Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T22:25:13.981Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lifelong learning in active ageing discourse: its conserving effect on wellbeing, health and vulnerability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2016

MIYA NARUSHIMA*
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Brock University, Ontario, Canada.
JIAN LIU
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Brock University, Ontario, Canada.
NAOMI DIESTELKAMP
Affiliation:
Independent Researcher, Canada.
*
Address for correspondence: Miya Narushima, Department of Health Sciences, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada L2S3A1 E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The Active Ageing Framework has been adapted as a global strategy in ageing policies, practices and research over the last decade. Lifelong learning, however, has not been fully integrated into this discourse. Using survey data provided by 416 adults (aged 60 years and above) enrolled in non-formal general-interest courses in a public continuing education programme in Canada, this study examined the association between older adults’ duration of participation in the courses and their level of psychological wellbeing, while taking their age, gender, self-rated health and vulnerability level into consideration. An analytical framework was developed based on the literature of old-age vulnerabilities and the benefits of lifelong learning. Two logistic regression and trend analyses were conducted. The results indicate that older adults’ participation is independently and positively associated with their psychological wellbeing, even among those typically classified as ‘vulnerable’. This result provides additional evidence that suggests the continuous participation in non-formal lifelong learning may help sustain older adults’ psychological wellbeing. It provides older learners, even those who are most vulnerable, with a compensatory strategy to strengthen their reserve capacities, allowing them to be autonomous and fulfilled in their everyday life. The result of this study highlights the value of the strategic and unequivocal promotion of community-based non-formal lifelong learning opportunities for developing inclusive, equitable and caring active ageing societies.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

It has been nearly 15 years since the World Health Organization (WHO) endorsed the Active Ageing Framework (hereafter the active ageing framework) in 2002. It has influenced ageing policies and practices across the world, and drawn public attention to the new opportunities and challenges that global ageing brings to both individuals and societies. The active ageing framework advocates optimising opportunities for ‘health’, ‘participation’ and ‘security’ – three key determinants of the quality of later life. It also advocates recognising physical health, mental health and social connections as equally important elements (WHO 2002). Active ageing discourse is a product of growing governmental concerns about financing health care and social services for ageing populations. At the same time, it mirrors and potentially promotes a positive shift in social attitudes towards old age: viewing later life not as a period of ‘deficits’, but as either a time of ‘competency and knowledge’ (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013: 1078) or a ‘period of opportunity and wellbeing, with retention or development of the psychological and cognitive resources to cope with life's challenges’ (Bowling and Iliffe Reference Bowling and Iliffe2011: 13).

Following this global trend, Canada has started working on active ageing policies, encouraging all level of governments to create age-friendly communities by improving services, programmes and infrastructure for ‘ageing in place’ (Federation of Canadian Municipalities 2013). Senior citizens (aged 65 and older) currently make up 15 per cent of the Canadian population, much smaller than other world regions like Japan and Western Europe. Yet, they are the fastest growing age group, projected to increase by 110 per cent over the next 20 years (Federation of Canadian Municipalities 2013; Statistics Canada 2012a ). Approximately 30 per cent of current Canadian seniors are immigrants, most of who came from Europe and Asia, and have lived in Canada for several decades (Turcotte and Schellenberg Reference Turcotte and Schellenberg2007). It is predicted that successive cohorts of seniors will be increasingly ethno-culturally diverse (Statistics Canada 2016). Although 92 per cent of Canadian seniors live in private households, three-quarters reported at least one chronic health condition and one-quarter reported more than three conditions (Canadian Institute for Health Information 2011). Moreover, one in four seniors are living alone (Federation of Canadian Municipalities 2013; Statistics Canada 2012b ), and 30 per cent of older Canadian women live in poverty (National Seniors Strategy n.d.). These facts suggest that one important task for seniors is to find the means to optimise wellbeing to help cope with chronic health conditions, loneliness, stress and other setbacks (Zarit Reference Zarit, Bengtson, Gans, Putney and Silverstein2009).

Lifelong learning in late adulthood deserves more attention in the current discourse of active ageing policies. The active ageing framework (WHO 2002) acknowledges that lifelong learning, along with formal education and literacy, is an important factor that facilitates participation, health and security as people grow older. While a few places, such as the European Commission (Oxley Reference Oxley2009) and the Province of British Columbia, Canada (Government of British Columbia n.d.), have explicitly inscribed ‘lifelong’ in their active ageing policies, generally speaking, it is mentioned far less often than topics like physical activities and paid and non-paid work. Nevertheless, an increasing body of literature has found clear links between adult learning and learners’ subjective wellbeing and health (Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe Reference Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe2012; Feinstein and Hammond Reference Feinstein and Hammond2004; Feinstein et al. Reference Feinstein, Budge, Vorhaus and Duckworth2008; Field Reference Field2009, Reference Field2011; Hammond and Feinstein Reference Hammond and Feinstein2006; Manninen et al. Reference Manninen, Sgier, Fleige, Thone-Geyer, Kil, Mozina, Danihelkova, Mallows, Duncan, Merilainen, Diez, Sava, Javrh, Vrecer, Mihajlovic, Kecap, Zappaterra, Kornilow, Ebener and Operti2014; Schuller et al. Reference Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004), while irrefutable evidence exists to show that these benefits are even greater for vulnerable groups (Bailey, Breen and Ward Reference Bailey, Breen and Ward2010; Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe Reference Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe2012; Feinstein et al. Reference Feinstein, Budge, Vorhaus and Duckworth2008; Manninen et al. Reference Manninen, Sgier, Fleige, Thone-Geyer, Kil, Mozina, Danihelkova, Mallows, Duncan, Merilainen, Diez, Sava, Javrh, Vrecer, Mihajlovic, Kecap, Zappaterra, Kornilow, Ebener and Operti2014).

Studies that directly examine the effects of later-life learning on older adults, however, are few and far between. In many countries, adults over the age of 65 are often excluded from adult learning statistics as they are assumed to be the post-work generation. In addition, research in adult learning traditionally focuses more on ‘transforming effects’ (e.g. personal change, community activism) than ‘conserving effects’ (e.g. self-maintenance, the social fabric), where ‘education prevents decay or collapse or consolidates a positive state of stability’ (Schuller Reference Schuller, Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004: 25). Given the context of global ageing, it becomes more important to investigate the latter effects of learning, which can be quite invisible, yet play an important role in maintaining autonomy, health and quality of life among older adults. Although still limited in volume, recent studies examining the association between later-life learning and psychological wellbeing suggest that continued engagement may help older learners sustain wellbeing over the long term (Jenkins Reference Jenkins2011; Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2015; Leung and Liu Reference Leung and Liu2011; Narushima Reference Narushima2008). The purpose of this study, therefore, is to identify further the effect of the duration of older adults’ participation in lifelong learning on their psychological wellbeing, while taking into consideration their health state, vulnerability and other key demographic variables.

