Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:58:07.306Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Expressions of self-ageism in four European countries: a comparative analysis of predictors across cultural contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 December 2020

Belia Schuurman
Affiliation:
Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing, Leiden, The Netherlands Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands
Jolanda Lindenberg*
Affiliation:
Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing, Leiden, The Netherlands Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands
Johanna M. Huijg
Affiliation:
Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing, Leiden, The Netherlands
Wilco P. Achterberg
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands
Joris P. J. Slaets
Affiliation:
Center for Geriatric Medicine, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Self-ageism has a significant negative impact on older people's ageing experiences and health outcomes. Despite ample evidence on cross-cultural ageism, studies have rarely looked into the way cultural contexts affect self-ageism. In this article, we compare expressions of self-ageism and its possible predictors across four European countries based on two questionnaires in a study sample of 2,494 individuals aged 55 and older. We explore how predictors of self-ageism are moderated by cultural values in a comparative fixed-effects regression model. We empirically show that similarly to ageism, self-ageism is not present in the same way and to the same extent in every country. Moreover, the level to which cultures value hierarchy and intellectual autonomy significantly moderates the association between self-ageism and individual predictors of self-ageism. Our study adds to the small existing body of work on self-ageism by confirming empirically that certain expressions of self-ageism and individual predictors are susceptible to change in different cultural contexts. Our research results suggest that self-ageism interventions may benefit from a culturally sensitive approach and imply that more culturally diverse comparisons of self-ageism are necessary to figure out fitting ways to reduce self-ageism.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction and rationale

In the broader characterisation of the term, ageism is defined as prejudice against people on the basis of their age (Palmore, Reference Palmore1990). In the context of this paper, we discuss and define ageism as stereotypes, attitudes, and prejudice specifically regarding older people (Butler, Reference Butler1969, Reference Butler1975), which appears to be common and widespread (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003; Angus and Reeve, Reference Angus and Reeve2006; Butler, Reference Butler2009). Stereotypes underlying ageism are not straightforwardly negative but rather multi-dimensional. Evaluatively mixed stereotypes of older people as incompetent and unproductive yet warm and wise appear to be especially pervasive, also in different cultural contexts (Cuddy and Fiske, Reference Cuddy, Fiske and Nelson2002; Cuddy et al., Reference Cuddy, Norton and Fiske2005). Such ambivalent stereotyping creates problematic ageist expectations and even avoidance of older people, thus contributing to their exclusion and discrimination in everyday life (Palmore et al., Reference Palmore, Branch and Harris2005).

Prejudice against older people is not restricted to younger generations; older people themselves are also known to hold ageist attitudes towards older people: a phenomenon designated as ‘self-ageism’ (Bodner, Reference Bodner2009). Unfortunately, research into self-ageism of older people is more scarce than research about ageism in the general population or amongst younger generations. Whilst ageism appears to be pervasive across different cultural and national contexts (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003; Löckenhoff et al., Reference Löckenhoff, De Fruyt, Terracciano, McCrae, De Bolle and Yik2009; Swift et al., Reference Swift, Abrams, Marques, Vauclair, Bratt, Lima, Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2018), self-ageism is rarely studied in a cross-cultural context, even though culturally shared social contexts are arguably important in shaping people's perceptions of age and their ageing selves (Swift et al., Reference Swift, Abrams, Marques, Vauclair, Bratt, Lima, Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2018). There are still many questions open when it comes to self-ageism amongst older people, especially regarding its possible factors of influence and its variation in different cultural contexts. More insights into the processes and manifestations of self-ageism are needed to find fitting ways in which self-ageism and its harmful outcomes might be reduced. In this article, we address this gap in knowledge and investigate self-ageism in a cross-cultural context.

Underlying mechanisms of self-ageism

There is mixed evidence on the underlying mechanisms of self-ageism in older people. Self-ageism might be the result of internalisation of prejudice (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003), as proposed in the embodiment theory of lifelong internalised ageist stereotypes (Levy, Reference Levy2009). When viewed through the lens of social identity theory (Tajfel, Reference Tajfel1981), self-ageism could also be connected to older people's negative perceptions of their own age group and its social status. The latter could be explained by social identity mechanisms in which older people distance themselves from other older people as a so-called ‘inner outgroup’ (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Bodner, Reference Bodner2009). Studies have found similarities as well as differences in ageism among older people compared to ageism in younger people, with perceptions of social roles and identity appearing to be much more likely to be related to ageist attitudes in older people (social identity/role theory), than explained by the widely researched Terror Management Theory in which death anxiety results in ageist attitudes (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Bodner, Reference Bodner2009; Lev et al., Reference Lev, Wurm, Ayalon, Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2018).

Regardless of the underlying mechanisms of self-ageism, there is ample evidence on its significant negative role in older people's ageing experiences and health outcomes (Minichiello et al., Reference Minichiello, Browne and Kendig2000; Levy et al., Reference Levy, Ryall, Pilver, Sheridan, Wei and Hausdorff2008, Reference Levy, Ferrucci, Zonderman, Slade, Troncoso and Resnick2016; Westerhof and Wurm, Reference Westerhof and Wurm2015). When negative ageist attitudes are held by older persons themselves, the confrontation with age-based discrimination might prime negative self-stereotyping and corresponding stereotypical behaviour, adversely influencing older persons’ mental and physical health (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Whitbourne and Sneed, Reference Whitbourne, Sneed and Nelson2002; Levy, Reference Levy2003). In this regard, self-ageism could function as a self-fulfilling prophecy through which ageist expectations might be confirmed and ageist attitudes increased, both in the affected older persons and in society at large. Much of the rather sparse research on self-ageism appears to focus on these effects. Lower levels of ageist attitudes may turn this perpetual mechanism of self-ageism around and allow older people to have a more positive ageing experience, which in turn could contribute to more positive attitudes towards the ageing self and others. A first study evidencing this mechanism in self-ageism was conducted by Bodner and Cohen-Fridel (Reference Bodner and Cohen-Fridel2010). They looked into explanations of various affective dimensions of self-ageism and found that older people's ability to relate positively to themselves as well as to other people decreased their ageism.

Self-ageism in cultural context

Cultural values, to the extent that they are shared in a society, provide the cultural context in which individual attitudes and personal experiences exist. In different cultural contexts, individual attitudes might be evaluated more positively or negatively, depending on the extent to which they are supported and enabled by society. Individual attitudes are impacted and structured by social identity processes (Smith and Hogg, Reference Smith and Hogg2008), including social identity processes that are theorised to influence self-ageism (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Bodner, Reference Bodner2009; Lev et al., Reference Lev, Wurm, Ayalon, Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2018). We start from the idea that older people's positive ageing experiences and more positive attitudes towards themselves as well as others might decrease their self-ageism (Bodner and Cohen-Fridel, Reference Bodner and Cohen-Fridel2010); an effect which could be explained by theorising that more positive experiences and perceptions of older people's social identity and social roles might lead to less ageism (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Bodner, Reference Bodner2009). Our expectation, based on the earlier studies on cross-cultural ageism, is that cultural contexts might have a moderating effect on individual attitudes that influence self-ageism. Our analytical presumptions are directed towards how likely positive attitudes towards oneself, expressed in individual attitudes – (self-)confidence in daily life, appreciation of intergenerational contact and the need for control over important aspects of one's life – are evaluated differently in different cultural contexts therewith influencing self-ageism.

This study uses survey data to measure self-ageism of older people in four European countries (France, Poland, Portugal and the Netherlands) with distinct societal features with regards to ageing, such as life expectancy, secularism and retirement age. To follow up on previously conducted cross-cultural studies on ageism and to explore whether self-ageism is also prone to cross-cultural variation, we included individual attitudes as well as country-level cultural values (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003; Cuddy et al., Reference Cuddy, Norton and Fiske2005; Löckenhoff et al., Reference Löckenhoff, De Fruyt, Terracciano, McCrae, De Bolle and Yik2009; North and Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2015; Swift et al., Reference Swift, Abrams, Marques, Vauclair, Bratt, Lima, Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2018).

Research design and methods

Survey

The data we use in our study are part of a larger survey that was originally conducted by the Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing in 2013, with support of the Medical Delta. In 2016, with the support of EIT Health, additional surveys were held in France, Poland and Portugal, with the aim of covering culturally and demographically distinct regions in the European Union while taking into account limited resources. The survey was informed by focus groups with older people and explicitly developed to capture the perspectives of older people aged 55 and over. To ensure face and content validity of the survey items, these questions were based on the statements and wordings of those participating in the focus groups (see Lindenberg and Westendorp, Reference Lindenberg and Westendorp2015; Huijg et al., Reference Huijg, van Delden, Ouderaa, Westendorp, Slaets and Lindenberg2016). It included a total of 95 questions which mainly focused on wishes, ambitions and attitudes of older people, among which two questionnaires were dedicated to ageism. The entire survey was translated and then cross-checked by professional translators and native-speaking social scientists in the respective countries. All survey respondents were sampled from research panel participants. The survey was administered digitally, except for participants of 75 years and older in Poland and Portugal, who were surveyed by phone following the recommendations of local social scientists, in order to ensure a sufficient number of respondents aged 75 and older in these countries. The different ways of administering the survey are regarded to be of no influence on the quality of data collection (Schillewaert and Meulemeester, Reference Schillewaert and Meulemeester2005). The number of respondents varied between 604 and 650 people aged 55 and over for each country (see the online supplementary material), with an average age of 67 in Poland (range 55–94) and Portugal (range 55–92), 68 in France (range 55–90) and 69 in the Netherlands (range 55–89). Table 1 shows the descriptive information on gender, age, education and income characteristics of the survey respondents.

Table 1. Descriptives of the study

Notes: N = 2,494. SD: standard deviation. ASD: Aging Semantic Differential scale. KAOP: Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People scale (positive and negative subscales).

Source: European Institute for Innovation & Technology Health, Survey Ambitions of 55+ 2013–2016.

Ageism

The ageism items in the survey consist of the Aging Semantic Differential scale (ASD; Rosencranz and McNevin, Reference Rosencranz and McNevin1969) and the negative and positive subscales of Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People scale (KAOP; Kogan, Reference Kogan1961). The ASD, although over 50 years old, is still a widely used instrument to measure ageist attitudes (Gluth et al., Reference Gluth, Ebner and Schmiedek2010). Its 32 items consist of a seven-point scale between pairs of adjectives which have opposite meanings, e.g. active versus passive and neat versus tidy. The KAOP consists of 32 items, divided between 17 statements about older people that are worded negatively (the negative subscale) and 17 statements that are worded positively (the positive subscale). On a seven-point scale respondents indicate how much they agree or disagree with a certain statement. Like the ASD, the KAOP is an older instrument, yet frequently used and validated in various countries, also in more recent years (see Yen et al., Reference Yen, Liao, Chen, Kao, Lee and Wang2009; Küçükgüçlü et al., Reference Küçükgüçlu, Mert and Akpinar2011; Bleijenberg et al., Reference Bleijenberg, Jansen and Schuurmans2012; Vitman-Schorr et al., Reference Vitman-Schorr, Iecovich and Alfasi2014). In the data preparation for our analysis, certain items were reverse-coded, so that on all ageism items, higher scores represent a more negative attitude towards older people.

Reliability and construct validity testing was conducted on the ageism items as well as on the specific scales, both for internal consistency and the correlation of item scores with the total (sub)scale scores. The factor matrix and the scree plot of an exploratory factor analysis pointed to a three-factor structure of the ageism items, corresponding to the three (sub)scales from which the items originated. The scales were then also analysed through a confirmatory factor analysis grouped by country. In the factor analyses, the ordinal nature and possible correlation of the ageism items were taken into account. On the basis of these, the reliability tests as well as the factor analysis results, various items had to be excluded for each scale. The revised new scales were then again tested for reliability and internal consistency, resulting in satisfactory Cronbach's α scores. The total scores of each revised scale were used in the following analyses. Since the three scales did not sufficiently correlate with one another, we did not combine their scores into one single dependent item and continued to analyse them as three separate expressions of self-ageism. An overview of the specific items that are included in each revised scale and the mean country scores and Cronbach's α per revised scale can be found in the online supplementary material. Analyses were conducted using SPSS 21 (with R Essentials) and R 3.2.3.

Individual attitudes

To enable a statistically sound and theoretically coherent cross-cultural and cross-level analysis, we chose to focus on individual attitudes that can be theorised to influence (self-)ageism as outlined in the background and objectives, are more or less likely to be evaluated differently in different socio-cultural contexts, and could be aggregated solely from the survey data. Given that self-ageism has been shown to be strongly related to the physical decline inherent in ageing, concomitant with a loss of control that may impact self-image and by extension social identity, we include in this study individual attitudes related to this process. These are: lack of confidence in daily life, appreciation of intergenerational contact and the need for control over important aspects of one's life.

The individual attitudes appreciation of intergenerational contact and the need for control over one's life were aggregated from multiple items in our survey, which intended to measure the same underlying attitude constructs and used similar phrasing. The need to control important aspects of one's life was aggregated by combining four survey items asking respondents how important is it for them to have control and responsibility concerning four different domains of their lives. Appreciation of intergenerational contact was aggregated by combining three survey items that asked respondents if they like to come into contact with young people during (voluntary) work, if they like to maintain social contacts with young people and if they would prefer to live in a neighbourhood where younger as well as older people reside. Lack of confidence in daily life was measured by a single item in which respondents could indicate if they lack the confidence to carry out daily activities. The specific composition and aggregation procedures of all variables can be found in the online supplementary material.

Macro-level cultural values

Our starting point was that a society is shaped by certain shared cultural values and our analysis is thus best served by including values that are explicitly conceptualised to relate to structures, relationships and social roles within societies – in contrast to an approach that assumes one score describing an entire cultural value system and applies that assumption to an entire country. Therefore, we chose to include Schwartz's (Reference Schwartz2006, Reference Schwartz2008) data on cultural value orientations in our analysis, which shares these premises in its conceptualisation. Schwartz makes a clear distinction between individual human values and cultural value orientations, of which cultural value orientations emphasise the normative aspects of culture and take cultural distance into account, which can be considered a different approach addressing in some way often-raised limitations of two other widely used cultural value dimension scores by Hofstede (Reference Hofstede2001) and Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000). The Schwartz cultural value orientations were not gathered in an effort to identify representative cultural traits of a society, but rather to reflect orientations towards cultural values that, according to Schwartz's (Reference Schwartz2006, Reference Schwartz, Ginsburgh and Throsby2014) theory, are assumed to be present in most if not all societies, though to different extents. Therefore, the scores are not an indication of, or necessarily representative of, individual cultural norms – which could indeed be dynamic over time. They are decidedly intended as a macro-level variable that is indicative of the societal organisation and social policies of a country and its implicit and explicit normative cultural expectations to which its citizens have to relate all the time. Indeed Schwartz (Reference Schwartz, Ginsburgh and Throsby2014) himself already proposed that cultural value orientations as moderators might help us to understand cross-cultural differences in the relations between individual variables, which is exactly what we aim to investigate in our study.

The macro-level, country-specific variables in our model are the cultural values of intellectual autonomy and hierarchy, based on Schwartz's (Reference Schwartz2008) dataset of cultural value orientation scores. Though a quantitative analysis of cultural values always carries contestable assumptions about homogeneity and pace of cultural change, by using these value orientation scores we at least aim to avoid conflation of the notions of country and culture, and of individual human values and societal value orientations.

The two cultural value orientations included follow the line of reasoning of our analytical presumptions, focusing on cultural values that may influence and contextualise the individual attitudes deemed of importance in regard to self-ageism. Figure 1 further clarifies our analytical presumptions in a visual model. Specifically, intellectual autonomy values indicate the extent to which a society encourages individuals to develop and pursue their own preferences and ideas independently and take control of their own directions in life. Hierarchy values indicate the extent to which societies rely on a hierarchical distribution of prescribed social roles and corresponding power, rules and obligations (Schwartz, Reference Schwartz2008).

Figure 1. Visual model of the study’s analytical presumtions.

Analyses

We performed regression analyses (in SPSS 21) to investigate the association of individual characteristics with self-ageism, while accounting for the hierarchical, nested structure of the survey data and allowing for country comparison where the regression results indicated that to be relevant (see Table 2). Since the number of countries in our dataset is arguably too small for a multi-level model (Maas and Hox, Reference Maas and Hox2005), we used a fixed-effects model as described by Allison (Reference Allison2009), following the fixed-effects approach as explained and advocated by Möhring (Reference Möhring2012). We included country fixed effects in our model through the use of country dummy variables to avoid both the risk of omitted variable bias and the assumption of a normally distributed country-specific error term (Allison, Reference Allison2009; Möhring, Reference Möhring2012). This fixed-effects approach allows for a first investigation of the relevance for cross-cultural investigation by looking at the R 2 results of the fixed-effects null models, which can be compared to the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of a multi-level model in determining the between-country variance.

Table 2. Convergence across countries

Notes: ASD: Aging Semantic Differential scale. KAOP: Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People scale (positive and negative subscales). ICC: intraclass correlation coefficient.

Source: European Institute for Innovation & Technology Health, Survey Ambitions of 55+ 2013–2016.

Following up on the R 2 results, this specific fixed-effects approach allows us to investigate the influence of individual characteristics (as control variables) and individual attitudes as well as the moderating effects of macro-level indicators on these individual attitudes, by including cross-level interaction effects (Möhring, Reference Möhring2012). In our model, the interaction effect of macro-level, country-specific cultural value scores with individual attitudes enables us to investigate if the cultural values of intellectual autonomy and hierarchy have a moderating effect on individual attitudes that influence self-ageism.

Results

Summary

The three analysed ageism scales represent different dimensions and expressions of self-ageism, prevalent in each country in our study. The KAOP+ scale scores barely differed between countries. Poland and the Netherlands scored somewhat lower on the ASD scale than average (104.85 and 106.23 versus 107.08). Poland scored higher and the Netherlands lower on the KAOP− scale (60.32 and 46.22) than the average score of the four countries (53.09). An overview of the country scores can be found in the online supplementary material. The ASD scale representing the semantic expression of self-ageism and the KAOP+ scale representing low positive self-ageism both show convergence among countries (Table 2). However, we observe a significant amount of country-level variance in the KAOP− scale representing the negatively formulated expression of self-ageism (Table 2). Following further analysis of this country-level variance, the results indicate that the influence of individual attitudes in different countries is moderated by the country-specific cultural values of hierarchy and intellectual autonomy (Table 3).

Table 3. Effects on self-ageism

Notes: Fixed-effects model, ordinary least squares regression. Dependent: Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People negative subscale score. SE: standard error. Ref.: reference category.

Source: European Institute for Innovation & Technology Health, Survey Ambitions of 55+ 2013–2016; Schwartz (Reference Schwartz2008).

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.

Convergence and divergence across countries

The R 2 results of the fixed-effects model estimations show that the explained variance at the country level indicates convergence among countries regarding the semantic expression of self-ageism and low positive self-ageism (Table 2). These R 2 results are comparable to the ICC in a multi-level model, therewith indicating that considering the lack of between-country variance for those scales in our data, no further cross-cultural investigation of these scales was considered useful.

The R 2 results for the KAOP− scale clearly imply between-country variance and warrant further cross-cultural investigation for negatively formulated self-ageism. We therefore investigated if there are possible cultural explanations for the (in)significant influence of individual attitudes on negatively formulated self-ageism by using Schwartz's (Reference Schwartz2006, Reference Schwartz2008) cultural value scores, which point to different cultural value orientations in the four countries in our study.

Figure 2 illustrates how the slopes of individual attitudes vary across countries in steepness and direction in a model that is not controlled for country differences, in contrast to the fixed-effects model which is controlled for country differences.

Figure 2. Comparison of individual attitudes with or without control for country differences.

Notes: KAOP−: Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People scale (negative subscale). FR: France. NL: The Netherlands. PL: Poland. PT: Portugal.

As illustrated by the country slopes, lack of confidence is a significant individual attitude that influences ageism, yet without much divergence across countries. We do, however, identify random slopes for the need for control and intergenerational contact, underscoring the relevance of investigating moderating country-level effects on these specific individual attitudes by adding to our country fixed-effects model cross-level interactions with cultural value orientation scores (Schwartz, Reference Schwartz2008). Since in a country fixed-effects model no variance is left to be explained on the country level and we cannot model the direct or main effect of any country-level predictors, these cross-level interaction effects solely represent the moderator effect of the country-level variable. The assumptions underlying our model are that the coefficients and significance levels of the individual-level variables as well as of the cross-level interaction effect are nearly identical to those of multi-level models and can be interpreted as such (Allison, Reference Allison2009; Möhring, Reference Möhring2012). From the results of this analysis, presented in Table 3, we then identify that there are indeed significant moderating effects of cultural values on the association between individual attitudes and self-ageism. Taking into account how introducing cross-level interactions in our model magnify the formerly insignificant main effects of individual attitudes, we will solely discuss the interaction effects and how they moderate the individual attitudes that influence self-ageism. In our study, we find two interaction effects. First, the need for control over one's life increases self-ageism, except in countries with a high emphasis on the value of intellectual autonomy. Second, more appreciation of intergenerational contact predicts less self-ageism, yet this effect decreases in countries scoring higher on the cultural value of hierarchy.

Discussion and implications

Our findings seem to confirm the pervasiveness of ageism even among older individuals themselves and to a large extent across the countries under study (for country scores, see the online supplementary material). Similar to the multiple dimensions of ageism in younger generations (Cuddy et al., Reference Cuddy, Norton and Fiske2005; Löckenhoff et al., Reference Löckenhoff, De Fruyt, Terracciano, McCrae, De Bolle and Yik2009), we found that self-ageism is multi-dimensional, mirrored by the fact that we found that each scale captured a different expression of ageist attitudes. Our results on self-ageism largely confirm findings of comparative cross-cultural studies on ageist attitudes in the general population. These studies found ageism to be a pervasive and global phenomenon, even when particular attitudes towards older people might vary across different cultures (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003; Cuddy et al., Reference Cuddy, Norton and Fiske2005; Löckenhoff et al., Reference Löckenhoff, De Fruyt, Terracciano, McCrae, De Bolle and Yik2009). As expected, this finding appears also applicable to self-ageism.

Both the semantic and the low positive expressions of self-ageism show convergence across countries in our data. This may be due to the pervasive and pan-cultural nature of these expressions. Alternatively, it might be that the specific four countries in our dataset are not distinct enough when it comes to these specific expressions of self-ageism, a hypothesis that evidently needs further exploration of these expressions across more or other countries.

As common and pan-cultural as ageism has been argued to be, studies have already established that there are variations in expression and intensity, and have found associations between cultural values and cross-cultural variation in ageism. These studies have hypothesised links between traditional values and respect for older people, yet without identifying which specific dimensions and expressions of ageism might vary across cultures (McConatha et al., Reference McConatha, Schnell, Volkwein, Riley and Leach2003; Löckenhoff et al., Reference Löckenhoff, De Fruyt, Terracciano, McCrae, De Bolle and Yik2009). In this study, we confirm these expectations empirically by showing that not every expression of self-ageism is present to the same extent in every country. We observed a considerable amount of country-level variance in the negatively formulated expression of self-ageism – meaning that although this particular expression of self-ageism can be found in every country in our study, the individual attitudes that influence it might do so differently in each of the studied countries, perhaps as a result of differing extents to which they embrace cultural values, as illustrated by Figure 2. We found significantly moderating effects of the cultural values intellectual autonomy and hierarchy on individual attitudes that influence negatively formulated self-ageism. The need for control over one's life increases self-ageism, except in countries with a higher emphasis on the cultural value of intellectual autonomy. More appreciation of intergenerational contact predicts less self-ageism, yet this effect decreases in countries scoring higher on the cultural value of hierarchy. This suggests that in countries that value hierarchical social relations between generations, intergenerational contact is less effective in decreasing self-ageism than in countries that value more egalitarian relations between generations.

We can only make informed speculations about the exact nature of the moderating effects of cultural values on individual attitudes. In countries with less appreciation for intellectual autonomy, the need for control could be considered a more negative and undesirable attitude. When independent decision-making is considered to be less desirable by one's social environment, the need to be independent and in control might lead towards more negative attitudes towards an ageing self that is not able to live as autonomously as one would feel the need to do so. However, in countries with a high emphasis on the value of intellectual autonomy, making individual decisions is more appreciated and enabled. The need for control over aspects of one's life is therefore evaluated as a more positive and desirable attitude in such a cultural context. For example, it has been theorised that strong control beliefs, when supported, help older people to engage in preventive behaviour regarding healthy ageing and make them less inclined to accept negative stereotypes about ageing and age-related decline (Wurm et al., Reference Wurm, Tesch-Römer and Tomasik2007; Nelson, Reference Nelson2016). Thus, in brief, although rather speculative, it may be that in countries with higher emphasis on intellectual autonomy the need for control does not increase self-ageism, because it fits with socio-cultural valued behaviour and opportunities to exert this need for control in these countries.

Our results show that appreciation of intergenerational contact predicts lower levels of self-ageism, yet this effect is less pronounced in hierarchical societies. This could be explained by the contact hypothesis (Allport, Reference Allport1954): it is known that it is not merely intergenerational contact that can result in less ageism, the quality of this contact is of crucial importance (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Aron, McLaughlin-Volpe and Ropp1997; Dovidio et al., Reference Dovidio, Gaertner and Kawakami2003). Conditions that influence the quality of this contact are equal status and mutual respect and co-operation. In more hierarchical societies, social contact is more strongly tied to hierarchical societal roles, which likely reproduces unequal and stereotype-confirming statuses and dependencies – thereby diminishing the quality of intergenerational contact. Another possible interpretation for the found effect is rooted in social identity theory explanations of self-ageism. This theory suggests that ageism in older people is connected to their perception of their own age group as having a low social status in interactions with other groups (Kite and Wagner, Reference Kite, Wagner and Nelson2002; Bodner, Reference Bodner2009). In this context, hierarchical societies that put more emphasis on social status could perceive intergenerational interactions as a more negative experience, since it might stir fears about an age-related decline in social status. In turn, such a negative assessment of one's own social status could contribute to self-ageism (Bodner, Reference Bodner2009; Butler, Reference Butler2009).

We have to emphasise that our explanations of the moderating effects of cultural values apply specifically to individual attitudes that influence negatively formulated self-ageism. Self-ageism in older people is triggered by mechanisms other than ageism in young people or the general population, which might also not necessarily be moderated by cultural values in the same way or to the same extent. As the effect of a lack of confidence in daily life underscores, not all of the individual attitudes in our analysis might be applicable to ageism among the general population or susceptible to change in different cultural contexts. The latter is illustrated in Figure 2, where we can see that the slope of ‘lack of confidence’ does not take a different direction across countries, in contrast to the other two variables in this illustration. The influence of lack of confidence could be specific to self-ageism and irrelevant for ageism in the general population, following Bodner and Cohen-Fridel (Reference Bodner and Cohen-Fridel2010), who state that insecurity in social attachments increases self-ageism. They explain this effect through proposing that social attachment is a central issue in old age, when people become increasingly dependent on other people for their daily care and functioning. The association between lack of confidence in daily life and self-ageism might also work the other way around: negative ageist perceptions could contribute to problems with activities in daily life, wellbeing and health (Nelson, Reference Nelson2016).

The two individual attitudes in our study that turn out to be moderated by cultural values on the societal level only apply to one particular expression of self-ageism: the negatively formulated kind. Therefore, the results of our research imply that future studies need to investigate if effects of individual attitudes are specific for certain expressions of self-ageism, or are also applicable to expressions of ageism among the general population. Such future research would benefit from a focus on explanations for moderating effects of cultural value orientations that go beyond theoretical conjecture. In order to achieve this, we would recommend incorporating methodologies that allow for a more nuanced and culture-centred understanding of these complex processes of influence, as well as for comparison between different expressions of self-ageism.

Another implication of our findings is that in certain contexts, cultural value variation might very well influence the effectiveness and outcomes of social interventions that aim to reduce (self-)ageism. This knowledge has to be taken into consideration in future interventions as well as in policies designed around this issue. Taking the outcomes of our study into account, intergenerational contact interventions need adaptation to the cultural dynamics of more hierarchically inclined societies, in order to be as effective as they would be in a more egalitarian cultural context. Improving older people's confidence in social contact, however, could help to reduce self-ageism regardless of a society's appreciation of intellectual autonomy and hierarchy. A culturally sensitive approach to interventions might be of key importance in enabling more positive ageing experiences and outcomes, without the detrimental effects of self-ageism.

Although being unique in its cultural contextualisation of self-ageism, a limitation of the current study is that our research was limited to four European countries that served as proxies for different cultural contexts. Regarding our study, the theoretical assumptions behind cultural value orientations line up with our analysis and data. It is up to future research to establish whether our findings will hold in studies with other, culturally more distinct or heterogeneous countries. For instance, certain effects that did not vary across the four countries in our study – such as lack of confidence in social contact – may turn out to be influenced by cultural values after all when non-European or other European societies, with different cultural meanings of confidence, are part of the comparison. A more culturally diverse comparison would therefore be welcomed to study the pan-cultural pervasiveness of self-ageism further and the moderating effects of cultural values on (certain) expressions of self-ageism. Preferably, such a cross-cultural comparison would not be limited to assumptions of cultural homogeneity within countries – which the cultural value scores in our study are to a large extent – and instead find better ways to account for variations in cultural value orientations within and between countries, as well as the perceptibility of cultural value orientations to change over time.

Though cultural variation in ageism has been investigated before, to our knowledge this is the first study that empirically investigated the influence of different cultural contexts on individual attitudes that influence self-ageism. More insight into the different expressions of self-ageism in diverse cultural contexts will contribute to finding new, culturally sensitive and fitting ways in which self-ageism can be diminished. Ultimately, those new-found ways may reduce the ambivalent (self)ageist attitudes that perpetuate the exclusion and discrimination of older people in societies across the globe and will lead to more positive ageing experiences for the generations to come.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X20001622

Financial support

This work was supported by the Medical Delta network of life sciences, health and technology organisations and the European Institute for Innovation and Technology (EIT) Health network, who funded the data collection through surveys.

Ethical standards

Data collection was done through an international marketing agency, Trendbox. Recruitment and data collection followed the ICC/ESOMAR international code on market and social research, including their ethical guidelines (ICC/ESOMAR, 2008).

Author contributions

B. Schuurman performed all data analyses and wrote the first draft with advice from J. Lindenberg and J.P.J. Slaets. The study was initiated by J. Lindenberg and J.M. Huijg. J. Lindenberg contributed to the design and planning of the study, data interpretation and supervised the data analysis. J.M. Huijg contributed to the design and planning of the study. J.P.J. Slaets supervised the data analysis and contributed to the data interpretation. W.P. Achterberg contributed to the data interpretation. All authors commented upon various revisions and approved the final manuscript.

