Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T05:47:59.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Empirische indicatoren voor regressies en sprangen bij baby's

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2015

Extract

Het is al lang bekend dat het optreden van ontwikke-lingsmijlpalen sterk van baby tot baby kan verschillen. Theorieën of modellen die daarentegen een strakke timing, gepaard gaande met geringe individuele verschillen, voor ontwikkelingsverschijnselen postuleren, hebben dan ook een zware bewijslast te torsen. Het model van Van de Rijt-Plooij en Plooij van tien strak getimede regressieperioden tijdens de eerste achttien levensmaanden is een voorbeeld van een dergelijk model. In het licht van het feit dat het model mede ten grondslag ligt aan een wijd verspreid praktisch adviezenboek voor ouders, mag worden verwacht dat de empirische ondersteuning er-voor uitgebreid en hecht is. In deze bijdrage zullen we de empirische evidentie voor en tegen het model bespreken en ingaan op de methodologische problemen die bij het verifiëren dan wel falsifiëren van dit model kornen kijken.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Literatuur

1.Meulen, BF van der, Smrkovsky Bos, M. Bayley ontwikkelings-schalen. Handleiding. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger, 1983: 230Google Scholar
2.Rijt-Plooij, HCC van de, Plooij, FX. Infantile regressions: Disorganization and the onset of transition periods. J Reprod Inf Psychol 1992; 10: 129–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3.Rijt-Plooij, HCC van de, Plooij, FX. Distinct periods of mother-infant conflict in normal development: Sources of progress and germs of pathology. J Child Psychol Psychiat 1993; 34: 229–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Maas, H van der, Molenaar, PCM. Hopkins, B, Kalverboer, AF. Over de almanak van FX Plooij. De Psycholoog 1998; 33: 164–5.Google Scholar
5.Sadurni, M, Rostan, C. Regression periods in infancy: a case study from Catalonia. Paper presented at the Research Conference on ‘Regression Periods in Early Infancy’, Göteborg Univ, Sweden, 10 1997; 1011.Google Scholar
6.Lindahl, LB, Ullstadius, E, Heimann, M. Occurrence of regressive periods in the normal development of Swedish infants. Paper presented at the Research Conference on ‘Regression Periods in Early Infancy’, Göteborg Univ, Sweden, 10 1997; 1011.Google Scholar
7.Hadders-Algra, M. Transities in de ontwikkeling van het gedrag van baby's. Neuropraxis 1998; 2: 5152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8.Quandt, SA. Maternal recall accuracy for dates of infant feeding transitions. Human Organization 1987; 46: 152–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Bernard, HR, Killworth, P, Kronenfeld, D, Sailer, L. The problem of informant accuracy: The validity of retrospective data. Ann Rev Anthropol 1984.Google Scholar
10.St. James-Roberts, I, Wolke, D. Comparison of mothers' with trained-observers' reports of neonatal behavioral style. Inf Behav Develop 1984; 7: 299310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11.Vitzthum, VJ. Suckling patterns: Lack of concordance between maternal recall and observational data. Am J Human Biol 1994; 6: 551–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Bates, JE, Bayles, K. Objective and subjective components in mothers' perceptions of their children from age 6 months to 3 years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1984; 30: 111–30.Google Scholar
13.Kalverboer, L. Over transities in de ontwikkeling. Neuropraxis 1998; 2: 4350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.van Hof-van Duin, J. Over transities in de visuele ontwikkeling. Neuropraxis 1998; 2: 53–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.Weerth, C de, Geert, P van. Emotional instability as an indicator of strictly timed infantile developmental transitions. Br J develop Psychol 1998a.Google Scholar
16.Weerth, C de. Emotion-related behaviors in infants: a longitudinal study of patterns and variability. Groningen: Academisch proefschrift 1998.Google Scholar
17.Bates, E. Language and Context: Studies in the Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Acad Press; 1976.Google Scholar
18.Bloom, L. One word at a time: The use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton, 1973.Google Scholar
