Figure A.6. The left- and right-hand panels show the distribution of the main point estimate ( $ {\beta}_1 $ ) and its associated t-statistic, respectively. For comparison, the vertical lines represent the result in column 2 of Table 2. Crucially, the distribution of point estimates in these counterfactual exercises is centered around zero and far from our actual estimate. This highlights that our main findings would be extremely unlikely to arise as a statistical fluke in a dataset dominated by false positives (or measurement error in partisan self-identification across survey waves). It furthermore suggests that any bias due to false positives and measurement error would most likely bias $ {\beta}_1 $ toward zero.

As mentioned, generalizing our findings beyond the set of “survivors” that make up the majority of state agency leaders should be treated with extreme caution. Even so, we can assess econometrically how our estimates would be affected by the inclusion of “non-survivors.” That is, we can estimate Equation 1 on a dataset extended with x% non-panel respondents (with x = 10, 20, 30, 40) for whom the dependent variable $ \Delta {PartyID}_i $ is set to 0. This imposes a “worst-case scenario” with no partisan adaptive adjustment among non-survivors. We replicate this exercise 1,000 times for each value of x and report the resulting distribution of our main parameter of interest (i.e., $ {\beta}_1 $ ) in Supplementary Figure A.7. As expected, our point estimates decline. Yet, crucially, our key results persist: the point estimate of $ {\beta}_1 $ remains consistently positive and statistically significant at conventional levels, even when adding 40% “non-survivors.”

Finally, some US states are prone to more regular political turnover than others (i.e., swing states versus safe states). One might worry that this varying baseline probability for political turnover could bias our findings. That is, while (the timing of) each gubernatorial/legislative shift is exogenous to our respondents, the different probability of being exposed to them may influence whether and how they respond to such shifts. We deal with this in two ways. First, we replicate the analysis 50 times excluding each US state one by one. The results in the top panel of Supplementary Figure A.8 show that this leaves our results unaffected, such that no one state is driving our findings (the same is true for any individual survey wave; see the bottom panel of Supplementary Figure A.8). Second, we count the number of shifts in the partisan affiliation of the governor in each US state since the end of World War II. Higher numbers reflect a less stable political environment, which may affect responsiveness to specific instances of political turnover (e.g., in the expectation that these will soon be reversed). Supplementary Table A.14 indicates that including these additional controls leaves the point estimates and statistical significance of our main variable of interest unchanged.

CONCLUSION

The partisanship of public bureaucracies and/or agency leaders is a major topic of recent scholarly inquiry and holds key implications for our understanding of the relationship between politics and administration (Bertelli and Grose Reference Bertelli and Grose2011; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton, Bertelli, Grose, Lewis and Nixon2012; Clinton and Lewis Reference Clinton and Lewis2008; Nixon Reference Nixon2004; Reference Nixon2023; Richardson, Clinton, and Lewis Reference Richardson, Clinton and Lewis2018). Research also illustrates that an agency’s partisanship may change over time as a result of turnover among its staff (Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021; Dahlström and Holmgren Reference Dahlström and Holmgren2019; Reference Dahlström and Holmgren2023; Doherty, Lewis, and Limbocker Reference Doherty, Lewis and Limbocker2019). Nevertheless, no scholarship to our knowledge contemplates the fact that bureaucratic elites’ partisan identification may itself be malleable and change due to a dynamic process of adaptive adjustment to shifts in their immediate political environment. In this article, we propose and test just this type of adaptation.

Methodologically, our article contributes to extant research by applying a novel tool to extract a panel dataset from repeated cross-sectional surveys (Geys Reference Geys2023). This innovative approach allows us to take advantage of a quasi-experimental longitudinal research design that strengthens causal inferences. We can draw three main conclusions from our analysis, which are applicable to the large share of state agency leaders (i.e., approximately 60%) retaining their positions after a change in the party in power. First, we find that, on average, such “surviving” US state agency leaders significantly reorient their partisan identity following political leadership changes. Second, we demonstrate that the direction of this adaptive adjustment is toward the party of incoming political leaders. Third, we uncover evidence that the prevalence and strength of this partisan malleability is affected by agency leaders’ appointment procedure and contact pattern with their political principals.

Taken together, our findings call for a re-evaluation of a tacit assumption present across much of the bureaucratic politics literature that the partisan identity of bureaucratic elites is stable across time (Bendor, Glazer, and Hammond Reference Bendor, Glazer and Hammond2001; Fiva et al. Reference Fiva, Geys, Heggedal and Sørensen2021; Spenkuch, Teso, and Xu Reference Spenkuch, Teso and Xu2023). Our results also contribute to a robust scholarly debate around the degree to which partisan identification may or may not be altered based on short-term political changes (Egan Reference Egan2020; Fiorina Reference Fiorina1981; Key Reference Key1966; Lazarsfeld, Berelson, and Gaudet Reference Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet1944; MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989). That literature, which operates within the political behavior tradition, focuses on the partisanship of the mass public, whereas we shift focus to bureaucratic elites (Kertzer and Renshon Reference Kertzer and Renshon2022).

Clearly, however, future research is needed to link the malleability explored in this article to an agency leader’s (potential) behavioral transformation, as well as to her/his agency’s policy decisions. Existing research suggests an overlap between citizens’ self-identified party identification and their preferences on numerous public policy topics—especially those that are more closely “branded” to one party or the other (Dias and Lelkes Reference Dias and Lelkes2021; Orr, Fowler, and Huber Reference Orr, Fowler and Huber2023). Future work might thus exploit variation in such “issue ownership” (Geys Reference Geys2012; Petrocik Reference Petrocik1996) to investigate the connection between self-professed party identification and the actual policy decisions made by bureaucratic elites. This would also allow novel insights into whether and when bureaucratic elites merely “act in accordance with [perceived] expectations” rather than truly internalizing their new principals’ values (Beyers Reference Beyers2010; Checkel Reference Checkel2005, 804; Murdoch et al. Reference Murdoch, Connolly, Geys and Kassim2019). From an organization theory perspective, distinguishing both possibilities is critical to understand when and how organizational design choices might be used to “guide” bureaucratic elites’ adaptive adjustments (Murdoch Reference Murdoch2015; Olsen Reference Olsen2010).

Our dataset concentrates exclusively on the US state agency leaders who stay in their position after a political turnover event, and our conclusions are circumscribed to those individuals. We acknowledge that this prevents us from generalizing our argument and findings to the entire population of US state agency leaders. Yet, crucially, it does not undermine our main insight, which is that state agency leader “survivors” on average display a meaningful tendency to reorient their own partisan identity toward that of their political principals. This implies that a malleability of partisanship may be an important “coping strategy” for public employees after political turnover (Bolton, de Figueiredo, and Lewis Reference Bolton, de Figueiredo and Lewis2021; Golden Reference Golden2000). Our findings also imply the need for future research to investigate malleability among federal political appointees and civil servants, who differ in important ways from their state leader counterparts. In the end, some may view our findings as a normative challenge to public governance because they imply that the advice and expertise of select agency leaders may be tainted by partisan pressures. Yet, others may see our results as normatively positive in that they suggest a bureaucratic responsiveness that has been previously underappreciated by scholars and observers alike. Given these divergent views, we see this topic as a fruitful avenue for future data collection, analysis, and inquiry in the US setting and beyond.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055424000844.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Limitations on data availability and explanation of the requirements to access the data used in this article are provided in the Supplementary Materials. Codes to replicate the findings of this study are openly available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OO6RB0.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of Deil Wright, the Earhart Foundation of Ann Arbor, Michigan, and the University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill; Cynthia Bowling, Theodore Arapis, and Auburn University; the University of Wisconsin-Madison; and the dozens of students and colleagues who drafted questions and collected ASAP data across the years. Henrik Nicolajsen, Liangnan Wang, and Megan Westerman provided excellent research assistance. We also thank the editor, three anonymous referees, Bjørn Mo Forum, Kenn Meyfroodt, Sebastian Schirner, Rune Sørensen, Stefanie Vedder, Arne Wiig, and seminar participants at the universities of Antwerp, UC Riverside, Purdue, UW-Madison, Konstanz, University College London, IE School of Politics, Economics and Global Affairs, and BI Norwegian Business School as well as at the 2024 MPSA meeting and the Biennial Conference of the ECPR Standing Group on Regulatory Governance for insightful suggestions.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

All authors contributed equally to this work.