References

Allison, PD (2009) Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences: Fixed Effects Regression Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, GW (1954) The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.Google Scholar
Angus, J and Reeve, P (2006) Ageism: a threat to ‘aging well’ in the 21st century. Journal of Applied Gerontology 25, 137152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleijenberg, N, Jansen, MJM and Schuurmans, MJ (2012) Dutch nursing students’ knowledge and attitudes towards older people – a longitudinal cohort study. Journal of Nursing Education and Practice 2, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodner, E (2009) On the origins of ageism among older and younger adults. International Psychogeriatrics 21, 10031014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, E and Cohen-Fridel, S (2010) Relations between attachment styles, ageism and quality of life in late life. International Psychogeriatrics 22, 13531361.Google ScholarPubMed
Butler, RN (1969) Age-ism: another form of bigotry. The Gerontologist 9, 243246.Google ScholarPubMed
Butler, RN (1975) Why Survive? Being Old in America. New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Butler, RN (2009) Combating ageism. International Psychogeriatrics 21, 211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuddy, AJ and Fiske, S (2002) Doddering but dear: process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In Nelson, TD (ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older Persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 326.Google Scholar
Cuddy, AJ, Norton, MI and Fiske, S (2005) This old stereotype: the pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Journal of Social Issues 61, 267285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, JF, Gaertner, SL and Kawakami, K (2003) Intergroup contact: the past, present, and the future. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 6, 521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gluth, S, Ebner, NC and Schmiedek, F (2010) Attitudes toward younger and older adults: the German Aging Semantic Differential. International Journal of Behavioral Development 34, 147158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G (2001) Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations Across Nations, 2nd Edn. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Huijg, JM, van Delden, AEQ, Ouderaa, FJG, Westendorp, RGJ, Slaets, JPJ and Lindenberg, J (2016) Being active, engaged and healthy: older persons' plans and wishes to age successfully. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 72, 228236. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw107.Google Scholar
ICC/ESOMAR (2008) ICC/ESOMAR International Code on Market, Opinion and Social Research and Data Analytics. Available at http://www.esomar.org/knowledge-and-standards/codes-and-guidelines.php.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R and Baker, WE (2000) Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review 65, 1951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kite, ME and Wagner, LS (2002) Attitudes toward older adults. In Nelson, TD (ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older Persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 129161.Google Scholar
Kogan, N (1961) Attitudes toward old people: the development of a scale and an examination of correlates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62, 4454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küçükgüçlu, Ö, Mert, H and Akpinar, B (2011) Reliability and validity of Turkish version of attitudes toward old people scale. Journal of Clinical Nursing 20, 31963203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lev, S, Wurm, S and Ayalon, L (2018) Origins of ageism at the individual level. In Ayalon, L and Tesch-Römer, C (eds), Contemporary Perspectives on Ageism. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, pp. 5172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, BR (2003) Mind matters: cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 58B, 203211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B (2009) Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18, 332336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, BR, Ryall, AL, Pilver, CE, Sheridan, PL, Wei, JY and Hausdorff, JM (2008) Influence of African American elders’ age stereotypes on their cardiovascular response to stress. Anxiety, Stress and Coping 21, 8593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, BR, Ferrucci, L, Zonderman, AB, Slade, MD, Troncoso, J and Resnick, SM (2016) A culture–brain link: negative age stereotypes predict Alzheimer's disease biomarkers. Psychology and Aging 31, 8288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberg, J and Westendorp, RGJ (2015) Overcoming old in age-friendliness. Journal of Social Work Practice 29, 8598. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650533.2014.993949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Löckenhoff, CE, De Fruyt, F, Terracciano, A, McCrae, RR, De Bolle, M and Yik, M (2009) Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging 24, 941954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maas, CJM and Hox, JJ (2005) Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology: European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences 1, 8692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConatha, JT, Schnell, F, Volkwein, K, Riley, L and Leach, E (2003) Attitudes toward aging: a comparative analysis of young adults from the United States and Germany. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 57, 203215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minichiello, V, Browne, J and Kendig, H (2000) Perceptions and consequences of ageism: views of older people. Ageing & Society 20, 253278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möhring, K (2012) The fixed effects approach as alternative to multilevel models for cross-national analyses. GK SOCLIFE, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany, Working Paper Series 16/2012. Available at http://www.soclife.uni-koeln.de/fileadmin/wiso_fak/gk_soclife/pdf/Working_papers/KMoehring_2012a_ssrn_the_fixed_effects.pdf.Google Scholar
Nelson, TD (2016) Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist 71, 276282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
North, MS and Fiske, ST (2015) Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: a cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 141, 9931021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmore, E (1990) Ageism: Negative and Positive. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Palmore, EB, Branch, L and Harris, D (2005) Encyclopedia of Ageism. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.Google Scholar
Rosencranz, HA and McNevin, TE (1969) A factor analysis of attitudes toward the aged. The Gerontologist 9, 5559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schillewaert, N and Meulemeester, P (2005) Comparing response distributions of offline and online data collection methods. International Journal of Market Research 47, 163178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, SH (2006) A theory of cultural value orientations: explication and applications. Comparative Sociology 5, 137182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, SH (2008) The 7 Schwartz cultural value orientation scores for 80 countries (Dataset with description). Available at https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.3313.3040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, SH (2014) National culture as value orientations: consequences of value differences and cultural distance. In Ginsburgh, VA and Throsby, D (eds), Handbook of the Economics of Art and Culture, Vol. 2. Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 547586.Google Scholar
Smith, JR and Hogg, MA (2008) Social identity and attitudes. In Crano W and Prislin R (eds) Attitudes and Attitude Change. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 337360.Google Scholar
Swift, HJ, Abrams, D, Marques, S, Vauclair, CM, Bratt, C and Lima, ML (2018) Ageism in the European region: finding from the European Social Survey. In Ayalon, L and Tesch-Römer, C (eds), Contemporary Perspectives on Ageism. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, pp. 441459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H (1981) Human Groups and Social Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vitman-Schorr, A, Iecovich, E and Alfasi, N (2014) Reliability and validity of a Hebrew version of the Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People Scale. Educational Gerontology 40, 315326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westerhof, GJ and Wurm, S (2015) Longitudinal research on subjective aging, health, and longevity. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics 35, 145165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitbourne, SK and Sneed, JR (2002) The paradox of well-being, identity processes, and stereotype threat. In Nelson, TD (ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older Persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 247273.Google Scholar
Wright, SC, Aron, A, McLaughlin-Volpe, T and Ropp, SA (1997) The extended contact effect: knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73, 7390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wurm, S, Tesch-Römer, C and Tomasik, MJ (2007) Longitudinal findings on aging-related cognitions, control beliefs, and health in later life. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 62B, 156164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yen, C-H, Liao, W-C, Chen, Y-R, Kao, M-C, Lee, M-C and Wang, C-C (2009) A Chinese version of Kogan's Attitude toward Older People Scale: reliability and validity assessment. International Journal of Nursing Studies 46, 3844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptives of the study

Figure 1

Figure 1. Visual model of the study’s analytical presumtions.

Figure 2

Table 2. Convergence across countries

Figure 3

Table 3. Effects on self-ageism

Figure 4

Figure 2. Comparison of individual attitudes with or without control for country differences.Notes: KAOP−: Kogan's Attitudes Toward Old People scale (negative subscale). FR: France. NL: The Netherlands. PL: Poland. PT: Portugal.

Supplementary material: File

Schuurman et al. supplementary material

Schuurman et al. supplementary material

Download Schuurman  et al. supplementary material(File)
File 25 KB