Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:50:17.865Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

MCQs and learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
From the Editor
Copyright
Copyright © iStockphoto/Devon Stephens 

Readers may have noticed new text on the inside front cover of Advances. Psychiatrists participating in the American Medical Association (AMA) CME system can now claim credits by completing the MCQs that feature at the end of each article. Although MCQs are not universally loved, the attractions of using them for summative assessment are significant. Well-written MCQs are reliable, test breadth of knowledge and are easy to administer and mark. Certificates generated through the College's CPD Online website (www.psychiatrycpd.co.uk) give evidence of participation in CPD and CME programmes. Such evidence, held in portfolios, is kept for regulatory processes such as appraisal and revalidation.

MCQs can also be used for personal development. In the first issue of Advances the Editor wrote: ‘There will be opportunities for the reader to test what has been learnt from each article in the form of a few multiple choice questions’ (Reference SimsSims 1994). This in a nutshell is formative assessment – a personal test of understanding rather than a formal evaluation of knowledge. Answering MCQs gives feedback in self-assessment, checking for yourself rather than proving to someone else. Not knowing the best answer reveals a gap in knowledge. Getting a question wrong may be more useful still. ‘It is impossible for a man to learn what he thinks he already knows’, observed the Greek Stoic philosopher Epictetus (c. 55–135 ad). The disquiet experienced in finding out you are wrong when sure you are right, cognitive dissonance, is at the root of all learning. It has been called ‘learning-by-disturbing’ (Reference Robinson, Purves and WilsonRobinson 2003). And testing supports not only learning but also remembering. We sense that repeatedly reading an article is a good strategy to aid recall. But we are deceived. Testing has a powerful positive effect on long-term retention which re-reading does not (Reference Roediger and KarpickeRoediger 2006). What follows is that, to be helpful for learning, MCQs ought to focus on the key points of an article. Trivial, ambiguous or superficial questions (and answers) are unhelpful. Authors should use questions to signal their take-home messages.

Developmental dyslexia

Thambirajah (pp. 299–307) points out that, although dyslexia is ‘bound to be overrepresented’ in the clinics of CAMHS practitioners, ‘its relationship, if any, to adult mental health is very limited’. Nevertheless, his article, which is my Editor's Pick, will be of interest to many non-specialists. The author teases out the precise deficits and discusses whether dyslexia is a categorical disorder or simply at the extreme end of a spectrum of reading ability. The impact on the child may be substantial, and thus an important aspect of psychiatric intervention is ‘facilitating navigation through their normal developmental tasks’. He touches on language, culture, biology and history in an educational article which is intellectually stimulating without being patronising.

References

Sims, A (1994) Introducing continuing professional development. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 1: 38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P, Purves, I, Wilson, R (2003) Learning support for the consultation: information support and decision support should be placed in an educational framework. Medical Education 37: 429–33.Google Scholar
Roediger, HL, Karpicke, JD (2006) Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science 17: 249–55.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.