Literature review

Active ageing, health and chronic conditions

The idea of being active for a good old age is not new. However, the principle of the WHO's (2002) active ageing framework differs from traditional activity theory (Havighurst Reference Havighurst1961; Neugarten, Havighurst and Tobin Reference Neugarten, Havighurst, Tobin and Neugarten1968). Activity theory has been criticised for its unrealistic expectation that older adults can maintain the same levels of activity as they did in middle age by ‘denying the onset of old age’ (Walker Reference Walker2002: 122). The active ageing framework advocates a broader approach, which defines ‘being active’ as continuing to engage in any social, economic, cultural or civic activity at the level of one's capacity (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013). The idea is to embrace not only the individual's right and responsibility to remain active, but also the government's responsibility to create an age-friendly social system and community environment. Consequently, in theory, the framework calls for all levels of governments to take action to ensure opportunities for all older adults, especially those who are most vulnerable, to stay involved, healthy and secure (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013; Mayhew Reference Mayhew2005; Walker Reference Walker2002, Reference Walker2006; WHO 2002).

Nonetheless, the adaptation of this framework is far from flawless. Boudiny (Reference Boudiny2013) in the United Kingdom (UK) argues that the active ageing framework tends to be narrowly used to promote physical activity and prolonged labour participation, so that it is often used interchangeably with ‘productive ageing’ and ‘healthy ageing’. According to Boudiny, this dominant discourse risks falling into the same pattern as activity theory did a half century ago, because it posits health and independence – unattainable for vulnerable adults who are frail, dependent and very old – as its ultimate goal. In order to develop more inclusive policies and practices, she proposes an alternative vision that acknowledges health as a resource for and an outcome of active ageing, not as a prerequisite for or a goal of active ageing (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013).

Recent research has started paying attention to the experiences of active ageing with chronic conditions. Richardson, Grime and Ong's (Reference Richardson, Grime and Ong2014) qualitative study in the UK found that their participants (N = 27, aged 55–90), who lived with severe osteoarthritis, pragmatically just ‘kept going’ in body and mind despite chronic pain and stiffness. Lassen's (Reference Lassen2015) fieldwork in two activity centres in Denmark also found that his participants (N = 17, aged 58–92) enjoyed various activities despite a range of serious chronic conditions, thereby ‘keeping disease at arm's length’. Lassen (Reference Lassen2015: 1367) argues that engagement in ‘social and/or physical activity forms enough distance to help older adults focus on wellness rather than illness, and manage their everyday life to delay further physical deterioration’. What these studies (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013; Lassen Reference Lassen2015; Richardson, Grime and Ong Reference Richardson, Grime and Ong2014) share is a realistic picture of growing old, one that acknowledges the changes and challenges of late adulthood.

Vulnerabilities, reserve capacities and compensatory strategies in later life

The notion of ‘vulnerabilities’ provides a helpful basis to analyse the roles of lifelong learning in the light of active ageing. Vulnerabilities in old age involve various ‘risks’ that may cause a poor quality of later life or premature or unintended death (Grundy Reference Grundy2006; Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). According to the ‘old-age vulnerability framework’, the most high-risk sub-groups of older adults are the old-old, those with low incomes, those with limited education, those widowed and living alone, those with poor social networks, those in poor health and women (Grundy Reference Grundy2006; Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). In other words, vulnerabilities in late adulthood are determined by individuals’ socio-demographic characteristics (i.e. age, income, education level, marital status, gender), their living and health conditions, the availability of resources and their ability to use those resources. It is reasonable to assume that the more risk factors, the more likely an older person will suffer severe outcomes, as the risks are more likely to intersect.

Nonetheless, according to the vulnerabilities framework, negative consequences arise only when the balance between a person's risk factors/conditions and his or her reserve capacities falls below his or her control to ensure a reasonable quality of life (Grundy Reference Grundy2006; Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). Vulnerability involves four domains: ‘states’ (i.e. personal characteristics), ‘threat’ (i.e. events/environmental challenges), ‘coping capacity’ (i.e. an individual's reserve capacities such as positive wellbeing, social network and utilisation of compensatory strategies) and ‘outcome’ (i.e. quality of life and survival). The interaction of the first three domains determines the fourth: i.e. either a negative or positive outcome (Grundy Reference Grundy2006). Consequently, not every older adult who shares similar risk factors/conditions ends up with a negative outcome. It depends on one's reserve capacities and how one can utilise compensatory strategies to change and manage life (Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). While reserve capacities and compensatory strategies in later life are largely influenced by a person's life history and social environment, this does not mean that it is impossible to reinforce or develop them in old age. According to the old-age vulnerabilities framework, it is important to recognise the significant effects of current circumstances and behaviour on the general wellbeing of older people (Grundy Reference Grundy2006: 108). In this study, we thus view the participation in a specific form of lifelong learning programme as a compensatory strategy that older adults can draw on to improve those reserve capacities.

Psychological wellbeing as a predictor of health and quality of life

Psychological wellbeing is one of the most important reserve capacities. While its definition differs slightly from study to study (Diener Reference Diener2006; Olsson et al. Reference Olsson, Hagnelius, Olsson and Nilsson2013), psychological wellbeing generally involves the individual's subjective evaluation of various aspects of his or her life (e.g. of events, physical and mental issues, and life circumstances). It consists of three basic elements: the individual's affect (e.g. positive or negative emotional states), ability (e.g. adaptive capacity or coping skills) and perception (e.g. satisfaction, purpose and outlook) (Catolico 1997, cited in Momtaz et al. Reference Momtaz, Ibrahim, Hamid and Yahaya2011; Diener Reference Diener2006; Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) 2013; Olsson et al. Reference Olsson, Hagnelius, Olsson and Nilsson2013; Ryff et al. Reference Ryff, Love, Urry, Muller, Rosenkranz, Friedman, Davidson and Singer2006). Psychological wellbeing as used in this study is closely related to concepts such as eudaimonic wellbeing, positive functioning and self-actualisation, which entails aspects of life satisfaction, autonomy, integrity, personal growth, purpose of life, competence and mastery, and positive relatedness (Ryan and Deci Reference Ryan and Deci2000; Ryff and Keyes 1995, cited in Vanhoutte Reference Vanhoutte, Field, Burke and Cooper2014).

Psychological wellbeing in late adulthood is a complex variable which may be influenced by both personal as well as social factors including: the older individual's demographic characteristics (e.g. age, marital status, education level, occupation and income, degree of social integration and social support), health state (e.g. self-rated health, chronic diseases, functional capacity), availability of supportive social environment and cultural values in their society (e.g. ageism) (Iwasa et al. Reference Iwasa, Kawaai, Gondo, Inagaki and Suzuki2006; Momtaz et al. Reference Momtaz, Ibrahim, Hamid and Yahaya2011; Olsson et al. Reference Olsson, Hagnelius, Olsson and Nilsson2013; Vanhoutte Reference Vanhoutte, Field, Burke and Cooper2014). Not surprisingly, the personal and social factors affecting psychological wellbeing overlap with the aforementioned risk factors and vulnerabilities in later life. It is thus reasonable to assume that the higher the degree of vulnerability, the more chances an older person will report a lower level of psychological wellbeing.

Since these risk factors and conditions are related to mortality, psychological wellbeing is also used as a relevant predictor for physical health, survival and quality of life in later adulthood (Iwasa et al. Reference Iwasa, Kawaai, Gondo, Inagaki and Suzuki2006). A number of studies in psychology and health have found that a higher level of psychological wellbeing leads to better coping with life events (Andrews Reference Andrews2001), lower morbidity, decreased symptoms and pain (Pressman and Cohen Reference Pressman and Cohen2005), a lowered risk of cardiovascular disease and distress (Ryff et al. Reference Ryff, Love, Urry, Muller, Rosenkranz, Friedman, Davidson and Singer2006) and reduced falling (Anstey et al. Reference Anstey, Burns, von Sanden and Luszez2008). Conversely, a higher level of ill-being (e.g. anxiety, anger, depressive symptoms) is a predictor of an increased risk of functional disability (Cuijpers, de Graaf and van Dorsselaer Reference Cuijpers, de Graaf and van Dorsselaer2004), cardiovascular disease (Ryff et al. Reference Ryff, Love, Urry, Muller, Rosenkranz, Friedman, Davidson and Singer2006) and mortality (Howell Reference Howell2009; Iwasa et al. Reference Iwasa, Kawaai, Gondo, Inagaki and Suzuki2006).

Combined, these studies confirm the importance of maintaining positive psychological wellbeing as a reserve capacity for health and quality of life to help mitigate unwanted outcomes in the face of older adults’ vulnerabilities. They also suggest why we should examine psychological wellbeing not only in terms of individuals’ efforts for emotional regulation, but also as it is related to social contexts, including the availability of compensatory strategies to maintain positive wellbeing.

Benefits of lifelong learning participation

Over the last decade, a growing body of literature in adult education has found psychological wellbeing and health to be an important part of the wider benefits of lifelong learning. Large-scale studies in the UK and Europe have unanimously supported the positive link between lifelong learning participation and psychological wellbeing and health among adult learners (Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe Reference Dolan, Fujiwara and Metcalfe2012; Feinstein and Hammond Reference Feinstein and Hammond2004; Feinstein et al. Reference Feinstein, Budge, Vorhaus and Duckworth2008; Field Reference Field2009, Reference Field2011; Manninen et al. Reference Manninen, Sgier, Fleige, Thone-Geyer, Kil, Mozina, Danihelkova, Mallows, Duncan, Merilainen, Diez, Sava, Javrh, Vrecer, Mihajlovic, Kecap, Zappaterra, Kornilow, Ebener and Operti2014; Schuller et al. Reference Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004).

Although the complex pathways linking learning participation and positive wellbeing and health require more study, Hammond (Reference Hammond2004) in the UK has provided a helpful conceptualisation of psychological resources as the mediators between adult learning and health outcomes. Based on her interviews (N = 145, aged 16–70), she found that learning helps adults develop the five psycho-social resources (i.e. ‘self-esteem and self-efficacy’, ‘identity’, ‘purpose and hope’, ‘competences and communication’ and ‘social integration’) that promote their general ‘wellbeing’, ‘mental health’ and ‘effective cop(ing) with change and adversity including ill health’ (Hammond Reference Hammond2004: 40). These findings clearly suggest that participating in adult learning courses functions as a compensatory strategy for adults to help them develop psychological and social reserve capacities.

Other researchers who have focused on later-life learning see this point as especially relevant to ‘non-formal’ and ‘non-credit’ types of learning. Narushima (Reference Narushima2008) conducted interviews (N = 15, aged 64–83) and observations of five different general-interest courses for older adults (calligraphy, sewing, Chinese poetry, folk dance and fitness) in a public continuing education programme in Canada. She found that weekly engagement in the course fuels older learners with intrinsic motivations to keep going physically and mentally, providing joy and satisfaction, a sense of self-growth and mastery, and friendships with classmates and teachers. As with Richardson, Grime and Ong (Reference Richardson, Grime and Ong2014) and Lassen (Reference Lassen2015), all interviewees in Narushima's (Reference Narushima2008) study had some chronic conditions, and consciously pursued their weekly classes and related learning practice at home as part of their health maintenance and self-management practice. In the UK, based on focus groups (N = 98), questionnaires (N = 77) and follow-up interviews (N = 35), Withnall (Reference Withnall2010) found many of her post-work participants regarded their learning in retirement as keeping their minds active, gaining new knowledge, relaxation and increased social contact.

More recent quantitative studies have further examined the association between participation in later-life learning and psychological wellbeing. Jenkins and his colleague examined the long-term effects of different types of lifelong learning on the psychological wellbeing of British older adults, following more than 3,000 older adults between 2002 and 2009 with data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (Jenkins Reference Jenkins2011; Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013, Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2015). They found that while higher levels of education are generally related to higher levels of wellbeing, without intervention there is a gradual decline in older adults’ psychological wellbeing over time, regardless of their level of education (Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013). However, they also found that, after controlling for socio-economic characteristics and other unobserved factors, participation in non-formal and non-credit type learning (e.g. music/arts/evening classes, sports club/exercise classes) was especially beneficial to maintain older adults’ psychological wellbeing. The participation in these non-formal learning courses raised older adults’ psychological wellbeing by between 4 and 11 per cent of a standard deviation, while formal education and training courses were not associated with higher wellbeing. The authors concluded that more informal, leisure-type courses were most likely to sustain older adults’ participation, consequently enhancing their wellbeing over time (Jenkins Reference Jenkins2011; Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013).

In a similar vein, Leung and Liu (Reference Leung and Liu2011) in China undertook a cross-sectional study to identify the relationship between participation in lifelong learning, quality of life and the self-efficacy of older learners (N = 1,003). While they found overall benefits involving older adults’ participation in learning and their quality of life, they also found that older learners who reported a higher level of quality of life were not those who enrolled in many courses at one time, but rather those who participated in a single course continuously. The authors conclude that what significantly influences the quality of life among older adults is not the amount, but the continuation of learning and the maintenance of self-efficacy.

Following these studies, Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp (Reference Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp2013a ) went back to the previously studied public continuing education programme (non-credit general-interest courses) in Canada (Narushima Reference Narushima2008) to undertake a cross-sectional survey with 699 older learners (aged 60 and older) enrolled in order to investigate whether what had been found in the previous qualitative study (Naruushima 2008) could be generalised programme wide. The results showed that older learners generally reported positive wellbeing and healthy lifestyles such as non-smoking and regular exercise. The result also indicated a positive association between the duration of participation and overall levels of wellbeing after controlling for key covariates such as socio-demographic characteristics and health-related factors (Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp Reference Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp2013b ).

These existing studies suggest the positive effects of continuous participation in lifelong learning programmes on psychological wellbeing. Nonetheless, they still leave some questions unanswered. In particular, when examining the effects of duration, selective attrition (i.e. those who feel better and healthy tend to remain in the programme) is one of the questions. Can older adults facing multiple risk factors/conditions keep participating in and benefiting from these lifelong learning programmes? The present study approaches this question by examining whether the positive association between duration of participation and older learners’ psychological wellbeing remains even after levels of vulnerability and multiple risks have been taken into consideration.

Analytical framework

Bringing together the old-age vulnerabilities framework and the benefits of lifelong learning, we developed a conceptual framework for analysis in this study (Figure 1).

Figure 1. The effects of lifelong learning on wellbeing, health and vulnerabilities in later life.

Our framework includes potential ‘threats’ and ‘outcomes’ (presented in dotted squares and arrows), following the old-age vulnerabilities framework (Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). In this study, however, we only focused on the associations between ‘risk factors/conditions’, ‘compensatory strategy’ and ‘coping capacity’, which were operationalised as the ‘level of vulnerability’, ‘continuous participation in a lifelong learning programme’ (i.e. duration) and ‘psychological wellbeing as a reserve capacity’.

Methodology

Sampling group

Older learners aged 60 and above, enrolled in a public non-formal continuing education programme during the fall of 2010, were targeted with a programme-wide survey. The programme was run by a local school board, which partnered with various community facilities such as community centres, adult learning centres, high schools and retirement homes. It offered non-credit general-interest courses in four subject areas: (a) arts and crafts, (b) fitness and exercise, (c) music and dance, and (d) language, computer and other practical skills. Four research assistants visited 158 classes and distributed a self-administered survey that asked for three types of information (i.e. basic demographic and health-related information, general psychological wellbeing and participation patterns). The questionnaire was completed at home by participants and returned the following week in class. Ethics clearance was received from the research ethics boards of the researchers’ university and the school board.

In total, 1,331 older learners were approached with 921 (69.20%) responding. Of the 921, 416 individuals provided consent forms with no missing data on the variables necessary for the models of analysis that were included for this study. Since participants were recruited from one of the largest public continuing education programmes in a large metropolis, we had a diverse sample group. We thus further divided the participants into three groups (i.e. high risk, middle risk and low risk) based on their socio-demographic, behavioural and health states.

Measurements

Psychological wellbeing

The older adults’ psychological wellbeing during the previous six months was measured by the 22-item Psychological General Well-Being Index (PGWBI) (Dupuy Reference Dupuy and Wenger1984, Reference Dupuy, Chassany, Dimenas, Dubois, Wu and Dupuy2004). The PGWBI is reliable and has been validated by numerous studies (Dupuy Reference Dupuy, Chassany, Dimenas, Dubois, Wu and Dupuy2004; Grossi et al. Reference Grossi, Groth, Mosconi, Cerutti, Pace, Compare and Apolone2006; McDowell Reference McDowell and McDowell2006; Nilsson et al. Reference Nilsson, Ohrvik, Lonnberg and Hedberg2011; Wu Reference Wu, Chassany, Dimenas, Dubois, Wu and Dupuy2004). It uses six dimensions to measure psychological wellbeing, including both positive and negative attributes: (a) depressive moods, (b) anxiety and stress, (c) self-control, (d) concerns about general health, (e) life satisfaction and (f) vitality (Dupuy Reference Dupuy, Chassany, Dimenas, Dubois, Wu and Dupuy2004; McDowell Reference McDowell and McDowell2006). Answers are assigned scores from 0 to 5, and then used to make dimensional scores as well as a global score. The global wellbeing score is divided into three levels: ‘severely distressed’ (0–60), ‘moderately distressed’ (61–72) and ‘positive wellbeing’ (73–110) – the higher the score the higher the level of wellbeing. In order to examine whether continued participation in a local public lifelong learning programme might help maintain older adults’ psychological wellbeing, we created a dichotomous variable by classifying the participants’ wellbeing status into two categories (i.e. ‘distressed’ when the global wellbeing score was <73, and ‘positive wellbeing’ when the score was 73 or higher). This variable was the dependent variable in logistic regression analysis.

Vulnerability level

In order to reflect a participant's vulnerability level, we created an index vulnerability level score by using information obtained from the survey. This index was comprised of age and indicators of three domains (i.e. health, socio-economic status and social support), with a higher index score reflecting a higher level of vulnerability or risk. Health indicators included whether the person had chronic conditions (e.g. diabetes, high blood pressure, arthritis), difficulties in daily living (e.g. walking, climbing stairs, hearing), and whether he or she regularly exercised for at least 20 minutes a day. Socio-economic status indicators included income and education level, while social support indicators included marital status, number of confidants, living arrangements and whether an individual participated in other social activities. An overall breakdown of how the vulnerability index variable was created is shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Vulnerability index variable composition

Note: 1. Includes any other group-organised activities such as clubs, associations, religious organisations, volunteering, recreational activities, etc.

Components of the index of the vulnerability level variable were chosen based on results from univariate analyses and the old-age vulnerabilities framework (Grundy Reference Grundy2006; Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006). Each explanatory variable comprising the index was given a score; these scores were then added together to give an overall score for each participant. The average vulnerability level score of participants was 5.18, while the median score was 5.0 (a higher score indicates higher vulnerability). Participants were then divided into three categorical groups based on their overall score: low risk (0–4; n = 175), middle risk (5 or 6; n = 129) and high risk (7 or higher; n = 112).

Self-rated health

In addition to the health indicators in the index of the vulnerability level, we also asked older adults to rate their general state of health by asking the question: ‘Compared with other people your age, how would you say your general health is?’ The choice of answers included: excellent, very good, good, fair and poor. The fair and poor groups were joined during analysis to ensure the size of each of the groups remained roughly similar.

Duration of learning

Older learners’ participation patterns, including learning duration, were gathered through the final section of the survey. The duration of class participation was examined by asking participants how many years and months they had taken the course. For the purpose of this study, those who had participated for three months or less (i.e. those in their first term) were excluded as they were still likely adjusting to the programme. The time of duration was then split into three groups: 4–18 months (short duration group), 19–48 months (middle duration group) and 49 months and longer (long duration group).

Statistical analysis

Chi-square, analysis of variance and t-tests were performed to examine whether there were differences in frequency distributions or averages. Potential confounders included gender, age, vulnerability level and self-perceived health. Two different logistic regression models were used to examine the association between duration and wellbeing. For both models, the dependent variable was the overall wellbeing status (1: the global PGWBI score ⩾73; 0: <73, higher values of PGWBI indicate better wellbeing) and the predictive variable was learning duration. The first model examined the association between duration and wellbeing, while taking into account the covariates of age, gender and vulnerability level. The second model included age and gender, but the vulnerability level was replaced with self-perceived health, since they were strongly correlated. Following logistic regression, trend analysis was performed for each model. All data were analysed using SAS 9.2.2 with the significant level set at equal or less than 0.05 on two-tailed tests.

Results

Table 2 shows the characteristics of each of the three duration groups. Of the 416 participants included in this study, the majority of participants in each duration group were female (74.5% in the short duration, 77.3% in the middle duration and 74.0% in the long duration group). Participants in each group had similar characteristics except that individuals in the long duration group were older and had a higher proportion positive wellbeing.

Table 2. Characteristics of participants by duration of class participation

Notes: SD: standard deviation. Sample size may vary for some variables due to missing data. 1. Missing values: Age: 8; Education: 2; Marital status: 1; Income level: 30; Living arrangement: 1; Confidants: 22; Regular exercise: 7.

The overall results of both logistic regression models can be seen in Tables 3 and 4. In Model 2, as seen in Table 3, those who had a higher level of vulnerability were more likely to be distressed than those with lower levels of vulnerability. Compared to those with vulnerability index scores of 0–4, the odds ratio (OR; 95% confidence interval (CI)) of being distressed was 3.2 (1.3–7.8, p = 0.0102) for those with index scores of 5–6 and 8.8 (3.7–20.9, p < 0.0001) for those with index scores of 7 or more. Similarly, Table 4 shows that an individual reported lower self-perceived health, the OR (95% CI) of being distressed was 3.8 (0.8–17.1, p = 0.0867), 11.9 (2.7–52.4, p = 0.0011), and 57.2 (11.0–296.7, p < .0001) for those in very good, good and fair/poor groups, respectively when compared to those with excellent health.

Table 3. Odds ratios of distressed (Model 1)

Table 4. Odds ratios of distressed (Model 2)

Figures 2 and 3 provide OR (and 95% CI) for the duration variable from the logistic regression analyses. Figure 2 shows the adjusted OR of being distressed by learning duration when taking into account age, gender and vulnerability index score; while Figure 3 shows the adjusted OR by learning duration when taking into account age, gender and self-perceived health.

Significance level: p < 0.03 (for trends).

Figure 2. Odds ratios and 95 per cent confidence intervals of distressed for different learning durations after adjusting for gender (female versus male), age (years) and risk group (5 or 6 risks and 7 or more risks versus 0–4 risks) (Model 1).

Significance level: p < 0.03 (for trends).

Figure 3. Odds ratios and 95 per cent confidence intervals of distressed for different learning durations after adjusting for gender (female versus male), age (years) and self-perceived health (very good, good and fair/poor versus excellent) (Model 2).

When examining the association between an older individual's learning duration and their general psychological wellbeing in comparison with those in the shortest duration group, the odds of being in the distressed category for those in the longest duration group decreased 60 per cent after adjusting for age, gender and overall vulnerability index score (OR = 0.4, 95% CI = 0.2–0.9, p = 0.02). Similar results were observed in Model 2 when self-perceived health was taken into account (p-values for trends in both models were less than 0.05).

Discussion

The results of this study indicated a clear association between continuous participation in the specific form of lifelong learning courses and the psychological wellbeing of older adults, even after controlling for age, gender, health state and vulnerabilities. The findings generally support the benefits of meaningful social participation for active ageing and enhanced quality of life in old age (Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Butler Reference Butler2002; Gilmour Reference Gilmour2012). More specifically, this study provides additional evidence for the benefits of lifelong learning on older adults’ psychological wellbeing and health (Dench and Regan Reference Dench and Regan2000; Jenkins Reference Jenkins2011; Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013, Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2015; Leung and Liu Reference Leung and Liu2011; Narushima Reference Narushima2008; Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp Reference Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp2013a ; Preece and Findsen Reference Preece, Findsen, Robinson, Novelli, Pearson and Norris2007; Withnall Reference Withnall2010).

The results showed that the longer older adults remained on one course or pursued the same subject, the better psychological wellbeing they reported, even after all key covariates were taken into account. Continuous and ongoing participation in lifelong learning programmes contribute to the conservation of older learners’ psychological wellbeing over time. This point is noteworthy from the perspective of old-age vulnerability, as it confirms what Grundy (Reference Grundy2006: 108) articulated as the ‘very important effect of current circumstances and behaviour’ on the wellbeing of older adults.

In fact, an unexpected finding of our study is that educational level did not play a significant role in influencing the level of wellbeing in either of the two logistic regression models, despite the accumulated evidence of the lifelong effects of formal education for an individual's wellbeing and health (Mirowsky and Ross Reference Mirowsky and Ross2005, Reference Mirowsky and Ross2008). One possible reason might be that both the direct and indirect effects of schooling at a young age on health and wellbeing change across the lifecourse. A study found that the schooling–income effect increases until age 60, plateaus and then declines after age 80 (Lynch Reference Lynch2006). Another study found that average health–literacy scores consistently fall as people age, regardless of their socio-economic status (Will, Douglas and Murray 2007, cited in Murray et al. Reference Murray, Hagey, Willms, Shillington and Desjardins2008). The aforementioned longitudinal study of British older adults (Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013) also found that one's psychological wellbeing gradually and equally declines over the years regardless of one's educational background. Bringing together all these studies, one can say that, in order to sustain psychological wellbeing – an important reserve capacity – and avoid the negative consequences of vulnerabilities in old age, older people need to take some action to compensate for their decline, regardless of their degree of vulnerability. The result of the present study corroborates that the continuous participation in a lifelong learning course is one effective compensatory strategy.

In addition, the results of this study highlighted the protective effect of a specific form of lifelong learning for older adults, particularly those most vulnerable. In contrast to our hypothesis that the longer the participation, the lower the vulnerability levels due to selective attrition, nearly one-third of our participants in the longest duration group fell into the ‘high-risk’ category (i.e. with more than seven risk factors/conditions). Yet, members of that group reported equally increased psychological wellbeing regardless of that vulnerability. This is probably because their local, non-formal and non-credit leisure and recreational type of lifelong learning programme, with its supportive environment, can enable even the most vulnerable and frail older adults to continue to participate in their active ageing lifestyles at their own pace (Lassen Reference Lassen2015; Richardson, Grime and Ong Reference Richardson, Grime and Ong2014). This in turn helps restore their psychological reserves including wellbeing, motivating their participation and maintaining their coping capacity, while mitigating the effects of their risk conditions (Grundy Reference Grundy2006; Hammond Reference Hammond2004; Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti Reference Schroder-Butterfill and Marianti2006).

Aligned with other studies that take a more inclusive and broader view of active ageing (Boudiny Reference Boudiny2013; Lassen Reference Lassen2015; Richardson, Grime and Ong Reference Richardson, Grime and Ong2014), participants in this study also demonstrated that physical health is not a prerequisite for an active lifestyle in old age. Nonetheless, given the empirical link between a higher level of wellbeing and better physical function and health (Andrews Reference Andrews2001; Anstey et al. Reference Anstey, Burns, von Sanden and Luszez2008; Pressman and Cohen Reference Pressman and Cohen2005; Ryff et al. Reference Ryff, Love, Urry, Muller, Rosenkranz, Friedman, Davidson and Singer2006), and, conversely, higher levels of ill-being as a predictor of increased functional disability and mortality (Cuijpers, de Graaf and van Dorsselaer Reference Cuijpers, de Graaf and van Dorsselaer2004; Howell Reference Howell2009; Iwasa et al. Reference Iwasa, Kawaai, Gondo, Inagaki and Suzuki2006; Ryff et al. Reference Ryff, Love, Urry, Muller, Rosenkranz, Friedman, Davidson and Singer2006), it is quite possible that continuous engagement in later-life learning activities can create a virtuous cycle to prevent or slow down the onset of diseases and physical deterioration. This may also be why more vulnerable older adults with poorer health tend to report higher perceived benefits of and appreciation for lifelong learning programmes on their wellbeing and health (Dench and Regan Reference Dench and Regan2000; Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp Reference Narushima, Liu and Diestelkamp2013b ; Manninen et al. Reference Manninen, Sgier, Fleige, Thone-Geyer, Kil, Mozina, Danihelkova, Mallows, Duncan, Merilainen, Diez, Sava, Javrh, Vrecer, Mihajlovic, Kecap, Zappaterra, Kornilow, Ebener and Operti2014).

Nevertheless, the results of our study do not necessarily mean that all types of lifelong learning have a similar effect on older adults’ psychological wellbeing. Consistent with what previous studies have found (Jenkins Reference Jenkins2011; Jenkins and Mostafa Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2013, Reference Jenkins and Mostafa2015; Manninen et al. Reference Manninen, Sgier, Fleige, Thone-Geyer, Kil, Mozina, Danihelkova, Mallows, Duncan, Merilainen, Diez, Sava, Javrh, Vrecer, Mihajlovic, Kecap, Zappaterra, Kornilow, Ebener and Operti2014), our study showed the positive effects of non-formal and non-credit lifelong learning courses – often undervalued as leisure and recreation – on the maintenance of older adults psychological wellbeing. To be clear, the lifelong learning programme examined in this study is a publicly funded, non-formal and non-credit programme offering diverse general-interest subjects. It is also worth mentioning that all classes are easily accessible by public transportation, and that the school board subsidises fees for low-income seniors. This equity-oriented programme environment makes it easier for those with some physical and mental health conditions or lower incomes to continue to pursue their personal interests in their local communities.

Clearly, more research is needed to investigate how and why continuous participation in lifelong learning matters so much in later life. Nonetheless, combining the results of our study with the existing literature, we speculate that participation in community-based non-formal lifelong learning plays different roles at two levels. At the individual level, psychological qualities such as ‘self-esteem’, ‘self-efficacy’ and ‘resilience’ (Hammond Reference Hammond2004) – immediate and direct benefits of learning – may need to be more frequently reinforced in late adulthood to sustain an individual's psychological wellbeing, due to the increased need to cope with physical, material and social changes. Later-life development is a continuous process of behavioural and psychological adaptation, the regulation of one's resources through the process of ‘selection, optimisation and compensation’ (SOC) (Baltes and Baltes Reference Baltes, Baltes, Baltes and Baltes1990; Freund and Baltes Reference Freund and Baltes1998). According to this theory, those who successfully select valued activities continue to find compensatory strategies, optimise desired outcomes, and have higher levels of emotional wellbeing and life satisfaction. Older learners who stick with their subject of interest over the years may be going through a similar process as the SOC, thereby obtaining a sense of continuity and satisfaction that compensates for the changes and consequent worries and anxieties they face in their daily lives.

At the social level, continuous participation on the same course also helps older adults develop connections and a stronger sense of community with fellow students and instructors, providing them with informal social support (Dench and Regan Reference Dench and Regan2000; Narushima Reference Narushima2008; Schuller et al. Reference Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004; Withnall Reference Withnall2010). The results of this study showed that there was no difference in the number of confidants despite the higher average age of the longer duration group, and the fact that the number of one's confidants and friends decreases when one gets older. As this result suggested, staying in a single programme helps older adults build and maintain an informal social support network. While not the main focus of our investigation, this result nevertheless suggests that long-term participation in community-based non-formal lifelong learning is not only of benefit for psychological wellbeing at the individual level, it also provides a ‘social fabric’ or ‘a collective environment that is conducive to sustaining health’ as advocated by Schuller et al. (Reference Schuller, Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004: 27).

Given our goal to realise ageing in place through creating age-friendly communities within an active ageing framework (WHO 2002), this indirect benefit at the social level should be considered as an equally important aspect of the ‘conserving effects’ of community-based non-formal lifelong learning (Schuller Reference Schuller, Schuller, Preston, Hammond, Brassett-Grundy and Bynner2004).

Conclusion

Given the limitations of cross-sectional study design and the general methodological challenges involved in assessing the impact of lifelong learning on psychological wellbeing with its many possible confounding variables, we were unable to determine causation. Furthermore, the problem of selective attrition was not completely solved despite our creation of the three vulnerability levels, because those who participated in this survey may have had higher levels of wellbeing than those who did not. Moreover, those enrolled in this continuing education programme, to take full advantage of its benefits, may have had higher levels of wellbeing at the outset. Organised learning and educational activities may cause stress and anxiety for some older adults due to their negative experiences in formal education (Field Reference Field2009), so that such people may not participate in the programme to begin with. In addition, this study encompassed people on non-credit courses only, so no comparison can be made with those older people who take credit courses but are not engaged in lifelong learning activities.

Despite these limitations, the findings of this study signify that continuous and ongoing participation in a community-based non-formal lifelong learning programme may work as a compensatory strategy for older adults by helping them sustain their psychological wellbeing – an important source of support and protection for autonomy and self-management in later life. Continuing engagement in activities and relationships that they value can help older people focus on wellness rather than illness, despite chronic conditions and other challenges in later life. The role of later-life learning should be understood as far more than merely instrumental. Nevertheless, its conserving effect on wellbeing and health underscores the need for more strategic and unequivocal policies and practices to promote lifelong learning among older adults as a vital part of the active ageing framework.

Unfortunately, personal and structural barriers prevent many older adults from taking advantage of these benefits. Participation in adult learning and education is unevenly distributed between countries and within a given country. For example, in OECD countries, the participation of more educated adults is three to five times higher than those with less education – the more vulnerable group (Desjardins Reference Desjardins2015). In addition, participation in lifelong learning (both formal and non-formal) drastically decreases with age. A survey of 14 European countries found that the average participation rate in education or training courses among those 60–69 years old was 7 per cent, of those 70 years and older only 3 percent (Kolland and Wanka Reference Kolland, Wanka, Field, Burke and Cooper2014). Nevertheless, the same research also suggests that the participation rate varies hugely even between countries, and that Nordic countries with equity-oriented public policies have much higher participation rates (Kolland and Wanka Reference Kolland, Wanka, Field, Burke and Cooper2014).

Given the lifelong learning divide, the decrease in participation in learning activities in later life and the ongoing budget cuts for adult learning in many countries, it is crucial to promote the benefits of lifelong learning to allow all older adults – especially those whose poor educational, financial, social or health background makes them more vulnerable – to take advantage of this effective compensatory strategy. In order to develop more inclusive and equitable active ageing policies and practices, Boudiny (Reference Boudiny2013: 1088–93) advocates three additional principles: ‘fostering adaptability’, ‘supporting the maintenance of emotionally close relationships’ and ‘removing structural barriers related to age or dependency’. Publicly subsidised non-credit general-interest lifelong learning programmes, like the programme examined in this study, can play a vital role to help realise these principles. Policy makers at all level of governments should take a fresh look at lifelong learning programmes as a necessary and cost-effective strategy for promoting wellbeing and health, and develop more affordable, accessible and diverse later-life learning programmes to achieve the goal of active ageing in place for all. Creating an active and caring ageing society, after all, is the responsibility of both individuals and governments.

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank all participants who kindly took time to share their information and insights, and the school board staff and instructors for their co-operation during the data-collection period. Our special thanks go to the three research assistants (Iris Hartwig, Joycelyn Afrifa and Jenn Braisford) who carried out the data collection with energy and tact. We also express our gratitude to the anonymous reviewers whose constructive feedback helped us strengthen this article. This study was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant number 410-009-2255). There is no conflict of interest involved in this research.

References

Adams, B. K., Leibbrandt, S. and Moon, H. 2011. A critical review of the literature on social and leisure activity and wellbeing in later life. Ageing & Society, 31, 4, 683712.Google Scholar
Andrews, G. R. 2001. Promoting health and function in an ageing population. British Medical Journal, 322, 7288, 728–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anstey, K. J., Burns, R., von Sanden, C. and Luszez, M. A. 2008. Psychological well-being is an independent predictor of falling in an 8-year follow-up of older adults. Journal of Gerontology Social Sciences, 63, 4, 249–57.Google Scholar
Bailey, N., Breen, J. and Ward, M. 2010. Community Education: More than Just a Course – Exploring the Outcomes and Impact of Department of Education and Skills Funded Community Education. AONTAS, Dublin.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. and Baltes, N. N. 1990. Psychological perspectives on successful aging: the model of selective optimization with compensation. In Baltes, P. B. and Baltes, M. M. (eds), Successful Aging: Perspectives from the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge University Press, New York, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boudiny, K. 2013. ‘Active ageing’: from empty rhetoric to effective policy tool. Ageing & Society, 33, 6, 1077–98.Google Scholar
Bowling, A. and Iliffe, S. 2011. Psychological approach to successful ageing predicts future quality of life in older adults. Health and Quality of Life Outcome, 9, 1, 1322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, R. N. 2002. The study of productive aging. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Sciences, 57B, 6, S323.Google Scholar
Canadian Institute for Health Information 2011. Canada: Seniors and Health Care System: What is the Impact of Multiple Chronic Conditions? Available online at https://secure.cihi.ca/free_products/air-chronic_disease_aib_en.pdf. [Accessed on June 7, 2013].Google Scholar
Cuijpers, P., de Graaf, R. and van Dorsselaer, S. 2004. Minor depression: risk profiles, functional disability, health care use and risk of developing major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 79, 1, 71–9.Google Scholar
Dench, S. and Regan, J. 2000. Learning in later life: motivation and impact. DfEE Research Report RR183, Department for Education and Skills, Nottingham, UK.Google Scholar
Desjardins, R. 2015. Participation in Adult Education Opportunities: Evidence from PIAAC and Policy Trends in Selected Countries. Background Paper for the Education for All Global Monitoring Report 2015. UNESCO. Available online at unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0023/002323/232396e.pdf. [Accessed on August 23, 2015].Google Scholar
Diener, Ed. 2006. Guidelines for national indicators of subjective well-being and ill-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 4, 397404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, P., Fujiwara, D. and Metcalfe, R. 2012. Review and Update of Research into the Wider Benefits of Adult Learning. BIS Research Paper 20, Department for Business, Innovation & Skills, London. Available online at https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/34671/12-1243-review-wider-benefits-of-adult-learning.pdf. [Accessed on July 15, 2015].Google Scholar
Dupuy, H. 1984. The psychological general well-being (PGWB) index. In Wenger, N. K. (ed.), Assessment of Quality of Life in Clinical Trials of Cardiovascular Therapies. LeJacq, New York, 170–83.Google Scholar
Dupuy, H. J. 2004. Appendix B: Historical background. In Chassany, O., Dimenas, E., Dubois, D., Wu, A. and Dupuy, H. J. (eds), The Psychological General Well-Being Index (PGWBI) User Manual. MAPI Research Institute, Lyon, France, 4250.Google Scholar
Federation of Canadian Municipalities 2013. Canada's Aging Population: Municipal Roles in Canada's Demographic Shift. Available online at https://www.fcm.ca/Documents/reports/Canadas_Aging_Population_The_Municipal_Role_in_Canadas_Demographic_Shift_EN.pdf. [Accessed on February 15, 2015].Google Scholar
Feinstein, L., Budge, D.,Vorhaus, J. and Duckworth, K. (eds) 2008. The Social and Personal Benefits of Learning: A Summary of Key Research Findings. Centre for Research on the Wider Benefits of Learning, Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
Feinstein, L. and Hammond, C. 2004. The contribution of adult learning to health and social capital. Oxford Review of Education, 30, 2, 119221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, J. 2009. Well-being and happiness. Inquiry into the future for lifelong learning. Thematic Paper 4, National Institute of Adult Continuing Education, Leicester, UK.Google Scholar
Field, J. 2011. Adult learning, health and well-being – changing lives. Adult Learner (0790-8084), 13–25.Google Scholar
Freund, A. M. and Baltes, P. B. 1998. Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: correlations with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13, 4, 531–43.Google Scholar
Gilmour, H. 2012. Social participation and the health and well-being of Canadian seniors. Statistics Canada Health Reports, 23, 3, 312.Google Scholar
Government of British Colombia n.d. Active Aging. Available online at http://www2.gov.bc.ca/gov/content/family-social-supports/seniors/health-safety/active-aging. [Accessed on January 25, 2016].Google Scholar
Grossi, E., Groth, N., Mosconi, P., Cerutti, R., Pace, F., Compare, A. and Apolone, G. 2006. Development and validation of the short version of the Psychological General Well-Being Index (PGWB-S). Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 4, 1, 8895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grundy, E. 2006. Ageing and vulnerable elderly people: European perspectives. Ageing & Society, 26, 1, 105–34.Google Scholar
Hammond, C. 2004. Impacts of lifelong learning upon emotion resilience, psychological and mental health: fieldwork evidence. Oxford Review of Education, 30, 4, 551–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, C. and Feinstein, L. 2006. Are those who flourished at school healthier adults? What role for adult education? Wider Benefit of Learning Research Report 17, Centre for Research on the Wider Benefit of Learning, London.Google Scholar
Havighurst, R. J. 1961. Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 1, 1, 813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, R. T. 2009. Review: positive psychological well-being reduces the risk of mortality in both ill and healthy populations. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 12, 2, 41.Google Scholar
Iwasa, H., Kawaai, C., Gondo, Y., Inagaki, H. and Suzuki, T. 2006. Subjective well-being as a predictor of all-cause mortality among middle-aged and elderly people living in an urban Japanese community: a seven-year prospective cohort study. Geriatric Gerontology International, 6, 4, 216–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, A. and Mostafa, T. 2013. Learning and Wellbeing Trajectories among Older Adults in England. Working Paper. No. 13-02. Institute of Education, University of London. Available online at repec.ioe.ac.uk/REPEc/pdf/qsswp1302.pdf [Accessed on January 25, 2014].Google Scholar
Jenkins, A. 2011. Participation in learning and wellbeing among older adults. International Journal of Lifelong Education, 30, 3, 403–20.Google Scholar
Jenkins, A. and Mostafa, T. 2015. The effects of learning on wellbeing for older adults in England. Ageing & Society, 35, 10, 2053–70.Google Scholar
Kolland, F. and Wanka, A. 2014. Learning in later life. In Field, J., Burke, R. J. and Cooper, C. L. (eds), The Sage Handbook of Aging, Work and Society. Sage, Los Angeles, 380400.Google Scholar
Lassen, A. J. 2015. Keeping disease at arm's length – how older Danish people distance disease through active ageing. Ageing & Society, 35, 7, 1364–83.Google Scholar
Leung, D. S. Y. and Liu, B. C. P. 2011. Lifelong education, quality of life and self-efficacy of Chinese older adults. Educational Gerontology, 37, 11, 967–81.Google Scholar
Lynch, S. M. 2006. Explaining life course and cohort variation in the relationship between education and health: the role of income. Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 47, 4, 324–38.Google Scholar