FUNDING STATEMENT

Funding for this project was provided by the European Commission (Horizon Europe “DemoTrans” project no. 101059288; Work Package Leader: Zuzana Murdoch). Views and opinions expressed are those of the authors only and do not necessarily reflect those of the European Union nor the Horizon Europe program. Neither the European Union nor the granting authority can be held responsible for them.

CONFLICT OF INTERESTS

The authors declare no ethical issues or conflicts of interest in this research.

ETHICAL STANDARDS

The authors declare that they were not directly involved in data collection. Therefore, the authors affirm this research did not involve human participants.

Footnotes

1 As Nebraska has a unicameral legislature, we follow Anzia and Jackman (Reference Anzia and Jackman2013) in including it as an upper chamber. Nebraska’s formally non-partisan legislature is also coded as neither Democratic nor Republican majority. As mentioned below, dropping Nebraska—or any other state—does not affect our inferences.

2 We follow all ethical guidelines set out in Geys (Reference Geys2023), which are compatible with the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) as well as APSA’s Principles and Guidance for Human Subjects Research. While we identify specific background profiles, we do not personally identify individual respondents (which would be ethically and legally unacceptable).

3 Most matched respondents answer only two (85.5%) or three (11.2%) consecutive ASAP survey waves, while the maximum is six survey waves (one respondent).

4 ASAP survey protocols prohibit publicly revealing information shared by respondents. This promise of confidentiality appears to have been highly credible, since very few respondents decline to state their partisan self-identification despite the potentially sensitive nature of this question (1.7% in our sample; 3.7% in the full ASAP dataset). This increases our confidence that we capture bureaucratic elites’ sincere responses at the time of survey implementation. Note that the partisan leaning of Independents was asked starting with the 1974 survey wave.

5 These numbers are similar to those observed in panel datasets of American voters (Green and Platzman Reference Green and Platzman2024).

6 Since there are very few Independent state governors (0.56% of the sample), we exclude them throughout the analysis. Nonetheless, including them does not affect any of our inferences.

7 All models include a full set of survey wave fixed effects to accommodate potential temporal confounders. We also cluster standard errors at the individual level, which accommodates that our dataset includes multiple observations for the same individuals. Nonetheless, all inferences are unaffected when we cluster standard errors at the US state level instead (where shifts in the party in power take place).

8 Not shown in Figure 1, the probability that agency leaders do not change their partisan identification remains stable at 83–86% independent of any shift in the governor party.

9 The question wording changed slightly over time. In 1994, respondents were asked: “On a scale of 1 to 7, with 1 being very conservative and 7 being very liberal, do you generally consider yourself to be more conservative or more liberal?” In 1998–2008, the question was: “On a scale of 1 to 7, with 1 being very conservative and 7 being very liberal, please indicate how you rate or rank yourself on (a) social and morality issues, and (b) taxing and spending issues.” We combined both questions into an overall measure of liberal-conservative placement to maximize the number of years available for analysis (1994–2008).

10 One exception might have been the Database on Ideology, Money in Politics, and Elections (DIME) (Bonica Reference Bonica2014; Reference Bonica2019). Unfortunately, the credible identification of state agency leaders in the DIME dataset is difficult and error prone, which precluded its use.

11 This is calculated based on agency leaders’ years of service (as reported in the ASAP) and the number of years since the last partisan change in the state’s governor. Hence, it reflects the share of ASAP respondents “surviving” at least one governor party change during their tenure. Naturally, this number is higher among those whose appointment was without (65%) rather than with (50%) explicit gubernatorial consent (cf. Table 1). Note also that a vibrant literature on the pull and push factors behind public sector employee turnover (intentions) highlights a complicated mix of political, personal, organizational, or market-driven factors (Hur and Abner Reference Hur and Abner2024; Sowa Reference Sowa, Stein and Kneis2021). While political factors—such as partisan turnover, (lack of) policy influence, or democratic backsliding—are often stressed as important determinants (e.g., Alan-Barkat et al. Reference Alan-Barkat, Gilad, Kosti and Shpaizman2024; Bauer Reference Bauer2023; Dahlström and Holmgren Reference Dahlström and Holmgren2019; Doherty, Lewis, and Limbocker Reference Doherty, Lewis and Limbocker2019), our data do not indicate why state agency leaders (did not) leave their office.

12 These two groups show considerable overlap (pairwise correlation r = 0.336). Yet, estimating an encompassing model with both appointments and contacts highlights that both are important sources of heterogeneity in their own right (Supplementary Table A.10). Note also that separating frequent contacts between those with the governor/legislature or those with their staff does not affect our findings (Supplementary Table A.11).

13 We also assess whether respondents adjust their partisan alignment again after witnessing a second, opposing shift in the party in power. Unfortunately, our dataset includes only 28 respondents witnessing such multiple partisan turnovers. Interestingly, however, we observe three cases (or 11%) where respondents who shift their partisan identification in line with a first change in the governor’s party subsequently move back toward their original partisan self-identification after a second, opposing change in the governor’s party. While consistent with our main findings and argument, we are apprehensive about putting too much weight on this finding given the very small sample size.

14 Any measurement error in respondents’ partisan self-identification across survey waves would likewise imply our data may document shifts where no “true” change exists (e.g., Green and Platzman Reference Green and Platzman2024). Even though this type of measurement error appears less likely among bureaucratic elites than the general population (see Footnote footnote 4), this counterfactual exercise also addresses the potential impact of any such measurement error on our main findings. Note furthermore that false negatives are arguably less problematic in our setting, since they mainly reduce our sample size and, therefore, statistical power (Geys Reference Geys2023).