19.Emde, RN, Gaensbauer, Th. Some emerging models of emotion in human infancy. In: Immelmann, K, Barlow, GVV, Petrinovich, L, Main, M, eds. Behavioral Development. The Bielefeld Interdisciplinary Project, pp. 568588. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press; 1981.Google Scholar
20.Fischer, KW. A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychol Rev 1980; 87: 477531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21.Fischer, KW. Relations between brain and cognitive development. Child Develop 1987; 58: 623–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Kagan, J. The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books; 1984.Google Scholar
23.Konner, MJ. Relations among infants and juveniles in comparative perspective. Social Science Information 1976; 15:371402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24.Lamborn, SD, Fischer, KW. Optimal and functional levels in cognitive development: the individual's developmental range. ISSBD Newsletter 1988; 14: 14.Google Scholar
25.McCall, RB, Eichorn, DH, Hogarty, PS. Transitions in early mental development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1977;42(3), Serial No. 171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Siegler, RS. Mechanisms of cognitive development. Annual Rev Psychol 1989; 40:353–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Trevarthen, C. Sharing makes sense: Intersubjectivity and the making of an infant's meaning. In: Steele, R, Threadgold, T, eds. Language Topics: Essays in Honour of Michael Halliday. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins; 1988.Google Scholar
28.Volterra, V. From single communicative signal to linguistic combinations in hearing and deaf children. In: Montangero, J, Tryphon, A, Dionnet, S, eds. Symbolism and Knowledge, pp. 115132. Geneva: Archives Jean Piaget Found; 1987.Google Scholar
29.Zelazo, PR, Leonard, E. The dawn of active thought. In: Fischer, KW, ed. Levels and transitions in children's development. New directions for Child Development, Vol. 21. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1983.Google Scholar
30.Weerth, C de, Geert, P van. The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy. In: Lewis, MD, Granic, I, red. Emotion, development and self-organization. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press; 1998b.Google Scholar
31.McFarland, D. The Oxford Companion to Animal Behaviour. New York: Oxford Univ Press; 1981.Google Scholar
32.Plooij, FX, Rijt-Plooij, HHC van de. Vulnerable periods during infancy: hierarchically reorganized systems control, stress and disease. Ethol Sociobiol 1989; 10: 279–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33.PLooij, FX. Hersenveranderingen en ‘sprongen’ in de eerste 20 levensmaanden. Acta neuropsychiatrica 1998, 10: 63–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Spangler, G. The emergence of adrenocortical circadian function in newborns and infants and its relationship to sleep, feeding and maternal adrenocortical activity. Early human Develop 1991;25: 197208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Wilder, J. The law of initial value in neurology and psychiatry. J nerv ment Dis 1957; 125: 7386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Flinn, MV, England, B. Childhood stress and family environment. Cur Anthropol 1995; 35: 854–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37.Warner, R, Appleby, L, Whitton, A, Faragher, B. Demographic and obstetric risk factors for postnatal psychiatric morbidity. Br J Psychiat 1996; 168: 607–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38.Posner, NA, Unterman, RR, Williams, KN, Williams, GH. Screening for postpartum depression. An antepartum questionnaire. J reprod Med 1997; 42: 207–15.Google ScholarPubMed
39.Reighard, FT, Evans, ML. Use of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale in a southern, rural population in the United States. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiat 1995; 19: 1219–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
40.McGill, H, Burrows, VL, Holland, LA, Langer, HJ, Sweet, MA. Postnatal depression: a Christchurch study. New Zeal med J 1995; 108: 162–5Google ScholarPubMed
41.Beck, CT. The effects of postpartum depression on child development: A meta-analysis. Arch Psychiat Nursing 1998; 12: 1220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Murray, L, Fiori-Cowley, A, Hooper, R, Cooper, P. The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Develop 1996; 67:2512–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed