Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-30T23:02:23.628Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parsing reward sensitivity reveals distinct relationships with energy intake, metabolic markers, physical activity and fitness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2025

Evelyn Kiive
Affiliation:
Division of Special Education, Department of Education, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
Urmeli Katus
Affiliation:
Department of Family Medicine and Public Health, University of Tartu, Estonia, Tartu, Tartumaa
Diva Eensoo
Affiliation:
Department of Chronic Diseases, National Institute for Health Development, Tallinn, Harjumaa, Estonia
Inga Villa
Affiliation:
Department of Family Medicine and Public Health, University of Tartu, Estonia, Tartu, Tartumaa
Jarek Mäestu
Affiliation:
Division of Exercise Biology, Institute of Sport Sciences and Physiotherapy, University of Tartu, Tartu, Tartumaa, Estonia
Toomas Veidebaum
Affiliation:
Department of Chronic Diseases, National Institute for Health Development, Tallinn, Harjumaa, Estonia
Jaanus Harro*
Affiliation:
Division of Neuropsychopharmacology, Department of Chemistry, University of Tartu, Tartu, Tartumaa, Estonia
*
Corresponding author: Jaanus Harro; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Rewards are rewarding owing to their hedonic or metabolic value. Individual differences in sensitivity to rewards are predictive of mental health problems but may reflect variation in metabolic types. We have assessed the association of two distinguishable aspects of reward sensitivity, openness to rewards (the striving towards multiple rewards) and insatiability by reward (the strong pursuit and fixation to a particular reward), with measures of metabolism and activity in a longitudinal study of representative birth cohort samples. We used data of the Estonian Children Personality Behaviour and Health Study (original n = 1238) collected at age 15, 18 and 25. Reward sensitivity and physical activity were self-reported during a laboratory visit, when also blood sampling, measurement of blood pressure, height and weight, aerobic exercise testing and the diet interview, after the participants had kept food diary, took place. In the younger cohort, physical activity was also assessed by accelerometry at age 18 and 25. Across adolescence and young adulthood, openness to rewards was positively associated with physical activity and negatively with blood pressure and serum levels of glucose, insulin and cholesterol levels. In contrast, insatiability by reward was positively associated with serum triglyceride levels and negatively with energy intake and cardiorespiratory fitness. In conclusion, the two facets of reward sensitivity have a fairly different association with a variety of metabolic and health-related measures. This may explain the variable findings in literature, and suggests that individual differences in reward sensitivity are part of a complex physiological variability, including energy expenditure profiles.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • In birth cohort representative samples, sensitivity to rewards was associated with physical activity, aerobic fitness, energy intake and serum markers of metabolism.

  • These associations were clearly different for the two distinguishable aspects of reward sensitivity: Openness to Rewards (striving towards multiple rewards) versus Insatiability by Reward (fixation to a specific reward).

Limitations

  • While the sample was highly representative and data from three observations over 10 years were used, the study represents one setting and hence requires replication.

  • This study was observational, and causality cannot be implied in either direction. Studies on underlying biological mechanisms are needed to guide the design of experimental models.

Introduction

Sensitivity to rewards refers to the degree to which an individual’s behaviour is motivated by reward-relevant stimuli (Gray, Reference Gray1970; Carver and White, Reference Carver and White1994; Corr and Cooper, Reference Corr and Cooper2016). Indeed, individuals vary considerably in their sensitivity to rewards (Carver and White, Reference Carver and White1994; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Yoon, Kim and Hamann2015; Corr, Reference Corr2016), and this has major implications for the development of multiple psychiatric disorders (Holroyd and Umemoto, Reference Holroyd and Umemoto2016). On the other hand, reward sensitivity appears to be conserved across evolution, given that many mammalian species show reward-related behavioural patterns similar to humans (Spear, Reference Spear2011) and share its neurobiological substrate (Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998). From an evolutionary perspective, reward sensitivity is an absolutely vital construct; all necessities for survival and procreation are considered naturally rewarding, and rewards also reinforce the associated behaviours (Blaukopf and DiGirolamo, Reference Blaukopf and DiGirolamo2007; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Hagen and Hammerstein2008). Reward sensitivity is a trait strongly dependent on the activity of the mesolimbic dopamine system, which attributes salience to stimuli and makes them an incentive (Robinson and Berridge, Reference Robinson and Berridge1993) and supports seeking out resources (Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998) by increasing appetitive behavioural arousal and facilitating sensitivity to rewards while promoting adherence to safety (Ikemoto and Panksepp, Reference Ikemoto and Panksepp1999). Dopamine neurones fire in correspondence with the subjective value of reward (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Hickman, Al-Mohammad, Stasiak and Schultz2024) and can regulate dietary choices (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Gonzalez Magaña, Roshgadol, Tian and Ryan2024), promote habitual physical activity (Ruiz-Tejada et al., Reference Ruiz-Tejada, Neisewander and Katsanos2022) and balance feeding and physical exercise-related energy expenditure (Walle et al., Reference Walle, Petitbon, Fois, Varin, Montalban, Hardt, Contini, Angelo, Potier, Ortole, Oummadi, De Smedt-Peyrusse, Adan, Giros, Chaouloff, Ferreira, de d’Exaerde, Ducrocq, Georges and Trifilieff2024). Indeed, animals evolved as reward seekers, but reward-seeking behaviour entails severe costs and benefits (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Wise, Rhodes and Gottfried2011). A life of movement necessitates decisions about where to move, when and whether to do so. Animals must decide whether to abandon the relative safety that often comes from staying still to forage for nutrients and other rewards. In turn, dietary sugar and lipids can act as functional modulators of the dopaminergic reward circuit (May et al., Reference May, Rosander, Gottfried, Dennis and Dus2020; Berland et al., Reference Berland, Small, Luquet and Gangaross2021). The physiological checks and balances have evolved over millions of years in an effortful search for safety, energy and nutrients, in stark contrast with the modern lifestyle of humans, which is now superimposed over the drives shaped by evolution, with obvious consequences to public health (Higgs et al., Reference Higgs, Spetter, Thomas, Rotshtein, Lee, Hallschmid and Dourish2017; Wiss et al., Reference Wiss, Avena and Rada2018; Woessner et al., Reference Woessner, Tacey, Levinger-Limor, Parker, Levinger and Levinger2021).

Different strategies of exploration of the physical environment and seeking out rewards have been explained as an outcome of the interaction between individual traits of anxiety and motivation (Harro, Reference Harro2010), with reward sensitivity being embedded in the latter. However, individual differences in energy requirement may also play a role. These may also explain the variable level of health risks brought about by sedentary behaviour and excessive energy intake (Wells, Reference Wells2017; Reddon et al., Reference Reddon, Patel, Turcotte, Pigeyre and Meyre2018). The variability in energy requirement is affected by numerous physiological factors (National Academies of Sciences et al., 2023). Besides resting energy expenditure as the most significant component of total energy expenditure, physical activity is the second largest component. Metabolic responses to food, age, sex, body size, body composition and climate also contribute.

Some evidence does suggest that personality and metabolism are related. For example, individuals who scored lower on neuroticism and higher on extraversion, openness and conscientiousness had significantly higher energy expenditure at peak walking pace, suggesting an association of psychological processes with energy homeostasis and individual differences in aerobic capacity (Terracciano et al., Reference Terracciano, Schrack, Sutin, Chan, Simonsick and Ferrucci2013). Blum et al. (Reference Blum, Cull, Braverman and Comings1996) have described the reward deficiency syndrome, proposing that individuals who have functional genetic deviations of one or more of the components of the reward pathway tend to be less satisfied with natural rewards and seek enhanced stimulation of these pathways, for example, through drugs, food or engaging in dangerous sports. The syndrome is associated with hypo-dopaminergic function resulting in abnormal craving behaviour, and alterations in dopamine reward circuits can lead to pathologic food consumption, increased fat synthesis and digestive disorders (Blum et al., Reference Blum, Thanos and Gold2014). Miró-Padilla et al. (Reference Miró-Padilla, Adrián-Ventura, Cherednichenko, Monzonís-Carda, Beltran-Valls, MolinerUrdiales and Ávila2023) studied the impact of personality and the volume of reward-related brain areas on individual differences in voluntary physical activity, objectively measured by an accelerometer, in young adults. Smaller volumes of the right anterior cingulate cortex were related to lower scores in reward sensitivity, which contributed to explaining low levels of daily physical activity. The authors suggested that individual differences in the activity of the reward system constrain a behavioural repertoire characterised by more vigorous and frequent actions aimed at obtaining rewards, with practising physical activity being a good example of this behaviour (Miró-Padilla et al., Reference Miró-Padilla, Adrián-Ventura, Cherednichenko, Monzonís-Carda, Beltran-Valls, MolinerUrdiales and Ávila2023).

Intuitively, reward sensitivity should be in positive association with the consumption of palatable foods. Indeed, a positive association between sensitivity to reward and unhealthy snacks and sugar-sweetened beverage consumption in adolescents has been reported (De Cock et al., Reference De Cock, Van Lippevelde, Vervoort, Vangeel, Maes, Eggermont, Braet, Lachat, Huybregts, Goossens, Beullens, Kolsteren and Van Camp2016). In addition to tastiness, foods are also rewarding because of their caloric value: van Rijn et al. (Reference van Rijn, Griffioen-Roose, de Graaf and Smeets2016) found that neural responses to oral calories from a maltodextrin solution are modulated by reward sensitivity in reward-related areas such as the caudate nucleus, amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex. However, a systematic review concluded that while reward sensitivity, primarily measured by the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) or Behavioural Inhibition/Activation System Scale (BIS/BAS), is positively associated with binge eating and emotional, external, hedonic and excessive eating behaviours as well as obesity-related outcomes, the effect sizes are small to moderate (Sutton et al., Reference Sutton, L.’Insalata and Fazzino2022). Indeed, there is also a host of research not finding any association of reward sensitivity with food intake or body composition (e.g. Scholten et al., Reference Scholten, Schrijvers, Nederkoorn, Kremers and Rodenburg2014; Vandeweghe et al., Reference Vandeweghe, Verbeken, Vervoort, Moens and Braet2017; Goldschmidt et al., Reference Goldschmidt, Smith, Lavender, Engel and Haedt-Matt2019; Jonker et al., Reference Jonker, van Malderen, Glashouwer, Vervoort, Braet, Goossens and de Jong2019).