References

REFERENCES

Aberbach, Joel, Putnam, Robert, and Rockman, Bert. 1981. Bureaucrats and Politicians in Western Democracies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acharya, Avidit, Blackwell, Matthew, and Sen, Maya. 2018. “Explaining Preferences from Behavior: A Cognitive Dissonance Approach.” Journal of Politics 80 (2): 400–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alan-Barkat, Saar, Gilad, Sharon, Kosti, Nir, and Shpaizman, Ilana. 2024. “Civil Servants Divergent Reactions of Democratic Backsliding and Intended Exit, Voice and Work.” Mimeo. Retrieved January 23, 2024.Google Scholar
Anzia, Sarah, and Jackman, Molly. 2013. “Legislative Organization and the Second Face of Power: Evidence from U.S. State Legislatures.” Journal of Politics 75 (1): 210–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bach, Tobias, and Veit, Sylvia. 2018. “The Determinants of Promotion to High Public Office in Germany: Partisan Loyalty, Political Craft, or Managerial Competencies?Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 28 (2): 254–69.Google Scholar
Baekgaard, Martin, Herd, Pamela, and Moynihan, Donald. 2023. “Of ‘Welfare Queens’ and ‘Poor Carinas’: Social Constructions, Deservingness Messaging and the Mental Health of Welfare Clients.” British Journal of Political Science 53 (2): 594612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ban, Pamela, Park, Ju Yeon, and You, Hye Young. 2023. “Bureaucrats in Congress: Strategic Information Sharing in Policymaking.” Working Paper.Google Scholar
Bauer, Michael. 2023. “Public Administration under Populist Rule: Standing Up against Democratic Backsliding.” International Journal of Public Administration, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/01900692.2023.2243400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, Roy, and Leary, Mark. 1995. “The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachment as a Fundamental Human Motivation.” Psychological Bulletin 117 (3): 497529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bendor, Jonathan, Glazer, Amihai, and Hammond, Thomas. 2001. “Theories of Delegation.” Annual Review of Political Science 4: 235–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendor, Jonathan, and Moe, Terry. 1985. “An Adaptive Model of Bureaucratic Politics.” American Political Science Review 79 (3): 755–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertelli, Anthony, and Grose, Christian. 2011. “The Lengthened Shadow of Another Institution? Ideal Point Estimation for the Executive Branch and Congress.” American Journal of Political Science 55 (4): 766–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beyers, Jan. 2010. “Conceptual and Methodological Challenges in the Study of European Socialization.” Journal of European Public Policy 17 (6): 909–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, Alexander, de Figueiredo, John M., and Lewis, David E.. 2021. “Elections, Ideology, and Turnover in the US Federal Government.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 31 (2): 451–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonica, Adam. 2014. “Mapping the Ideological Marketplace.” American Journal of Political Science 58 (2): 367–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonica, Adam. 2019. “Are Donation-Based Measures of Ideology Valid Predictors of Individual-Level Policy Preferences?Journal of Politics 81 (1): 327–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowling, Cynthia. 2018. “State Bureaucracies.” In Politics in the American States: A Comparative Analysis, eds. Gray, Virginia, Hanson, Russel and Kousser, Thad, 503–27. Thousand Oaks, CA: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Brudney, Jeffrey, and Wright, Deil. 2002. “Revisiting Administrative Reform in the American States: The Status of Reinventing Government during the 1990s.” Public Administration Review 62 (3): 353–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkhardt, Marlene. 1994. “Social Interaction Effects Following a Technological Change: A Longitudinal Investigation.” Academy of Management Journal 37 (4): 869–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Angus, Converse, Philip, Miller, Warren, and Stokes, Donald. 1960. The American Voter. New York: Wiley Press.Google Scholar
Checkel, Jeffrey. 2005. “International Institutions and Socialization in Europe: Introduction and Framework.” International Organization 59 (4): 801–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Tom, and Lægreid, Per. 2003. “Complex Interaction and Influence among Political and Administrative Leaders.” International Review of Administrative Science 69 (3): 385400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Tom, and Lægreid, Per. 2013. “Context and Administrative Reforms: A Transformative Approach.” In Context in Public Policy and Management, ed. Pollitt, Christopher, 131–56. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Christensen, Tom, Lægreid, Per, and Arne Røvik, Kjell. 2021. Organization Theory and the Public Sector. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Christensen, Jørgen, Klemmensen, Robert, and Ostrup, Niels. 2014. “Politicization and the Replacement of Top Civil Servants in Denmark.” Governance 27 (2): 215–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Jørgen, and Ostrup, Niels. 2018. “Bureaucratic Dilemmas: Civil Servants between Political Responsiveness and Normative Constraints.” Governance 31 (3): 481–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinton, Joshua, and Lewis, David. 2008. “Expert Opinion, Agency Characteristics, and Agency Preferences.” Political Analysis 16 (1): 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinton, Joshua, Bertelli, Anthony, Grose, Christian, Lewis, David, and Nixon, David. 2012. “Separated Powers in the United States: The Ideology of Agencies, Presidents, and Congress.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (2): 341–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Mollie, and Cassell, Kaitlen. 2023. “Reducing Item Nonresponse to Vote-Choice Questions: Evidence from a Survey Experiment in Mexico.” Public Opinion Quarterly 87 (1): 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colonnelli, Emanuele, Prem, Mounu, and Teso, Edoardo. 2020. “Patronage and Selection in Public Sector Organizations.” American Economic Review 110 (10): 3071–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlström, Carl, and Holmgren, Mikael. 2019. “The Political Dynamics of Bureaucratic Turnover.” British Journal of Political Science 49 (3): 823–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlström, Carl, and Holmgren, Mikael. 2023. “Loyal Leaders, Affluent Agencies: The Budgetary Implications of Political Appointments in the Executive Branch.” Journal of Politics 85 (2): 640–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, Nicolas, and Lelkes, Yphtach. 2021. “The Nature of Affective Polarization: Disentangling Policy Disagreement from Partisan Identity.” American Journal of Political Science 66 (3): 775–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, Kathleen, Lewis, David, and Limbocker, Scott. 2019. “Executive Control and Turnover in the Senior Executive Service.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 29 (2): 159–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, Patrick. 2020. “Identity as Dependent Variable: How Americans Shift their Identities to Align with their Politics.” American Journal of Political Science 64 (3): 699716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeberg, Morten. 1999. “The Impact of Bureaucratic Structure on Policy Making.” Public Administration 77 (1): 155–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeberg, Morten, and Trondal, Jarle. 2011. “Agencification and Location: Does Agency Site Matter?Public Organization Review 11 (2): 97108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epitropaki, Olga, Kark, Ronit, Mainemelis, Charalampos, and Lord, Robert. 2017. “Leadership and Followership Identity Processes: A Multilevel Review.” Leadership Quarterly 28 (1): 104–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, Margaret. 2018. “Governors and the Executive Branch.” In Politics in the American States: A Comparative Analysis, eds. Gray, Virginia, Hanson, Russel and Kousser, Thad, 235–74. Thousand Oaks, CA: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, Leon. 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorina, Morris. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Fiva, Jon, Geys, Benny, Heggedal, Tom-Reiel, and Sørensen, Rune. 2021. “Political Alignment and Bureaucratic Pay.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 31 (3): 596615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folke, Olle, Hirano, Shigeo, and Snyder, James. 2011. “Patronage and Elections in US States.” American Political Science Review 105 (3): 567–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foos, Florian, and Bischof, Daniel. 2022. “Tabloid Media Campaigns and Public Opinion: Quasi-Experimental Evidence on Euroscepticism in England.” American Political Science Review 116 (1): 1937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geuijen, Karin, ‘t Hart, Paul, Princen, Sebastiaan, and Yesilkagit, Kutsal. 2008. The New Eurocrats: National Civil Servants in EU Policymaking. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geys, Benny. 2012. “Success and Failure in Electoral Competition: Selective Issue Emphasis under Incomplete Issue Ownership.” Electoral Studies 31 (2): 406–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geys, Benny. 2023. “Fancy Seeing You Here…Again: Uncovering Individual-Level Panel Data in Repeated Cross-Sectional Surveys.” Public Administration Review 83 (6): 1761–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geys, Benny, Connolly, Sara, Kassim, Hussein, and Murdoch, Zuzana. 2020. “Follow the Leader? Leader Succession and Staff Attitudes in Public Sector Organizations.” Public Administration Review 80 (4): 555–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geys, Benny, Lægreid, Per, Murdoch, Zuzana, and Yackee, Susan Webb. 2024. “Replication Data for: I’m a Survivor: Political Dynamics in Bureaucratic Elites’ Partisan Identification.” Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OO6RB0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golden, Marissa. 2000. What Motivates Bureaucrats? Politics and Administration during the Reagan Years. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, Donald, and Platzman, Paul. 2024. “Partisan Stability during Turbulent Times: Evidence from Three American Panel Surveys.” Political Behavior 46: 377403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Homola, Jonathan, Pereira, Miguel, and Tavits, Margit. 2020. “Legacies of the Third Reich: Concentration Camps and Out-Group Intolerance.” American Political Science Review 114 (2): 573–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hur, Hyunkang, and Abner, Gordon. 2024. “What Makes Public Employees Want to Leave their Job? A Meta-Analysis of Turnover Intention Predictors among Public Sector Employees.” Public Administration Review 84 (1): 115–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jilke, Sebastian, Van de Walle, Steven, and Kim, Soonhee. 2016. “Generating Usable Knowledge through an Experimental Approach to Public Administration.” Public Administration Review 76 (1): 6972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappe, Roland, and Schuster, Christian. 2022. “Agents of Past Principals: The Lasting Effects of Incumbents on the Political Ideology of Bureaucrats.” European Journal of Political Research 61 (3): 807–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua, and Renshon, Jonathan. 2022. “Experiments and Surveys on Political Elites.” Annual Review of Political Science 25: 529–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Key, V.O. Jr. 1942. “Politics and Government.” In The Future of Government, ed. White, Leonard, 145–63. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Key, V.O. Jr. 1966. The Responsible Electorate. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiecolt, Jill. 1988. “Recent Developments in Attitudes and Social Structure.” Annual Review of Sociology 14: 381403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollman, Ken, and Jackson, John. 2021. Dynamic Partisanship: How and Why Voter Loyalties Change. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarsfeld, Paul, Berelson, Bernard, and Gaudet, Hazel. 1944. The People’s Choice: How the Voter Makes up his Mind in a Presidential Campaign. New York: Duell, Sloan, and Pearce.Google Scholar
Lewis, Jeffrey, Poole, Keith, Rosenthal, Howard, Boche, Adam, Rudkin, Aaron, and Sonnet, Luke. 2024. “Voteview: Congressional Roll-Call Votes Database.” Retrieved April 10, 2024. https://voteview.com.Google Scholar
Liden, Robert, and Mitchell, Terence. 1988. “Ingratiatory Behaviors in Organizational Settings.” Academy of Management Review 13 (4): 572–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, Martin, and Hood, Christopher. 2003. “Competency and Bureaucracy: Diffusion, Application and Appropriate Response?West European Politics 26 (3): 131–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, Robert, Gatti, Paola, and Chui, Susanna. 2016. “Social-Cognitive, Relational, and Identity-Based Approaches to Leadership.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136: 119–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKuen, Michael, Erikson, Robert, and Stimson, James. 1989. “Macropartisanship.” American Political Science Review 83 (4): 1125–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March, James, and Olsen, Johan. 1984. “The New Institutionalism: Organizational Factors in Political Life.” American Political Science Review 78 (3): 734–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March, James, and Olsen, Johan. 1975. “The Uncertainty of the Past: Organizational Learning under Ambiguity.” European Journal of Political Research 3 (2): 147–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayntz, Renate, and Derlien, Hans-Ulrich. 1989. “Party Patronage and Politicization of the West German Administrative Elite 1970-1987: Toward Hybridization?Governance 2 (4): 384404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moe, Terry. 1985. “Control and Feedback in Economic Regulation: The Case of the NLRB.” American Political Science Review 79 (4): 1094–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moynihan, Donald, and Ingraham, Patricia. 2010. “The Suspect Handmaiden: The Evolution of Politics and Administration in the American State.” Public Administration Review 70 (S1): 229–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdoch, Zuzana. 2015. “Organization Theory and the Study of European Union Institutions: Lessons and Opportunities.” Organization Studies 36 (12): 1675–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdoch, Zuzana, Connolly, Sara, and Kassim, Hussein. 2018. “Administrative Legitimacy and the Democratic Deficit of the European Union.” Journal of European Public Policy 25 (3): 389408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdoch, Zuzana, Connolly, Sara, Geys, Benny, and Kassim, Hussein. 2019. “Do International Institutions Matter? Socialization and International Bureaucrats.” European Journal of International Relations 25 (3): 852–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, David. 2004. “Separation of Powers and Appointee Ideology.” Journal of Law, Economics, and Organizations 20 (2): 438–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, David. 2023. “Do Bureaucratic Appointees Change their Minds? Preference Stability at the NLRB.” Social Science Quarterly 104 (4): 702–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olsen, Johan. 2010. Governing through Institution Building: Institutional Theory and Recent European Experiments in Democratic Organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, Anthony, and Weiss, David. 2000. “The Impact of Anonymity on Responses to Sensitive Questions.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 30 (8): 1691–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, Lilla, Fowler, Anthony, and Huber, Gregory. 2023. “Is Affective Polarization Driven by Identity, Loyalty, or Substance?American Journal of Political Science 67 (4): 948–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palus, Christine, and Yackee, Susan Webb. 2016. “Clerks or Kings? Partisan Alignment and Delegation to the U.S. Bureaucracy.