Manninen, J., Sgier, I., Fleige, M., Thone-Geyer, B., Kil, M., Mozina, E., Danihelkova, H., Mallows, D., Duncan, S., Merilainen, M., Diez, J., Sava, S., Javrh, P., Vrecer, N., Mihajlovic, D., Kecap, E., Zappaterra, P., Kornilow, A., Ebener, R. and Operti, F. 2014. Benefits of Lifelong Learning in Europe: Main Results of the BeLL Project: Research Report. Available online at http://www.alice.ch/fileadmin/user_upload/alicech/dokumente/sveb/projekte/BELL_RESEARCH_REPORT_310514_Final.pdf. [Accessed on July 7, 2015].Google Scholar
Mayhew, L. 2005. Active ageing in the UK – issues, barriers, policy directions. Innovation: The European Journal of Social Science Research, 18, 4, 455–77.Google Scholar
McDowell, I. 2006. The general well-being schedule. In McDowell, I. (ed.), Measuring Health: A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires. Third edition, Oxford University Press, New York, 240–6.Google Scholar
Mirowsky, J. and Ross, C. E. 2005. Education, cumulative advantage and health. Ageing International, 30, 1, 2762.Google Scholar
Mirowsky, J. and Ross, C. 2008. Education and self-rated health: cumulative advantage and its rising importance. Research on Aging, 30, 1, 93122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Momtaz, Y. A., Ibrahim, R., Hamid, T. A. and Yahaya, N. 2011. Sociodemographic predictors of elderly's psychological well-being in Malaysia. Aging & Mental Health, 15, 4, 437–45.Google Scholar
Murray, S., Hagey, J., Willms, D., Shillington, R. and Desjardins, R. 2008. Health Literacy in Canada: A Healthy Understanding. Canadian Council of Learning, Ottawa.Google Scholar
Narushima, M. 2008. More than nickels and dimes: the health benefits of a community-based lifelong learning program for older adults. International Journal of Lifelong Education, 27, 6, 673–92.Google Scholar
Narushima, M., Liu, J. and Diestelkamp, N. 2013 a. The association between lifelong learning and psychological well-being among older adults: implications for an interdisciplinary health promotion in an aging society. Activities, Adaptation &Aging, 37, 3, 239–50.Google Scholar
Narushima, M., Liu, J. and Diestelkamp, N. 2013 b. Motivations and perceived benefits of older learners in a public continuing education program: influence of gender, income, and health. Education Gerontology, 39, 8, 569–84.Google Scholar
National Seniors Strategy n.d. Ensure that Older Canadians Do Not Live in Poverty. Available online at http://www.nationalseniorsstrategy.ca/the-four-pillars/pillar-1/older-canadians-and-poverty/. [Accessed on March 3, 2015].Google Scholar
Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J. and Tobin, S. 1968. Personality and patterns of aging. In Neugarten, B. L. (ed.), Middle Age and Aging. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 173–7.Google Scholar
Nilsson, G., Ohrvik, J., Lonnberg, I. and Hedberg, P. 2011. Low psychological general well-being (PGWB) is associated with deteriorated 10-year survival in men but not in women among the elderly. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 52, 2, 167–71.Google Scholar
Olsson, L. A., Hagnelius, N.-O., Olsson, H. and Nilsson, T. K. 2013. Subjective well-being in Swedish active seniors or seniors with cognitive complaints and its relation to commonly available biomarkers. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 56, 2, 303–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) 2013. OECD Guidelines on Measuring Subjective Well-being. Available online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264191655-en. [Accessed on June 10, 2015].Google Scholar
Oxley, J. 2009. Policies for healthy ageing: an overview. OECD Health Working Papers 24, Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, Paris.Google Scholar
Preece, J. and Findsen, B. 2007. Keeping people active: continuing education programs that work. In Robinson, M., Novelli, W., Pearson, C. and Norris, L. (eds), Global Health & Global Aging. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, 313–22.Google Scholar
Pressman, S. D. and Cohen, S. 2005. Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 6, 925–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, J. C., Grime, J. C. and Ong, B. N. 2014. ‘Keeping going’: chronic joint pain in older people who describe their health as good. Ageing & Society, 34, 8, 1380–96.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. and Deci, E. L. 2000. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 1, 6878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryff, C. D., Love, G. D., Urry, H. L., Muller, D., Rosenkranz, M. A., Friedman, E. M., Davidson, R. J. and Singer, B. 2006. Psychological well-being and ill-being: do they have distinct or mirrored biological correlates? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 2, 8595.Google Scholar
Schroder-Butterfill, E. and Marianti, R. 2006. A framework for understanding old-age vulnerabilities. Ageing & Society, 26, 1, 935.Google Scholar
Schuller, T. 2004. Three capitals. In Schuller, T., Preston, J., Hammond, C., Brassett-Grundy, A. and Bynner, J. (eds), The Benefit of Learning. Routledge Falmer, London, 1234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuller, T., Preston, J., Hammond, C., Brassett-Grundy, A. and Bynner, J. (eds) 2004. The Benefit of Learning. Routledge Falmer, London.Google Scholar
Statistics Canada 2012 a. The Canadian Population in 2011: Age and Sex. Available online at http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2011/as-sa/98-311-x/98-311-x2011001-eng.pdf. [Accessed on March 21, 2013].Google Scholar
Statistics Canada 2012 b. Living Arrangements of Seniors: Families, Households and Marital Status, Structural Type of Dwelling and Collectives. 2011 Census of Population. Census in Brief. Catalogue number 98-312-X2011003. Available online at http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2011/as-sa/98-312-x/98-312-x2011003_4-eng.pdf. [Accessed on November 4, 2013].Google Scholar
Statistics Canada 2016. The Contribution of Immigration to the Size and Ethnocultural Diversity of Future Cohort of Seniors. Catalogue number 75-006-X. Available online at http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/75-006-x/2016001/article/14345-eng.htm. [Accessed on July 7, 2016].Google Scholar
Turcotte, M. and Schellenberg, G. 2007. A Portrait of Seniors in Canada. Statistics Canada, Ottawa. Available online at http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/89-519-x/89-519-x2006001-eng.pdf. [Accessed on April 1, 2008]Google Scholar
Vanhoutte, B. 2014. The measurement of multiple dimensions of subjective well-being in later life. In Field, J., Burke, R. J. and Cooper, C. L. (eds), The Sage Handbook of Aging, Work and Society. Sage, Los Angeles, 493513.Google Scholar
Walker, A. 2002. Strategy for active ageing. International Social Security Review, 55, 1, 121–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, A. 2006. Active ageing in employment: its meaning and potential. Asia Pacific Review, 13, 1, 7893.Google Scholar
Withnall, A. 2010. Improving Learning in Later Life. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO) 2002. Active Aging: A Policy Framework. Available online at http://whqlibdoc.who.int/hq/2002/who_nmh_nph_02.8.pdf. [Accessed on May 23, 2009].Google Scholar
Wu, A. W. 2004. Introduction. In Chassany, O, Dimenas, E., Dubois, D., Wu, A. and Dupuy, H. L. (eds), The Psychological General Well-Being Index (PGWBI) User Manual. MAPI Research Institute, Lyon, France, 910.Google Scholar
Zarit, S. H. 2009. A good old age: theories of mental health and aging. In Bengtson, V. L., Gans, D., Putney, N. M. and Silverstein, M. (eds), Handbook of Theories of Aging. Second edition, Springer, New York, 675–91.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The effects of lifelong learning on wellbeing, health and vulnerabilities in later life.

Figure 1

Table 1. Vulnerability index variable composition

Figure 2

Table 2. Characteristics of participants by duration of class participation

Figure 3

Table 3. Odds ratios of distressed (Model 1)

Figure 4

Table 4. Odds ratios of distressed (Model 2)

Figure 5

Figure 2. Odds ratios and 95 per cent confidence intervals of distressed for different learning durations after adjusting for gender (female versus male), age (years) and risk group (5 or 6 risks and 7 or more risks versus 0–4 risks) (Model 1).

Significance level:p 
Figure 6

Figure 3. Odds ratios and 95 per cent confidence intervals of distressed for different learning durations after adjusting for gender (female versus male), age (years) and self-perceived health (very good, good and fair/poor versus excellent) (Model 2).

Significance level:p