Individual energy expenditure and intake differences depend on the underlying metabolic phenotype, such as thrifty versus spendthrift (e.g. Piaggi et al., Reference Piaggi, Vinalis, Basolo, Santini and Krakoff2018; Hollstein et al., Reference Hollstein, Basolo, Ando, Votruba, Walter, Krakoff and Piaggi2020). A thrifty phenotype is characterised by a greater decrease in energy expenditure with fasting associated with its smaller increase in response to overfeeding. A thrifty phenotype is thus prone to less weight loss during underfeeding but greater weight gain with overfeeding. This phenotype is considered metabolically efficient and presumably engenders a risk of obesity. By contrast, a spendthrift phenotype markedly increases energy expenditure in response to overfeeding but shows only small decreases with underfeeding. Individuals with different energy profiles may differ in their reward processing, as the homeostatic and hedonic systems interact to regulate body weight (Berthoud et al., Reference Berthoud, Münzberg and Morrison2017). The functional interaction of the hypothalamus with the corticolimbic system and the brainstem provides the emotional, cognitive and executive support to the drive to consume food.

The relatively inconsistent association of reward sensitivity with dietary intake and body composition may however derive from the heterogeneity of the reward sensitivity construct. We have recently shown in a birth cohort representative sample that reward sensitivity can be parsed into two independent components: one that represents striving towards multiple rewards (Openness to Rewards) and the other characterising the strong pursuit and fixation to a particular reward (Insatiability by Reward) (Pulver et al., Reference Pulver, Kiive and Harro2020a). Compared to existing reward sensitivity models, for example, with the BIS/BAS framework, the Reward Openness and Insatiability Scale (ROIS) does not, unlike the behavioural activation system, assess the affective arousal or drive intensity associated with the obtaining or anticipating of rewards. Instead, it seeks to distinguish an individual’s tendency to remain receptive to a broad array of, particularly novel, rewards from a tendency to fixate on a specific reward due to impaired impulse control.

These two components of reward sensitivity, Openness to Rewards and Insatiability by Reward, were distinctly related to the affective neuroscience personality traits, and only the latter was associated with symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Only Insatiability by Reward was associated with body weight, body mass index (BMI), sum of five skinfolds, waist circumference, hip circumference and waist-to-height ratio in adolescents and young adults (Katus et al., Reference Katus, Villa, Ringmets, Pulver, Veidebaum and Harro2020a). Another interesting finding was the complex relationship of the two components of reward sensitivity with aggressiveness (Pulver et al., Reference Pulver, Kiive, Kanarik and Harro2020b). Thus, while in the whole sample, aggressiveness was in positive association with Insatiability by Reward, there was a distinction in one fifth of the sample, the homozygotes for an aggressiveness-related minor variant of the orexin/hypocretin receptor type 1 encoding gene HCRTR1 (Harro et al., Reference Harro, Laas, Eensoo, Kurrikoff, Sakala, Vaht, Parik, Mäestu and Veidebaum2019): in HCRTR1 rs2271933 A/A homozygotes, aggressiveness was, exceptionally, related to Openness to Rewards. Given the central role of orexins in reward processing and motivation (Mahler et al., Reference Mahler, Moorman, Smith, James and Aston-Jones2014; Baimel et al., Reference Baimel, Bartlett, Chiou, Lawrence, Muschamp, Patkar, Tung and Borgland2015), glucose homeostasis (Tsuneki et al., Reference Tsuneki, Wada and Sasaoka2010), promotion of foraging and homeostatic eating (Barson, Reference Barson2020), but also in cardiorespiratory system (Shahid et al., Reference Shahid, Rahman and Pilowsky2012) and voluntary physical exercise (Tesmer et al., Reference Tesmer, Li, Bracey, Schmandt, Polania, Peleg-Raibstein and Burdakov2024), and considering the apparently dual association of orexin receptor genotype in shaping reward sensitivity (see Pulver et al., Reference Pulver, Kiive, Kanarik and Harro2020b), we aimed at assessing the association of the two distinct aspects of reward sensitivity with dietary intake, metabolic markers, physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness in this longitudinally studied birth cohort representative sample.

Materials and methods

Study sample

This study was carried out using a sample from the Estonian Children’s Personality Behaviour and Health Study (ECPBHS). The rationale of the formation of the original sample and the study procedure have been described previously (Harro et al., Reference Harro, Eensoo, Kiive, Merenäkk, Alep, Oreland and Harro2001). Of the eligible group, 79.1% participated, and the original sample consisted of 583 subjects aged 9 years and 593 subjects aged 15 years. Follow-up studies took place at ages 15, 18 and 25 years. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants and, in the case of minors, also from their parents. The study was approved by the Ethics Review Committee on Human Research of the University of Tartu and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measurement of reward sensitivity

Reward sensitivity was assessed using the ROIS; the construction and description of ROIS have been described in detail elsewhere (Pulver et al., Reference Pulver, Kiive and Harro2020a). ROIS comprises 28 items, divided equally between two higher-order factors: Openness to Rewards, a strive towards a multiplicity of rewards, and Insatiability by Reward, characterised by an excessive fixation on a particular reward. Openness to Rewards includes Excitement and Novelty, with items reflecting the search for new experiences and excitement, and Social Experiences, with items primarily associated with sociability and social exchange. Insatiability by Reward comprises Excessive Spending, with items related to impulsive buying and excessive spending, and Giving in to Cravings, with items related to low self-control and trouble in resisting temptations. Personality information for analysis by ROIS was collected at age 25 or 33. The mean item score was used in statistical analysis.

Dietary intake

In different study waves, dietary food intake recall was used for 24 h, 48 h or 72 h (Joost et al., Reference Joost, Villa, Comasco, Oreland, Veidebaum and Harro2019). Participants were asked to complete a diet record at home during the day(s) before the study day. A face-to-face interview was performed on the study day. Data on portion size that was not recorded in the food diary were estimated using pictures of portion sizes (Haapa et al., Reference Haapa, Toponen, Pietinen and Räsänen1985). Where data from 2 or 3 days were available, the mean nutrient intake relative energy intake per day was calculated. Dietary intake was assessed using the Finnish Micro-Nutrica Nutritional Analysis programme adapted to include Estonian foods, Estonian version 2.0 (Tallinn University of Technology, Food Processing Institute, Estonia) and using the NutriData food consumption database, versions 4.0–7.0 (National Institute for Health Development, Estonia).

Cardiorespiratory fitness and physical activity

Cardiorespiratory fitness was determined using a cycle-ergometer (Tunturi T8, Tunturi New Fitness B.V., Finland) test with progressively increasing workload until exhaustion and was defined as maximal power output calculated per kilogram of body weight (W/kg). The procedure has been described in detail elsewhere (Lätt et al., Reference Lätt, Jürimäe, Harro, Loit and Mäestu2018). Physical activity was assessed using self- and parent-reported questionnaires. Individual physical activity scores were calculated as previously described (Katus et al., Reference Katus, Villa, Ringmets, Vaht, Mäestu, Mäestu and Harro2020b), and standardised (z-scores) scores of each year were used. In the younger cohort at ages 18 and 25, physical activity was also measured by uniaxial accelerometer GT1M (ActiGraph, Monrovia, CA, USA) to detect vertical accelerations ranging in magnitude from 0.05 to 2.00 g with a frequency response of 0.25–2.50 Hz. Each participant was asked to carry the monitor on the right hip for seven consecutive days during the awakening hours, and steps per day were used in the analysis.

Blood pressure, metabolic markers and blood lipid measurements

Resting systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were measured in a laboratory setting on the left arm with an automatic oscillometric method from the left arm in a sitting position. Five consecutive measurements were made at 2-min intervals, and the mean value was used in the analysis.

Venous blood samples were taken from the antecubital vein after a fast of 8–12 h and analysed in a certified clinical laboratory. Insulin resistance was estimated using the homeostatic model assessment (HOMA) index, which was calculated as fasting glucose (mmol/l) × fasting insulin (mU/l)/22.5 (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Hosker, Rudenski, Naylor, Treacher and Turner1985). Fasting basal cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol and low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, as well as triglyceride (TRG) levels, were measured by conventional techniques in the Central Laboratory of the Tartu University Hospital and presented in mmol/l (Tomson et al., Reference Tomson, Merenäkk, Loit, Mäestu and Harro2011).

Statistical analysis

Longitudinal associations between reward sensitivity and dietary intake, cardiorespiratory fitness, blood pressure and metabolic measures were assessed using the linear mixed-effects regression models with random intercept and random slope. Mixed models are well suited for longitudinal data as they account for the correlations between repeated measurements within each subject, and in such data, the variance is often heterogeneous over time. These models allow different correlation matrices as observations may be correlated in several different ways. In addition, unlike repeated measures ANOVA, mixed models include all available data to the analysis (Detry and Ma, Reference Detry and Ma2016). Mixed models accommodate unbalanced data patterns using all available observations in the analysis assuming that missingness is random, that is, independent of unobserved measurements but dependent on the observed measurements. Imputation techniques were not applied. Dependent measures were dietary intake, cardiorespiratory fitness, blood pressure and metabolic measures at baseline (age 15 years) and follow-up points (18 years and 25 years). The reward sensitivity score was defined as the independent variable. Time was treated as a continuous variable. The likelihood ratio test was used to assess the goodness of fit of the statistical models. Unstructured or exchangeable covariance structure and restricted maximum likelihood method were used. In the younger cohort, accelerometry data were available but only for two timepoints (18 and 25 years), and thus less complex mixed-effects regression models were fitted with only random intercept and using compound symmetric covariance structure. For illustrative purposes, the relationship of aspects of reward sensitivity with selected measures at age 25 was also compared by reward sensitivity groups (lowest and highest quartile and medium 50%) after one-way ANOVA analysis and subsequent post hoc LSD tests.

Results

Results from the linear mixed-effects regression models showing the relationship between reward sensitivity scores and glucose and lipid metabolism from 15 to 25 years of age are presented in Table 1. Blood glucose, insulin and HOMA index were inversely associated with Openness to Rewards (OR) and especially with higher Social Experience subscale. The relationship of Openness to Rewards with glucose levels at age 25 is depicted in Figure 1A (F2,741 = 7.81; p < 0.001; η = 0.021). A trend of negative association of Openness to Rewards with total cholesterol levels was also noted. Furthermore, a negative association between OR Social Experience subscale (p < 0.05) and LDL cholesterol was observed, while no associations between reward sensitivity and HDL cholesterol were detected (Table 1).

Figure 1. Aspects of reward sensitivity and selected measures of metabolism and lifestyle at age 25. This illustrative analysis is based on quartile groups of rewards sensitivity facets. Blood glucose level (A), energy intake (B), physical activity score (C) and cardiovascular fitness by maximal power output (D) at age 25 years in ECPBHS participants with low (25%), medium (50%) or high (25%) reward sensitivity. *p < 0.05; **<0.005, ***<0.001 different from Low group. ¤ p < 0.05; ¤¤ p < 0.005 different from High group. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Table 1. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in insulin resistance and food lipid concentration (mmol/l), from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model

Similar associations with glucose and lipid metabolism were not present with the other facet of reward sensitivity, Insatiability by Reward. Instead, linear mixed-effects regression models suggested a significant negative relationship between Insatiability by Reward and its both subscales and body weight-adjusted daily energy intake, protein, lipid and carbohydrate intake (Table 2; Figure 1B for energy intake; F2,725 = 4.73; p < 0.01; η=0.013). In contrast, a subscale of Openness to Rewards, Excitement and Novelty, was positively associated with protein intake as a percentage from daily energy intake (Table 2).

Table 2. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in daily energy intake (kcal), nutrient intake (g/kg) and nutrient intake as a percentage from daily energy intake (E%) from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model

The analysis also revealed a significant negative relationship between Openness to Rewards and SBP and DBP. Furthermore, a positive association between Openness to Rewards and its subscales and physical activity was noted (Table 3 and Figure 1C; F2,761 = 2.98; p < 0.05; η = 0.01). Again, such associations were not found for Insatiability by Reward, but this aspect of reward sensitivity was negatively associated with cardiorespiratory fitness (Table 3; Figure 1D; F2,711 = 9.14; p < 0.001; η = 0.025). In contrast, maximal power output was in positive association with the Excitement and Novelty subscale of Openness to Rewards.

Table 3. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in blood pressure, cardiovascular fitness expressed as maximum power output (MPO; W/kg) and physical activity from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model

sPA score, standardized physical activity score; *analysis is restricted to data of the younger cohort collected at ages 18 and 25.

Discussion

Herewith, we describe how parsing reward sensitivity can help to reveal the association of this behavioural trait with metabolic markers, dietary intake, physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness.

Openness to Rewards, an aspect of reward sensitivity corresponding to striving towards reward variety, was associated throughout adolescence and young adulthood with lower fasting glucose and insulin levels and HOMA index. This association was rather owing to one of the two subscales of Openness to Rewards, which specifically addresses rewarding aspects of social relationships. Social competence is negatively affected by impulsivity traits, and impulse control may be metabolically expensive process, creating higher glucose demand (Gailliot and Baumeister, Reference Gailliot and Baumeister2007). Low blood glucose levels are indeed associated with impulsive aggression (DeWall et al., Reference DeWall, Deckman, Gailliot and Bushman2011); administration of glucose can reduce aggression in response to provocation among people high in trait aggression (Denson et al., Reference Denson, von Hippel, Kemp and Teo2010) and reduce disinhibition in a continuous performance test (Flint and Turek, Reference Flint and Turek2003), whereas behavioural flexibility depends on glucoregulation (Riby et al., Reference Riby, Lai Teik Ong, Azmie, Ooi, Regina, Yeo, Massa and Aquili2017).

Higher Openness to Rewards was also associated with higher habitual physical activity. This appears as fitting with the construct of an active, energetic reward seeker and may relate to the similar positive association with cardiovascular fitness and lower blood pressure and the trend of lower blood lipid levels. It should be noted that Openness to Rewards did not associate with body weight and BMI in these subjects (Katus et al., Reference Katus, Villa, Ringmets, Pulver, Veidebaum and Harro2020a). It appears important to observe the dynamics of these relationships as the subjects age.

Earlier analysis on the same sample had revealed that Insatiability by Reward and both of its components were significantly positively associated with body weight and BMI (Katus et al., Reference Katus, Villa, Ringmets, Pulver, Veidebaum and Harro2020a). This aspect of reward sensitivity did not have any similar associations with blood measures of carbohydrate or lipid metabolism as the other. A positive relationship between this fixation to reward measure was present specifically with TRG levels. It was also not related to habitual physical activity and was, not surprisingly given the association with BMI, negatively correlated with cardiovascular fitness. Nonetheless, Insationability by Reward was in inverse association with nutrient intake if this was adjusted to body weight. Hence, it could be assumed that this aspect of reward sensitivity is related to metabolic efficiency, this being lower in subjects with higher Insatiability. Openness to Rewards, in contrast, was not associated with nutrient intake; however, its facet with a more general nature had a trend for a positive association with protein intake, which was statistically significant for the proportional intake of protein in the diet. Disruption of the interaction between homeostatic and reward circuitry might promote overeating and contribute to obesity (Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Tomasi and Baler2012), and obese individuals experience less activation of reward circuits from the actual food consumption, whereas they show greater activation of somatosensory cortical regions that process palatability when they anticipated consumption (Stice et al., Reference Stice, Spoor, Bohon, Veldhuizen and Small2008). The trait of fixation to a reward may subserve the association between ADHD and obesity (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Hartman, Haavik, Harro, Klungsøyr, Hegvik, Wanders, Ottosen, Dalsgaard, Faraone and Larsson2018), as far as specifically Insatiability by Reward was the reward sensitivity component that is associated with ADHD symptoms (Pulver et al., Reference Pulver, Kiive and Harro2020a). Both facets could theoretically be associated with central dopaminergic neurotransmission, but their distinct nature is suggesting that variabilities in the genetically determined regulation of dopamine neurones (Gillies et al., Reference Gillies, Virdee, McArthur and Dalley2014; Ghosal et al., Reference Ghosal, Sandi and van der Kooij2019) underlie the independent variation in openness to multiple rewards and insatiability by a reward.

While it may be thought of as premature at this stage of investigation, it appears prudent to consider the public health implications of the findings. Previous research, from this laboratory among others, has suggested that efficacy of health behaviour interventions varies by individual differences in affective constructs (Paaver et al., Reference Paaver, Eensoo, Kaasik, Vaht, Mäestu and Harro2013). Thus, consideration of components of reward sensitivity may help to tailor promotional projects for dietary behaviour or physical activity. However, much more work needs to be done, including the investigation of the potential interaction of reward sensitivity components with genetic or environmental factors (e.g. socio-economic status).

Conclusively, making a distinction between the two independent aspects of reward sensitivity, striving towards multiple rewards and fixation on particular rewards, can reveal the relationship of reward sensitivity with lifestyle, metabolic markers and possibly metabolism efficiency and may also help to explain why so many studies on implications of reward sensitivity have not yielded in consistent findings. The highly distinct association of the two aspects with a number of metabolically important variables indirectly suggests that these (sub-)traits are an integral part of the physiological homeostatic machinery of an individual and, by their independent variability within population, support variation in metabolism and lifestyle. Further studies should aim at understanding of whether and how are these associations causal, first gaining knowledge on the underlying biological mechanisms in order to guide the design of experimental models.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all ECPBHS participants and the whole ECPBHS team.

Author contributions

EK, UK, TV and JH designed the study; EK, DE, IV and JM collected the data; UK and EK analysed the data; and all authors revised the paper critically for important intellectual content.

Funding statement

This study was funded by the Estonian Research Council (PRG1213).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

Baimel, C, Bartlett, SE, Chiou, LC, Lawrence, AJ, Muschamp, JW, Patkar, O, Tung, LW and Borgland, SL (2015) Orexin/hypocretin role in reward: implications for opioid and other addictions. British Journal of Pharmacology 172(2), 334348. DOI: 10.1111/bph.12639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barson, JR (2020) Orexin/hypocretin and dysregulated eating: promotion of foraging behavior. Brain Research 1731, 145915. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.08.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berland, C, Small, DM, Luquet, S and Gangaross, G (2021) Dietary lipids as regulators of reward processes: multimodal integration matters. Trends in Endocrinology and Metabolism 32(9), 693705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthoud, HR, Münzberg, H and Morrison, CD (2017) Blaming the brain for obesity: integration of hedonic and homeostatic mechanisms. Gastroenterology 152(7), 17281738. DOI: 10.1053/j.gastro.2016.12.050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaukopf, CL and DiGirolamo, GJ (2007) Reward, context, and human behaviour. The Scientific World Journal 7, 626640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blum, K, Cull, JG, Braverman, ER and Comings, DE (1996) Reward deficiency syndrome. American Scientist 84, 132145.Google Scholar
Blum, K, Thanos, PK and Gold, MS (2014) Dopamine and glucose, obesity, and reward deficiency syndrome. Frontiers in Psychology 5, Article–919. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, CS and White, TL (1994) Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67(2), 319333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Q, Hartman, CA, Haavik, J, Harro, J, Klungsøyr, K, Hegvik, TA, Wanders, R, Ottosen, C, Dalsgaard, S, Faraone, SV and Larsson, H (2018) Common psychiatric and metabolic comorbidity of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cross-sectional study. PLoS One 13(9), e0204516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, PJ (2016) Reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality questionnaires: structural survey with recommendations. Personality and Individual Differences 89, 6064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, PJ and Cooper, AJ (2016) The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): development and validation. Psychological Assessment 28(11), 14271440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Cock, N, Van Lippevelde, W, Vervoort, L, Vangeel, J, Maes, L, Eggermont, S, Braet, C, Lachat, C, Huybregts, L, Goossens, L, Beullens, K, Kolsteren, P and Van Camp, J (2016) Sensitivity to reward is associated with snack and sugar-sweetened beverage consumption in adolescents. European Journal of Nutrition 55(4), 16231632. DOI: 10.1007/s00394-015-0981-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denson, TF, von Hippel, W, Kemp, RI and Teo, LS (2010) Glucose consumption decreases impulsive aggression in response to provocation in aggressive individuals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46(6), 10231028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Detry, MA and Ma, Y (2016) Analyzing repeated measurements using mixed models. JAMA 315(4), 407408. DOI: 10.1001/jama.2015.19394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWall, CN, Deckman, T, Gailliot, MT and Bushman, BJ (2011) Sweetened blood cools hot tempers: physiological self-control and aggression. Aggressive Behavior 37(1), 7380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, RW Jr and Turek, C (2003) Glucose effects on a continuous performance test of attention in adults. Behavioural Brain Research 142(1-2), 217228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gailliot, MT and Baumeister, RF (2007) The physiology of willpower: linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review 11(4), 303327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghosal, S, Sandi, C and van der Kooij, MA (2019) Neuropharmacology of the mesolimbic system and associated circuits on social hierarchies. Neuropharmacology 159, 107498. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillies, GE, Virdee, K, McArthur, S and Dalley, JW (2014) Sex-dependent diversity in ventral tegmental dopaminergic neurons and developmental programing: a molecular, cellular and behavioral analysis. Neuroscience 282, 6985. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldschmidt, AB, Smith, KE, Lavender, JM, Engel, SG and Haedt-Matt, A (2019) Trait-level facets of impulsivity and momentary, naturalistic eating behavior in children and adolescents with overweight/obesity. Journal of Psychiatric Research 110, 2430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, JA (1970) The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research & Therapy 8(3), 249266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haapa, E, Toponen, T, Pietinen, P and Räsänen, L (1985) Annoskuvakirja (Picture book on food portions). Helsinki, Finland: Kansanterveyslaitos.Google Scholar
Harro, J (2010) Inter-individual differences in neurobiology as vulnerability factors for affective disorders: implications for psychopharmacology. Pharmacology & Therapeutics 125(3), 402422. DOI: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2009.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harro, J, Laas, K, Eensoo, D, Kurrikoff, T, Sakala, K, Vaht, M, Parik, J, Mäestu, J and Veidebaum, T (2019) Orexin/hypocretin gene (HCRTR1) variation is associated with aggressive behaviour. Neuropharmacology 156, 107527. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.02.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harro, M, Eensoo, D, Kiive, E, Merenäkk, L, Alep, J, Oreland, L and Harro, J (2001) Platelet monoamine oxidase in healthy 9- and 15-years old children: the effect of gender, smoking and puberty. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 25(8), 14971511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgs, S, Spetter, MS, Thomas, JM, Rotshtein, P, Lee, M, Hallschmid, M and Dourish, CT (2017) Interactions between metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control: implications for novel weight management therapies. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 31(11), 14601474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, DF, Hickman, RW, Al-Mohammad, A, Stasiak, A and Schultz, W (2024) Dopamine neurons encode trial-by-trial subjective reward value in an auction-like task. Nature Communications 15(1), 8138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollstein, T, Basolo, A, Ando, T, Votruba, SB, Walter, M, Krakoff, J and Piaggi, P (2020) Recharacterizing the metabolic state of energy balance in thrifty and spendthrift phenotypes. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 105(5), 13751392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holroyd, CB and Umemoto, A (2016) The research domain criteria framework: the case for anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 71, 418443. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ikemoto, S and Panksepp, J (1999) The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews 31(1), 641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonker, NC, van Malderen, E, Glashouwer, KA, Vervoort, L, Braet, C, Goossens, L and de Jong, PJ (2019) No differential reward responsivity and drive, punishment sensitivity or attention for cues signaling reward or punishment in adolescents with obesity. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 2363. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joost, U, Villa, I, Comasco, E, Oreland, L, Veidebaum, T and Harro, J (2019) Association between transcription factor AP-2B genotype, obesity, insulin resistance and dietary intake in a longitudinal birth cohort study. International Journal of Obesity 43(10), 20952106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katus, U, Villa, I, Ringmets, I, Pulver, A, Veidebaum, T and Harro, J (2020a) The role of reward sensitivity in obesity and its association with transcription factor AP-2B: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Neuroscience Letters 735, 135158. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katus, U, Villa, I, Ringmets, I, Vaht, M, Mäestu, E, Mäestu, J, Veidebaum and Harro, J (2020b) Association of FTO rs1421085 with obesity, diet, physical activity, and socioeconomic status: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Nutrition, Metabolism, and Cardiovascular Diseases 30(6), 948959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, SH, Yoon, H, Kim, H and Hamann, S (2015) Individual differences in sensitivity to reward and punishment and neural activity during reward and avoidance learning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10(9), 12191227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lätt, E, Jürimäe, J, Harro, J, Loit, H-M and Mäestu, J (2018) Low fitness is associated with metabolic risk independently of central adiposity in a cohort of 18-year-olds. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports 28(3), 10841091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahler, SV, Moorman, DE, Smith, RJ, James, MH and Aston-Jones, G (2014) Motivational activation: a unifying hypothesis of orexin/hypocretin function. Nature Neuroscience 17(10), 12981303. DOI: 10.1038/nn.3810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, DR, Hosker, JP, Rudenski, AS, Naylor, BA, Treacher, DF and Turner, RC (1985) Homeostasis model assessment: insulin resistance and beta-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man. Diabetologia 28(7), 412419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, CE, Rosander, J, Gottfried, J, Dennis, E and Dus, M (2020) Dietary sugar inhibits satiation by decreasing the central processing of sweet taste. eLife 9, e54530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miró-Padilla, A, Adrián-Ventura, J, Cherednichenko, A, Monzonís-Carda, I, Beltran-Valls, MR, MolinerUrdiales, D and Ávila, C (2023) Relevance of the anterior cingulate cortex volume and personality in motivated physical activity behaviors. Communications Biology 6(1), 1106. DOI: 10.1038/s42003-023-05423-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, EA, Wise, SP and Rhodes, SEV (2011) What can different brains do with reward?. In Gottfried, JA (ed.), Neurobiology of Sensation and Reward. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, pp 6197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine (2023) Dietary reference intakes for energy. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/26818.Google Scholar
Paaver, M, Eensoo, D, Kaasik, K, Vaht, M, Mäestu, J and Harro, J (2013) Preventing risky driving: a novel and efficient brief intervention focusing on acknowledgement of personal risk factors. Accident Analysis & Prevention 50, 430437. DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.05.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J (1998) Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaggi, P, Vinalis, KL, Basolo, A, Santini, F and Krakoff, J (2018) Energy expenditure in the etiology of human obesity: spendthrift and thrifty metabolic phenotypes and energy sensing mechanisms. Journal of Endocrinological Investigation 41(1), 8389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulver, A, Kiive, E and Harro, J (2020a) Reward sensitivity, affective neuroscience personality, symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and TPH2-703G/T (rs4570625) genotype. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 32(5), 247256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulver, A, Kiive, E, Kanarik, M and Harro, J (2020b) Association of orexin/hypocretin receptor gene (HCRTR1) with reward sensitivity, and interaction with gender. Brain Research 1746, 147013. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddon, H, Patel, Y, Turcotte, M, Pigeyre, M and Meyre, D (2018) Revisiting the evolutionary origins of obesity: lazy versus peppy-thrifty genotype hypothesis. Obesity Reviews 19(11), 15251543. DOI: 10.1111/obr.12742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riby, LM, Lai Teik Ong, D, Azmie, NBM, Ooi, EL, Regina, C, Yeo, EKW, Massa, J and Aquili, L (2017) Impulsiveness, postprandial blood glucose, and glucoregulation affect measures of behavioral flexibility. Nutrition Research (New York, N.Y.) 48, 6575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, TE and Berridge, KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews 18(3), 247291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruiz-Tejada, A, Neisewander, J and Katsanos, CS (2022) Regulation of voluntary physical activity behavior: a review of evidence involving dopaminergic pathways in the brain. Brain Sciences 12(3), 333. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12030333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholten, EW, Schrijvers, CT, Nederkoorn, C, Kremers, SP and Rodenburg, G (2014) Relationship between impulsivity, snack consumption and children’s weight. PLoS One 9(2), e88851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shahid, IZ, Rahman, AA and Pilowsky, PM (2012) Orexin and central regulation of cardiorespiratory system. Vitamins and Hormones 89, 159184. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394623-2.00009-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spear, L (2011) Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1(4), 390403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stice, E, Spoor, S, Bohon, C, Veldhuizen, MG and Small, DM (2008) Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 117(4), 924935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, RJ, Hagen, EH and Hammerstein, P (2008) Revealing the paradox of drug reward in human evolution. Proceedings. Biological Sciences 275(1640), 12311241.Google ScholarPubMed
Sutton, CA, L.’Insalata, AM and Fazzino, TL (2022) Reward sensitivity, eating behavior, and obesity-related outcomes: a systematic review. Physiology & Behavior 252, 113843. DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2022.113843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terracciano, A, Schrack, JA, Sutin, AR, Chan, W, Simonsick, EM and Ferrucci, L (2013) Personality, metabolic rate and aerobic capacity. PloS One 8(1), e54746. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tesmer, AL, Li, X, Bracey, E, Schmandt, C, Polania, R, Peleg-Raibstein, D and Burdakov, D (2024) Orexin neurons mediate temptation-resistant voluntary exercise. Nature Neuroscience 27(9), 17741782. DOI: 10.1038/s41593-024-01696-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomson, K, Merenäkk, L, Loit, HM, Mäestu, J and Harro, J (2011) The relationship between serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism and serum lipid levels at young age in a longitudinal population-representative study. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 35(8), 18571862. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsuneki, H, Wada, T and Sasaoka, T (2010) Role of orexin in the regulation of glucose homeostasis. Acta Physiologica (Oxford) 198, 335348. DOI: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.2009.02008.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Rijn, I, Griffioen-Roose, S, de Graaf, C and Smeets, PAM (2016) Neural processing of calories in brain reward areas can be modulated by reward sensitivity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 371. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandeweghe, L, Verbeken, S, Vervoort, L, Moens, E and Braet, C (2017) Reward sensitivity and body weight: the intervening role of food responsive behavior and external eating. Appetite 112, 150156. DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, ND, Wang, GJ, Fowler, JS, Tomasi, D and Baler, R (2012) Food and drug reward: overlapping circuits in human obesity and addiction. Current Topics in Behavioral Neuroscience 11, 124. DOI: 10.1007/7854_2011_169.Google Scholar
Walle, R, Petitbon, A, Fois, GR, Varin, C, Montalban, E, Hardt, L, Contini, A, Angelo, MF, Potier, M, Ortole, R, Oummadi, A, De Smedt-Peyrusse, V, Adan, RA, Giros, B, Chaouloff, F, Ferreira, G, de d’Exaerde, Kerchove A, Ducrocq, F, Georges, F and Trifilieff, P (2024) Nucleus accumbens D1- and D2-expressing neurons control the balance between feeding and activity-mediated energy expenditure. Nature Communications 15, 2543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, JCK (2017) Body composition and susceptibility to type 2 diabetes: an evolutionary perspective. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 71(7), 881889. DOI: 10.1038/ejcn.2017.31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiss, DA, Avena, N and Rada, P (2018) Sugar addiction: from evolution to revolution. Frontiers in Psychiatry 9, 545. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woessner, MN, Tacey, A, Levinger-Limor, A, Parker, AG, Levinger, P and Levinger, I (2021) The evolution of technology and physical inactivity: the good, the bad, and the way forward. Frontiers in Public Health 9, 655491. DOI: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.655491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, CT, Gonzalez Magaña, D, Roshgadol, J, Tian, L and Ryan, KK (2024) Dietary protein restriction diminishes sucrose reward and reduces sucrose-evoked mesolimbic dopamine signaling on mice. Appetite 9, 107673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Aspects of reward sensitivity and selected measures of metabolism and lifestyle at age 25. This illustrative analysis is based on quartile groups of rewards sensitivity facets. Blood glucose level (A), energy intake (B), physical activity score (C) and cardiovascular fitness by maximal power output (D) at age 25 years in ECPBHS participants with low (25%), medium (50%) or high (25%) reward sensitivity. *p < 0.05; **<0.005, ***<0.001 different from Low group. ¤p < 0.05; ¤¤p < 0.005 different from High group. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 1

Table 1. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in insulin resistance and food lipid concentration (mmol/l), from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model

Figure 2

Table 2. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in daily energy intake (kcal), nutrient intake (g/kg) and nutrient intake as a percentage from daily energy intake (E%) from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model

Figure 3

Table 3. Estimated main effects (mean and 95% CI) of the ECPBHS sample in blood pressure, cardiovascular fitness expressed as maximum power output (MPO; W/kg) and physical activity from 15 to 25 years of age by reward sensitivity score according to the linear mixed-effects regression model