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 26 (4): 693708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palus, Christine, and Yackee, Susan Webb. 2022. “When Does the Multiple Principals Hypothesis Hold? The Politics of US Agency Policymaking Autonomy.” Governance 35 (1): 4364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrocik, John. 1996. “Issue Ownership in Presidential Elections, with a 1980 Case Study.” American Journal of Political Science 40 (3): 825–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rainey, Hal, and Steinbauer, Paula. 1999. “Galloping Elephants: Developing Elements of a Theory of Effective Government Organizations.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 9 (1): 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, Mark, Clinton, Joshua, and Lewis, David. 2018. “Elite Perceptions of Agency Ideology and Workforce Skill.” Journal of Politics 80 (1): 303–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, Ronald, Tran, Felix, Lewark, Lauren, and Epstein, Robert. 2018. “Estimates of Non-Heterosexual Prevalence: The Roles of Anonymity and Privacy in Survey Methodology.” Archives of Sexual Behavior 47 (4): 1069–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidheiny, Kurt, and Siegloch, Sebastian. 2023. “On Event Studies and Distributed-Lags in Two-Way Fixed Effects Models: Identification, Equivalence, and Generalization.” Journal of Applied Econometrics 38 (5): 695713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholz, John. 1986. “Regulatory Enforcement in a Federalist System.” American Political Science Review 80 (4): 1249–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shor, Boris, and McCarty, Nolan. 2011. “The Ideological Mapping of American Legislatures.” American Political Science Review 105 (3): 530–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, Herbert. 1957. Administrative Behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sowa, Jessica. 2021. “Voluntary Turnover in Public Administration.” In Research Handbook on HMR in the Public Sector, eds. Stein, Bram and Kneis, Eva, 145–57. London: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Spenkuch, Jörg, Teso, Edoardo, and Xu, Guo. 2023. “Ideology and Performance in Public Organizations.” Econometrica 91 (4): 1171–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toral, Guillermo. 2024. “How Patronage Delivers: Political Appointments, Bureaucratic Accountability, and Service Delivery in Brazil.” American Journal of Political Science 68 (2): 797815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, Patrick, Montgomery, Jacob, and Smith, Steven. 2019. “Party Identification in the Age of Obama: Evidence on the Sources of Stability and Systematic Change in Party Identification from a Long-Term Panel Survey.” Political Research Quarterly 72 (2): 309–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Ryzin, Gregg. 2021. “Nudging and Muddling through.” Perspectives on Public Management and Governance 4 (4): 339–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vantaggiato, Francesca, Murdoch, Zuzana, Kassim, Hussein, Geys, Benny, and Connolly, Sara. 2024. “Intraorganizational Mobility and Employees’ Work-related Contact Patterns: Evidence from Panel Data in the European Commission.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory. https://doi.org/10.1093/jopart/muae014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldo, Dwight. 1952. “Development of Theory of Democratic Administration.” American Political Science Review 46 (1): 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weick, Karl. 1995. Sensemaking in Organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Wooldridge, Jeffrey. 2010. Econometric Analysis of Cross-Section and Panel Data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Yackee, Susan Webb. 2023. “US Agency Growth Aspirations and the Effect of Ideological Extremism.” Public Administration Review 83 (6): 1785–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yackee, Jason Webb, and Yackee, Susan Webb. 2021. “The American State Administrators Project: A New 50-State, 50-Year Data Resource for Scholars.” Public Administration Review 81 (3): 558–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Appointment Procedures for US State Agency Leaders

Figure 1

Table 2. Main Results on Shifts in (Self-Reported) Partisan Identification

Figure 2

Figure 1. Predicted Probabilities from Multinomial Logit ModelsNote: The figure displays the predicted probability of a change in respondents’ partisan identification (on the Y-axis) following a change (or not) in the governor’s partisan affiliation (on the X-axis). Shift Governor party ranges from -2 (i.e., move to the left from Republican to Democrat) to 2 (move to the right from Democrat to Republican). The full (dashed) line represents the probability of respondents’ partisan identification moving toward the left (right). The whiskers capture 95% confidence intervals. Predictions are based on a multinomial logit model; full details are provided in Supplementary Table A.15.

Figure 3

Table 3. Heterogeneity by Nature of Appointment and Contact Frequency

Supplementary material: File

Geys et al. supplementary material

Geys et al. supplementary material
Download Geys et al. supplementary material(File)
File 736.6 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Geys et al. Dataset

Link
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.