Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T22:38:00.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brainwaves

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2014

Anthony Harris*
Affiliation:
Discipline of Psychological Medicine, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
*
Dr Anthony Harris, Department of Psychiatry, Westmead Hospital, Westmead, NSW 2145, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Invited Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © 2006 Blackwell Munksgaard

This issue of Acta Neuropsychiatrica publishes the abstracts of the Australasian Society for Psychiatric Research (ASPR) annual conference for 2006. The society is the peak body of psychiatric and mental health researchers in Australia and New Zealand and brings together researchers across a wide range of professions in the pursuit of a greater understanding of psychiatric disorders and improvement in the treatments offered for them. As such, it has been a place where the basic science researcher can meet his/her colleague in clinical and epidemiological research and exchange ideas.

Brainwaves, the theme of the conference, plays upon metaphors of inspiration and of communication that are essential for the scientific work that we do, the ripple effect of ideas thrown into the pond of scientific thought propagating across the whole community of researchers. Successful and fruitful research can be achieved within the restricted focus of a single field; however, insights into more complex problems such as disease require communication across the ‘levels of explanation’Reference Mortimer and McKenna(1) in which we accrue our knowledge. This is one of the prime purposes of conferences such as ASPR that do not have a specialty focus.

Waves of synchronized cortical electrical activity may underlie the brain activation required for these moments of inspiration. The identification of multiple areas of discrete cognitive activity with functions as defined as the recognition of a specific name Reference Quiroga, Reddy, Kreiman, Koch and Fried(2) or the definition of contour as against form or movement for a simple visual percept has emphasized again the ‘binding problem’ of how these discrete and often distant areas of the brain can be rapidly coordinated to produce coherent cognition. The formation and modulation of connectivity between regions has been seen as the possible mechanism underlying cognition and consciousness Reference Hebb(3,Reference Revonsuo4). The level of connectivity between areas has been determined either as a function of the covariation or correlation of power, phase or activation between regions using techniques such as electroencephalography Reference Schack, Grieszbach and Krause(5), evoked potentials Reference Haig, Gordon, Wright, Meares and Bahramali(6), positron emission tomography Reference Friston, Frith, Liddle and Frackowiak(7) and functional magnetic resonance imaging Reference McGuire and Frith(8), or as an effective connectivity, an estimate of the direct influence that one neural system exerts upon another Reference Friston(9). These measures provide a means of determining the network of brain regions that together are responsible for cognition but do not of themselves describe how this occurs.

Singer and Gray Reference Singer and Gray(10) have suggested that synchronous high-frequency (40 Hz) activity may be an index if not a mechanism for the binding of different cell assemblies together. High-frequency coincident bursts of presynaptic potentials, at the peaks of gamma rhythm, has been related to processing of different perceptual characteristics in the visual cortex Reference Gray and Singer(11,Reference Lisman12). The activation of a network of cells, recognized by the ‘coincidence detection’ of their simultaneous firing, increases the reliability and the informational value of the cellular activity many fold Reference Lisman(12). These bursts of activity appear to delineate the limits of perception with peaks of gamma activity accompanying the ability to perceive and discriminate auditory phenomena Reference Joliot, Ribary and Llins(13). However, it seems unlikely that information transfer or neuronal cell assembly coordination should be the province of such a restricted frequency band. Functional activity clearly extends over a broad range of gamma (30–100 Hz) for visual perception Reference Siegel and Konig(14). Further, work over several decades has implicated alteration in other frequency bands in numerous cognitive and motoric actions. The interplay between alpha and beta synchronization/desynchronization has been used to track selective attention Reference Pfurtscheller(15), and the preparation and execution of movement, both imagined Reference Pfurtscheller, Neuper, Flotzinger and Pregenzer(16) and actual Reference Pfurtscheller(15). The manipulation of cognitive load or stage of processing in working memory tasks sees marked alteration in the interplay of theta and alpha power, desynchronization Reference Klimesch, Schack and Sauseng(17) and phase Reference Schack, Klimesch and Sauseng(18). The distance over which integration of cognition is to occur influences and may determine the frequency band used by the brain. At the local scale, visual phenomena elicit an increase in gamma power and synchrony over the occipital cortex but not over other brain regions. Supramodal integration of sensory inputs requiring interaction of areas across the brain elicited synchronization in the beta 1 frequency range, with possible contributions within the gamma band. Longer range interactions in a working memory task involving prefrontal and posterior cortical areas elicit theta activity Reference von Stein and Sarnthein(19).

What happens when the free flow of this information is disturbed as is seen in schizophrenia Reference Friston(20)? Schizophrenia research has identified very many areas of deficit, none of which alone has been able to account for the manifestations of the disease. A connectivity hypothesis allows one to account for this, as Friston comments ‘… the symptoms and signs of schizophrenia do not generally represent a single deficit but can be seen as resulting from the abnormal integration of two or more processes’Reference Friston(20). Genes identified as plausible susceptibility genes particularly affect glutamatergic transmission and synaptic plasticity Reference Harrison and Weinberger(21), having obvious implications for cortical communication, but alone may be unable to explain the multitude of clinical and experimental deficits. A conception of schizophrenia as a disorder of connectivity allows for a broad conception of polygenetic, epigenetic and environmental insults coming together to compromise the connectivity and coordination of the brain network, emphasizing the interactivity of these factors and potentially spanning some of the levels of explanation for schizophrenia that have existed independent of each other.

References

Mortimer, AM, McKenna, PJ. Levels of explanation – symptoms, neuropsychological deficit and morphological abnormalities in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1994;24:541545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quiroga, RQ, Reddy, L, Kreiman, G, Koch, C, Fried, I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 2005;435:11021107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, DO. The organization of behaviour. New York: Wiley, 1949.Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. Inner presence: consciousness as a biological phenomenon, 1st edition. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2006.Google Scholar
Schack, B, Grieszbach, G, Krause, W. The sensitivity of instantaneous coherence for considering elementary comparison processing. Part I: the relationship between mental activities and instantaneous EEG coherence. Int J Psychophysiol 1999;31:219240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haig, AR, Gordon, E, Wright, JJ, Meares, RA, Bahramali, H. Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition. NeuroReport 2000;11:669675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, KJ, Frith, CD, Liddle, PF, Frackowiak, RSJ. Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1993;13:514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, PK, Frith, CD. Disordered functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1996;26:663667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum Brain Mapp 1994;2:5678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, W, Gray, CM. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci 1995;18:555586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, CM, Singer, W. Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1989;86:16981702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisman, JE. Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliable. Trends Neurosci 1997;20:3843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joliot, M, Ribary, U, Llins, R. Human oscillatory brain activity near 40 Hz coexists with cognitive temporal binding. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1994;91:1174811751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, M, Konig, P. A Functional Gamma-Band Defined by Stimulus-Dependent Synchronization in Area 18 of Awake Behaving Cats. J Neurosci 2003;23:42514260.Google ScholarPubMed
Pfurtscheller, G. Event-related synchronisation (ERS): an electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1992;83:6269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfurtscheller, G, Neuper, C, Flotzinger, D, Pregenzer, M. EEG-based discrimination between imagination of right and left hand movement. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1997;103:642651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klimesch, W, Schack, B, Sauseng, P. The functional significance of theta and upper alpha oscillations. Exp Psychol 2005;52:99108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schack, B, Klimesch, W, Sauseng, P. Phase synchronisation between theta and upper alpha oscillations in a working memory task. Int J Psychophysiol 2005;57:105114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Stein, A, Sarnthein, J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha theta synchronisation. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;38:301313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, KJ. The disconnection hypothesis. Schizophr Res 1998;30:115125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, PJ, Weinberger, DR. Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence. Mol Psychiatry 2005;10:4068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar