Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-h6jzd Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2025-03-02T00:29:36.903Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Universality of Emotion

Perspectives from the Sciences and Humanities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2025

Bradley J. Irish
Affiliation:
Arizona State University

Summary

Not supplied by the author. This Element surveys how a number of major disciplines − psychology, neuroscience, sociology, anthropology, philosophy, history, linguistics, and literary/cultural studies − have addressed the long-standing research question of whether human emotions should be thought of as meaningfully 'universal.' The Element presents both the universalist and anti-universalist positions, and concludes by considering attempts to move beyond this increasingly unhelpful binary.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009442534
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 06 February 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Lughod, L. (1986). Veiled sentiments: Honor and poetry in a Bedouin society. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Abu-Lughod, L., & Lutz, C. A. (1990). Introduction: Emotion, discourse, and the politics of everyday life. In Lutz, C. A., & Abu-Lughod, L. (Eds.), Language and the politics of emotion (pp. 123). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2017). How should neuroscience study emotions? By distinguishing emotion states, concepts, and experiences. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., & Anderson, D. J. (2018). The neuroscience of emotion: A new synthesis. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ahern, S. (2024). Affect theory and literary criticism. Emotion Review, 16(2), 96106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahmed, S. (2004). The cultural politics of emotion. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Amberber, M. (2001). Testing emotional universals in Amharic. In Harkins, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds.), Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective (pp. 3568). Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariza, M. (2021). The sociology of emotions in Latin America. Annual Review of Sociology, 47(1), 157175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armon-Jones, C. (1986a). The social functions of emotion. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The social construction of emotions (pp. 5782). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Armon-Jones, C. (1986b). The thesis of constructionism. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The social construction of emotions (pp. 3256). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Armony, J., & Vuilleumier, P. (Eds.). (2013). The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, P. B. (2020). Stories and the brain: The neuroscience of narrative. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist, 63(7), 602614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Asher, K. G. (2017). Literature, ethics, and the emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auracher, J., Albers, S., Zhai, Y., Gareeva, G., & Stavniychuk, T. (2010). P is for happiness, N is for sadness: Universals in sound iconicity to detect emotions in poetry. Discourse Processes, 48(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1980). A constructivist view of emotion. In Plutchik, R., & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 305339). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1994). In the eyes of the beholder. In Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 714). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (2012). The future of social constructionism: Introduction to a special section of Emotion Review. Emotion Review, 4(3), 215220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., et al. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19(7), 724732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Bentin, S., & Trope, Y. (2008). Putting facial expressions back in context. In Ambady, N., & Skowronski, J. J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 255286). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Azari, B., Westlin, C., Satpute, A. B., et al. (2020). Comparing supervised and unsupervised approaches to emotion categorization in the human brain, body, and subjective experience. Scientific Reports, 10(1), article 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bamberg, M. (1997a). Emotion talk(s): The role of perspective in the construction of emotions. In Niemeier, S., & Dirven, R. (Eds.), The language of emotions (pp. 209225). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M. (1997b). Language, concepts and emotions: The role of language in the construction of emotions. Language Sciences, 19(4), 309340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbalet, J. M. (1998). Emotion, social theory, and social structure: A macrosociological approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, K. (2020). The history of emotions: A student guide to methods and sources. London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, K. (2021a). Emotions in the history of emotions. History of Psychology, 24(2), 112115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barclay, K. (2021b). State of the field: The history of emotions. History, 106(371), 456466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimensions? The role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition and Emotion, 12(4), 579599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 2858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 2046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2012). Emotions are real. Emotion, 12(3), 413429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2013). Psychological construction: The Darwinian approach to the science of emotion. Emotion Review, 5(4), 379389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017a). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017b). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2022). Context reconsidered: Complex signal ensembles, relational meaning, and population thinking in psychological science. American Psychologist, 77(8), 894920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., & Lida, T. (2024). Constructionist theories of emotion in psychology and neuroscience. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 350387.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (1999). The structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 1014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2019). Historical pitfalls and new directions in the neuroscience of emotion. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). On the automaticity of emotion. In Bargh, J. A. (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 173217). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., et al. (2007). Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 297312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., & Barsalou, L. W. (2015). The conceptual act theory: A roadmap. In Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The psychological construction of emotion (pp. 83110). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.). (2016). Handbook of emotions: Fourth edition. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, G., & Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character: A photographic analysis. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Beatty, A. (2013). Current emotion research in anthropology: Reporting the field. Emotion Review, 5(4), 414422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty, A. (2014). Anthropology and emotion. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 20(3), 545563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty, A. (2019). Emotional worlds: Beyond an anthropology of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behar, R. (1996). Vulnerable observer: Anthropology that breaks your heart. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1935). Patterns of culture. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bericat, E. (2016). The sociology of emotions: Four decades of progress. Current Sociology, 64(3), 491513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Besnier, N. (1990). Language and affect. Annual Review of Anthropology, 19, 419451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, B. (2014). Linguistic diversity and universals. In Enfield, N. J., Kockelman, P., & Sidnell, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic anthropology (pp. 102127). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilimoria, P., & Wenta, A. (Eds.). (2015). Emotions in Indian thought-systems. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Birx, J. H. (Ed.). (2010). 21st century anthropology: A reference handbook. New York: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliss-Moreau, E., Williams, L. A., & Karaskiewicz, C. L. (2021). Evolution of emotion in social context. In Shackelford, T. K., & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp. 24872499). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, F. (1910). Psychological problems in anthropology. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 371384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boddice, R. (2017). The history of emotions: Past, present, future. Revista de Estudios Sociales, 62, 1015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boddice, R. (2018). The history of emotions. In Handley, S., McWilliam, R., & Noakes, L. (Eds.), New directions in social and cultural history (pp. 4563). London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boddice, R. (2019). The developing brain as historical artifact. Developmental Psychology, 55(9), 19941997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boddice, R. (2020a). From the ashes, a fertile opportunity for historicism. History of the Human Sciences, 33(2), 126133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boddice, R. (2020b). History looks forward: Interdisciplinarity and critical emotion research. Emotion Review, 12(3), 131134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boddice, R. (2024). The history of emotion. 2nd ed. Manchester: Manchester University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012a). Emotion science needs to account for the social world. Emotion Review, 4(3), 236237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012b). The construction of emotion in interactions, relationships, and cultures. Emotion Review, 4(3), 221229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2015). A sociodynamic perspective on the construction of emotion. In Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The psychological construction of emotion (pp. 377398). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Boiger, M., Ceulemans, E., De Leersnyder, J., et al. (2018). Beyond essentialism: Cultural differences in emotions revisited. Emotion, 18(8), 11421162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bound Alberti, F. (2018). This “modern epidemic”: Loneliness as an emotion cluster and a neglected subject in the history of emotion. Emotion Review, 10(3), 242254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Brady, M. S. (2024). An overview of contemporary theories of emotion in philosophy. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 215231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briggs, J. L. (1970). Never in anger: Portrait of an Eskimo family. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, J. A., Shablack, H., Gendron, M., et al. (2017). The role of language in the experience and perception of emotion: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 169183.Google ScholarPubMed
Brooks, J. A., Chikazoe, J., Sadato, N., & Freeman, J. B. (2019). The neural representation of facial-emotion categories reflects conceptual structure. PNAS, 116(32), 1586115870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brothers, L. (1997). Friday’s footprint: How society shapes the human mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, D. (2001). Human universals. In Wilson, R. A., & Keil, F. C. (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 382383). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (2000). Human universals and their implications. In Roughley, N. (Ed.), Being humans: Anthropological universality and particularity in transdisciplinary perspective (pp. 156174). New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (2015). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. 2 vols. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2018). Sexual and emotional infidelity: Evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 155160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campeggiani, P. (2023). Theories of emotion: Expressing, feeling, acting. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Fatani, S. S., Fraumeni, B. R., & Shuster, M. M. (2016). The development of facial expressions: Current perspectives on infant emotions. In Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, Handbook of emotions, pp. 255271.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. (2018). Evolutionary literary theory. In Richter, D. H. (Ed.), A companion to literary theory (pp. 425438). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. (2022a). Evolution: How evolved emotions work in literary meaning. In Hogan, Irish, & Hogan, Routledge Companion, pp. 8597.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. (2022b). Narrative theory and neuroscience: Why human nature matters. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 6(2), 81100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J., Clasen, M., Jonsson, E., et al. (2018). Biocultural theory: The current state of knowledge. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J., Clasen, M., & Jonsson, E. (Eds.). (2020). Evolutionary perspectives on imaginative culture. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 205218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, N. (2022). Philosophy, literature, and emotion. In Hogan, Irish, & Hogan, Routledge Companion, pp. 110120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celeghin, A., Diano, M., Bagnis, A., Viola, M., & Tamietto, M. (2017). Basic emotions in human neuroscience: Neuroimaging and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, article 1432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C. (1995). Feeling and representing: Computational theory and the modularity of affect. Synthese, 105(3), 273301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C. (2001). In defence of “emotion.” Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 31(1), 133154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C. (2002). The natural kind status of emotion. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 53(4), 511537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 334347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Gold, A., Gomes, A., & Ryder, A. G. (2020). Feelings in the body: Cultural variations in the somatic concomitants of affective experience. Emotion, 20(8), 14901494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, J. Y. (Ed.). (2009). Cultural neuroscience: Cultural influences on brain function. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y. (2015). Current emotion research in cultural neuroscience. Emotion Review, 7(3), 280293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, J. Y. (2018). Philosophy of cultural neuroscience. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., & Immordino-Yang, M. H. (2013). Modularity and the cultural mind: Contributions of cultural neuroscience to cognitive theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(1), 5661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, J. Y., Li, S.-C., Seligman, R., & Turner, R. (Eds.). (2016). The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Church, T., Katigbak, M. S., Reyes, J. A. S., & Jensen, S. M. (1998). Language and organisation of Filipino emotion concepts: Comparing emotion concepts and dimensions across cultures. Cognition and Emotion, 12(1), 6392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciofalo, N. (Ed.). (2019). Indigenous psychologies in an era of decolonization. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clanton, G. (2019). Envy: Hostility towards superiors. In Jacobsen, Emotions, everyday life and sociology, pp. 142156.Google Scholar
Clark, J. A. (2010). Relations of homology between higher cognitive emotions and basic emotions. Biology & Philosophy, 25(1), 7594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark-Polner, E., Wager, T. D., Satpute, A. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Neural fingerprinting: Meta-analysis, variation, and the search for brain-based essences in the science of emotion. In Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, Handbook of emotions, pp. 146165.Google Scholar
Clark-Polner, E., Johnson, T. D., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Multivoxel pattern analysis does not provide evidence to support the existence of basic emotions. Cerebral Cortex, 27(3), 19441948.Google Scholar
Clore, G. R. (2018). The impact of affect depends on its object. In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions, 2nd ed. (pp. 186189). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2019). The emotional mind: A control theory of affective states. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A., & Stern, S. (Eds.). (2017). Thinking about the emotions: A philosophical history. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2009). From affect programs to dynamical discrete emotions. Philosophical Psychology, 22(4), 407425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2013). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Comer, C., & Taggart, A. (2021). Brain, mind, and the narrative imagination. London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cordaro, D. (2024). Basic emotion theory: A beginner’s guide. In Al-Shawaf, L., & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolution and the emotions (pp. 320). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cordaro, D. T., Sun, R., Keltner, D. et al. (2018). Universals and cultural variations in 22 emotional expressions across five cultures. Emotion, 18(1), 7593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordaro, D. T., Sun, R., Kamble, S. et al. (2020). The recognition of 18 facial-bodily expressions across nine cultures. Emotion, 20(7), 12921300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, second edition (pp. 91115). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 201229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(38), E7900E7909.Google ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2018). Clarifying the conceptualization, dimensionality, and structure of emotion: Response to Barrett and colleagues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 274276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2020). What the face displays: Mapping 28 emotions conveyed by naturalistic expression. American Psychologist, 75(3), 349364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2021). Semantic space theory: A computational approach to emotion. Trends in Cognitive Science, 25(2), 124136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., Elfenbein, H. A., Laukka, P., & Keltner, D. (2019). Mapping 24 emotions conveyed by brief human vocalization. American Psychologist, 74(6), 698712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., Sauter, D., Tracy, J. L., & Keltner, D. (2019). Mapping the passions: Toward a high-dimensional taxonomy of emotional experience and expression. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 6990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowen, A. S., Keltner, D., Schroff, F. et al. (2021). Sixteen facial expressions occur in similar contexts worldwide. Nature, 589(7841), 251257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, L. E. (2009). Conceptual metaphors of affect. Emotion Review, 1(2), 129139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crivelli, C., Jarillo, S., Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2016). Reading emotions from faces in two indigenous societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 145(7), 830843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A., & Stillman, P. E. (2013). Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review, 5(4), 344355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1995). Toward a neurobiology of emotion and feeling: Operational concepts and hypotheses. The Neuroscientist, 1(1), 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 143152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daneš, F. (2004). Universality vs. cultural-specificity of emotion. In Weigand, E. (Ed.), Emotion in dialogic interaction (pp. 2332). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1990). Generative metaphor and the history of psychological discourse. In Leary, D. E. (Ed.), Metaphors in the history of psychology (pp. 331356). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, J., & Spencer, D. (Eds.). (2010). Emotions in the field: The psychology and anthropology of fieldwork experience. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2021). What has culture got to do with emotions? (A lot). In Gelfand, M. J., Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (Eds.), Handbook of advances in culture and psychology (pp. 62119). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Sousa, R. (1987). The rationality of emotion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degli Esposti, M., Altmann, E., & Pachet, F. (2016). Creativity and universality in language. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deigh, J. (Ed.). (2013). On emotions: Philosophical essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLancey, C. (2002). Passionate engines: What emotions reveal about mind and artificial intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demertzis, N. (2020). The political sociology of emotions: Essays on trauma and ressentiment. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deonna, J. A., & Teroni, F. (2012). The emotions: A philosophical introduction. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M. (2010). Emotions in multiple languages. New York: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, T. (2020). What is the history of anger a history of? Emotions: History, Culture, Society, 4(1), 134.Google Scholar
Dodds, P. S., Clark, E. M., Desu, S., et al. (2015). Human language reveals a universal positivity bias. PNAS, 112(8), 23892394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodman, T. (2021). Theories and methods in the history of emotions. In Barclay, K., Crozier-De Rosa, S., & Stearns, P. N. (Eds.), Sources for the history of emotions: A guide (pp. 1525). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Downey, G., & Gillett, A. J. (2023). Linguistic relativity in cross-cultural context: Converging evidence from neuroanthropology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 15(4), 693697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyle, C. M., & Lindquist, K. A. (2018). When a word is worth a thousand pictures: Language shapes perceptual memory for emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 6273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, C. M., Lane, S. T., Brooks, J. A., et al. (2022). Unsupervised classification reveals consistency and degeneracy in neural network patterns of emotion. Social Cognitive and Affect Neuroscience, 17(11), 9951006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durán, J. I., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2021). Do emotions result in their predicted facial expressions? A meta-analysis of studies on the co-occurrence of expression and emotion. Emotion, 21(7), 15501569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durán, J. I., Reisenzein, R., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2017). Coherence between emotions and facial expressions: A research synthesis. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 107129). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Durst, U. (2003). The natural semantic metalanguage approach to linguistic meaning. Theoretical Linguistics, 29(3), 157200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1971). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 19, 207283.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3/4), 169200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In Dalgleish, T., & Power, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 4560). New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (2016). What scientists who study emotion agree about. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 3134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221(4616), 12081210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgments of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(4), 712717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellsworth, P. C. (2024). Appraisal theories of emotions. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 331349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emanatian, M. (1995). Metaphor and the expression of emotion: The value of cross-cultural perspectives. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 10(3), 163182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, A. L. (1992). In the midst of life: Affect and ideation in the world of the Tolai. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Eustace, N. (2008). Passion is the gale: Emotion, power, and the coming of the American revolution. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Eustace, N., Lean, E., Livingston, J., et al. (2012). AHR conversation: The historical study of emotion. American Historical Review, 117, 14871531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fang, X., Sauter, D. A., Heerdink, M. W., & van Kleef, G. A. (2022). Culture shapes the distinctiveness of posed and spontaneous facial expressions of anger and disgust. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 53(5), 471487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B. (1988). Prototype analysis of the concepts of love and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 557579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B. (1994). Prototype-based assessment of laypeople’s views of love. Personal Relationships, 1(4), 309331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B. (2005). The role of prototypes in interpersonal cognition. In Baldwin, M. W. (Ed.), Interpersonal cognition (pp. 180205). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(3), 464486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1991). The concept of love viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 425438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. A. (1995a). Variations in the circumplex structure of mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(8), 806817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. A. (1995b). Valence focus and arousal focus: Individual differences in the structure of affective experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 153166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firth-Godbehere, R. (2021). A human history of emotion: How the way we feel built the world we know. New York: Little, Brown, Spark.Google Scholar
Floman, J. L., Brackett, M. A., LaPalme, M. L., et al. (2023). Development and validation of an ability measure of emotion understanding: The core relational themes of emotion (CORE) test. Journal of Intelligence, 11, article 195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontaine, J. J. R., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (Eds.). (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (2018). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Franks, D. D. (2010). Neurosociology. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Parrott, W. G. (2011). Basic emotions or Ur-emotions? Emotion Review, 3(4), 406415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., & Wojciehowski, H. C. (2018). Embodiment and universals. Literary Universals Project. https://literary-universals.uconn.edu/2018/09/25/embodiment-and-universals.Google Scholar
Gallois, C., Vanman, E. J., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Greenaway, K. H. (2021). Emotion and its management: The lens of language and social psychology. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 40(1), 4259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (2006). Words and other wonders: Papers on lexical and semantic topics. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1962). The growth of culture and the evolution of mind. In Scher, J. M. (Ed.), Theories of the mind (pp. 713740). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1966). The impact of the concept of culture on the concept of man. In Platt, J., (Ed.), New views of the nature of man (pp. 93118). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1975). On the nature of anthropological understanding. American Scientist, 63(1), 4753.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1980). Negara: The theatre state in nineteenth-century Bali. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, H. (1959). The vocabulary of emotion: A study of Javanese socialization processes. Psychiatry, 22(3), 225237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gendron, M. (2017). Revisiting diversity: Cultural variation reveals the constructed nature of emotion perception. Current Opinions in Psychology, 17, 145150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gendron, M., Lindquist, K. A., Barsalou, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). Emotion words shape emotion percepts. Emotion, 12(2), 314325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gendron, M., Roberson, D., Vyver, Van Der, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: Evidence from a remote culture. Emotion, 14(2), 251262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in making meaning of facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 211219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerber, E. R. (1975). The cultural patterning of emotions in Samoa. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Gerber, E. R. (1985). Rage and obligation: Samoan emotion in conflict. In White, G. M., & Kirkpatrick, J. (Eds.), Person, self, and experience: Exploring pacific ethnopsychologies (pp. 121167). Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gladkova, A., Vanhatalo, U., & Goddard, C. (2016). The semantics of interjections: An experimental study with natural semantic metalanguage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(4), 841865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, C. (2014). On “disgust.” In Baider, F. H., & Cislaru, G. (Eds.), Linguistic approaches to emotions in context (pp. 7398). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, C. (2015). The complex, language-specific semantics of “surprise.” Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 13(2), 291313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, C. (2021). Natural semantic metalanguage. In Xu, W., & Taylor, J. R. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 93–110). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 351374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldie, P. (2002). The emotions: A philosophical exploration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldie, P. (Ed.). (2010). Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, S. L. (1981). The sociology of sentiments and emotion. In Rosenberg, M., & Turner, R. H. (Eds.), Social psychology: Sociological perspectives (pp. 551575). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gordon, S. L. (1990). Social structural effects on emotions. In Kemper, T. D. (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotions (pp. 145179). Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997). Foundations of meaning: Primary metaphors and primary scenes. PhD Thesis, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (2007). Metaphor. In Geeraerts, D., & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 188213). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. (1975). Research on language universals. Annual Review of Anthropology, 4, 7594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg, M., & Seigworth, G. J. (Eds.). (2010). The affect theory reader. Durham: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (2001). Emotion and the problem of psychological categories. In Kaszniak, A. W. (Ed.), Emotions, qualia, and consciousness (pp. 2841). Singapore: World Scientific.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (2004a). Emotions as natural and normative kinds. Philosophy of Science, 71(5), 901911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (2004b). Is emotion a natural kind? In Solomon, R. C. (Ed.), Thinking about feeling: Contemporary philosophers on emotions (pp. 233249). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (2017). Current emotion research in philosophy. In Kingston, R., Banerjee, K., McKee, J., Y. -Chien, C., and Vassiliou, C. C. (Eds.), Emotions, community, and citizenship (pp. 107125). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Grima, B. (1992). The performance of emotion among Paxtun women. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Gross, D. M. (2010). Defending the humanities with Charles Darwin’s The expression of the emotions in man and animals (1872). Critical Inquiry, 37(1), 3459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guillory, S. A., & Bujarski, K. A. (2014). Exploring emotions using invasive methods: Review of 60 years of human intracranial electrophysiology. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 11801889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S. (2017). The sociocultural brain: A cultural neuroscience approach to human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, S., Northoff, G., Vogeley, K., et al. (2013). A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 335359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkins, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds.). (2001). Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harré, R. (1986). An outline of the social constructivist viewpoint. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The social construction of emotions (pp. 214). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harré, R. (2000). Encountering the other through grammar. In Roughley, N. (Ed.), Being humans: Anthropological universality and particularity in transdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 107130). Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Harré, R., & Llored, J. P. (2018). Contingent universals as the expression of a culture. In Jovanović, G., Allolio-Näcke, L., & Ratner, C. (Eds.), The challenges of cultural psychology: Historical legacies and future responsibilities (pp. 189206). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Harris, S. R. (2015). An invitation to the sociology of emotions. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, K. G. (2011). The cultural context of emotion: Folk psychology in West Sumatra. New York: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, D. J., & Raybeck, D. (1981). Similarities and differences in meaning in six cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 12(2), 194206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (Eds.). (2019). The social nature of emotion expression: What emotions can tell us about the world. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochschild, A. (1983a). Comment on Kemper’s “Social constructionist and positivist approaches to the sociology of emotion.” American Journal of Sociology, 89(2), 432434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85(3), 551575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1983b). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hochschild Denzin, N. K. (1990). On understanding emotion: The interpretive-cultural agenda. In Kemper, T. D. (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotion (pp. 85116). Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Hoemann, K., Khan, Z., Feldman, M. J., et al. (2020). Context-aware experience sampling reveals the scale of variation in affective experience. Science Report, 10(1), article 12459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoemann, K., Gendron, M., Crittenden, A. N., et al. (2024). What we can learn about emotion by talking with the Hadza. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 19(1), 173200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, L. P. (2011). Dhvani and rasa. In Hogan, P. (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of the language sciences (pp. 251252). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2003). The mind and its stories: Narrative universals and human emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2008a). For evolutionary criticism, against genetic absolutism. Style, 42(2–3), 202206.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2008b). Of literary universals: Ninety-five theses. Philosophy and Literature, 32(1), 145160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2011a). Affective narratology: The emotional structure of stories. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2011b). What literature teaches us about emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2013). How authors’ minds make stories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2018). Literature and emotion. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2022). Stories: Particular causes and universal genres. In Hogan, Irish, & Hogan, Routledge companion, pp. 328339.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. C., Irish, B. J., & Hogan, L. P. (Eds.). (2022). The Routledge companion to literature and emotion. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horikawa, T., Cowen, A. S., Keltner, D., & Kamitani, Y. (2020). The neural representation of visually evoked emotion is high-dimensional, categorical, and distributed across transmodal brain regions. iScience, 23(5), article 101060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y.-A., Jastorff, J., Van den Stock, J., et al. (2018). Studying emotion theories through connectivity analysis: Evidence from generalized psychophysiological interactions and graph theory. NeuroImage, 172, 260262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntington, R., & Metcalf, P. (1979). Celebrations of death: The anthropology of mortuary ritual. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hupka, R. B., Lenton, A. P., & Hutchison, K. A. (1999). Universal development of emotion categories in natural language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 247278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H. C., & Matsumoto, D. (2015). Evidence for the universality of facial expressions of emotion. In Mandal, M. K., & Awasthi, A. (Eds.), Understanding facial expressions in communication: Cross-cultural and multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 4156). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H. C., & Matsumoto, D. (2020). Cross-cultural emotional expression. In Carducci, B. J., & Nave, C. S. (Eds.), The Wiley encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. 4 vols. (pp. 4.257263). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Illouz, E., & Wilf, E. (2009). Hearts of wombs? A cultural critique of radical feminist critiques of love. In Hopkins, D., Kleres, J., Flam, H., and Kuzmics, H. (Eds.), Theorizing emotions: sociological explorations and applications (pp. 121142). New York: Campus Verlag.Google Scholar
Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Yang, X.-F. (2017). Cultural differences in the neural correlates of social-emotional feelings: An interdisciplinary, developmental perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 3440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Immordino-Yang, M. H., Yang, X. F., & Damasio, H. (2014). Correlations between social-emotional feelings and anterior insula activity are independent from visceral states but influenced by culture. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, article 728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irish, B. J. (2018). Emotion in the Tudor court: Literature, history, and early modern feeling. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irish, B. J. (2020). A strategic compromise: Universality, interdisciplinarity, and the case for modal emotions in history of emotion research. Emotions: History, Culture, Society, 4(2), 231251.Google Scholar
Irish, B. J. (2023). Shakespeare and disgust: The history and science of early modern revulsion. London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Israelashvili, J., Hassin, R. R., & Aviezer, H. (2019). When emotions run high: A critical role for context in the unfolding of dynamic, real-life facial affect. Emotion, 19(3), 558562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 288299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 260280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (2011). Forms and functions of emotions: Matters of emotion-cognition interactions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 371378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. PNAS, 109(19), 72417244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, M. H. (Ed.). (2019). Emotions, everyday life and sociology. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, M. H., & Kristiansen, S. (2019). Embarrassment: Experience awkward self-awareness in everyday life. In Jacobsen, Emotions, everyday life and sociology, pp. 104125.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9(34), 188205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. 2 vols. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (2022). How poetry evokes emotions. Acta Psychologica, 224, article 103506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, E., & Olson, L. (2015). The feeling brain: The biology and psychology of emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Jonsson, E. (2020). Evolutionary literary theory. In Shackelford, T. K. (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 403420). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.Google Scholar
Jonsson, E. (2021). The early evolutionary imagination: Literature and human nature. New York: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 770814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamiloğlu, R. G., Cong, Y., Sun, R., & Sauter, D. A. (2024). Emotions across cultures. In Al-Shawaf, L., & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolution and the emotions (pp. 983996). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keith, K. D. (2019). Psychology and culture: An introduction. In Keith, K. D. (Ed.), Cross-cultural psychology: Contemporary themes and perspectives (pp. 322). New York: Wiley Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Cordaro, D. T. (2017). Understanding multimodal emotional expressions: Recent advances in basic emotion theory. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 5775). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006). Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A., & Kenrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 115142). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Sauter, D., Tracy, J., & Cowen, A. (2019). Emotional expression: Advances in basic emotion theory. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43(2), 133160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keltner, D., Tracy, J. L., Sauter, D., & Cowen, A. (2019). What basic emotion theory really says for the twenty-first century study of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43(2), 195201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keltner, D., Sauter, D., Tracy, J. L., Wetchler, E., & Cowen, A. S. (2022). How emotions, relationships, and culture constitute each other: Advances in social functionalist theory. Cognition and Emotion, 36(3), 388401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keltner, D., Brooks, J. A., & Cowen, A. (2023). Semantic space theory: Data-driven insights into basic emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 32(3), 242249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, T. D. (1978). A social interactional theory of emotions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kemper, T. D. (1981). Social constructionist and positivist approaches to the sociology of emotions. American Journal of Sociology, 87(2), 336362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, T. D. (1987). How many emotions are there? Wedding the social and autonomic components. American Journal of Sociology, 93(2), 263289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, T. D. (1990). Themes and variations in the sociology of emotions. In Kemper, T. D., (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotion (pp. 323). Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Kemper, T. D. (2006). Power and status and the power-status theory of emotions. In Stets & Turner, Handbook of the sociology of emotions, pp. 87113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, T. D., & Collins, R. (1990). Dimensions of microinteraction. American Journal of Sociology, 96(1), 3268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidron, Y., & Kuzar, R. (2002). My face is paling against my will: Emotion and control in English and Hebrew. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10(1–2), 129157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Ŭ., Yang, G., & Hwang, K. (2006). Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchner, A., Boiger, M., Uchida, Y., et al. (2018). Humiliated fury is not universal: The co-occurrence of anger and shame in the United States and Japan. Cognition and Emotion, 32(6), 13171328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleres, J. (2009). Preface: Notes on the sociology of emotions in Europe. In Hopkins, D., Kleres, J., Flam, H., and Kuzmics, H. (Eds.), Theorizing emotions: Sociological explorations and applications (pp. 727). New York: Campus Verlag.Google Scholar
Knatz, J. (2023). History of emotions and intellectual history. In Geroulanos, S., & Sapiro, G. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of the history and sociology of ideas. New York: Routledge, pp. 275291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollareth, D., Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Russell, J. A. (2018). Shame as a culture-specific emotion concept. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 18(3–4), 274292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2000a). The concept of anger: Universal or culture specific? Psychopathology, 33(4), 159170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kövecses, Z. (2000b). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture, and body in human feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2013). Multivariate pattern classification reveals autonomic and experiential representations of discrete emotions. Emotion, 13(4), 681690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2014). Advancing emotion theory with multivariate pattern classification. Emotion Review, 6(2), 160174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2015). Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 14371448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2016). Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), 444455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kret, M. E., Prochazkova, E., Sterck, E. H. M., & Clay, Z. (2020). Emotional expressions in human and non-human great apes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 115, 378395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labarre, W. (1947). The cultural basis of emotions and gestures. Journal of Personality, 16(1), 4968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2014). Mapping the brain’s metaphor circuitry: Metaphorical thought in everyday reason. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, article 958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Kövecses, Z. (1987). The cognitive model of anger inherent in American English. In Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 195221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(7), 926933.Google ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J. (2014). Emotion’s response patterns: The brain and the autonomic nervous system. Emotion Review, 6(2), 9399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange, J., & Zickfeld, J. H. (2021). Emotions as overlapping causal networks of emotion components: Implications and methodological approaches. Emotion Review, 13(2), 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange, J., Dalege, J., Borsboom, D., van Kleef, G. A., & Fischer, A. H. (2020). Toward an integrative psychometric model of emotions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 444468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukka, P., & Elfenbein, H. A. (2021). Cross-cultural emotion recognition and in-group advantages in vocal expression: A meta-analysis. Emotion Review, 13(1), 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, E. (1981). A poetics of power. The New Republic, 184(14), 3033.Google Scholar
Leavitt, J. (1996). Meaning and feeling in the anthropology of emotions. American Ethnologist, 23(3), 154539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: A view through fear. Progress in Brain Research, 195, 431442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, D. H., & Anderson, A. K. (2016). Form and function in facial expressive behavior. In Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, Handbook of emotions, pp. 495509.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 481504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2003). Autonomic specificity and emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 212224). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2011). Basic emotion questions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 379386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2014). The autonomic nervous system and emotion. Emotion Review, 6(2), 100112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27(4), 363384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. V. (1992). Emotion and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West Sumatra. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 972988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, R. I. (1973). Tahitians: Mind and experience in the society islands. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R. I. (1984). Emotion, knowing, and culture. In Shweder, & LeVine, Culture Theory, pp. 214237.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. M. G., Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2017). Evolutionary psychology: A how-to guide. American Psychologist, 72(4), 353373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M. D., & Liu, Z. (2011). Three time scales of neural self-organization underlying basic and nonbasic emotions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 416423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leys, R. (2017). The ascent of affect: Genealogy and critique. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leys, R. (2021). The trouble with affect. History of Psychology, 24(2), 127129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindholm, C. (2007). An anthropology of emotion. In Conerly, C., & Edgerton, R. B. (Eds.), A Companion to psychological anthropology: Modernity and psychocultural change (pp. 3047). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A. (2013). Emotions emerge from more basic psychological ingredients: A modern psychological constructionist model. Emotion Review, 5(4), 356368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Constructing emotion: The experience of fear as a conceptual act. Psychological Science, 19(9), 898903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). A functional architecture of the human brain: Emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(11), 533540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Russell, J. A. (2006). Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion, 6(1), 125138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behaviorial and Brain Science, 35, 121143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Siegel, E. H., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: Are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on lench, flores, and bench (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 255263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Jackson, J. C., Leshin, J., Satpute, A. B., & Gendron, M. (2022). The cultural evolution of emotion. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(11), 669681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, K. J. (2024). An overview of contemporary theories of emotions in sociology. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 287309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lo Bosco, M. C. (2021). Feelings in the field: The emotional labour of the ethnographer. Anthropology in Action, 28(2), 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loderer, K., Gentsch, K., Duffy, M. C., et al. (2020). Are concepts of achievement-related emotions universal across cultures? A semantic profiling approach. Cognition and Emotion, 34(7), 14801488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, W. J. (1909). English literature: Its history and its significance for the life of the English-speaking world. Boston: Ginn.Google Scholar
Longo, M. (2020). Emotions through literature: Fictional narratives, society, and the emotional self. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lonner, W. J. (1980). The search for psychological universals. In Triandis, H. C., & Lambert, W. W. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 143204). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. (1982). The domain of emotion words on Ifaluk. American Ethnologist, 9(1), 113128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, C. (1986). Emotion, thought, and estrangement: Emotion as a cultural category. Cultural Anthropology, 1(3), 287309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll & their challenge to western theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, C. (2017). What matters. Cultural Anthropology, 32(2), 181191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, C., & White, G. M. (1986). The anthropology of emotions. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15, 405436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, O. M. (1990). The social construction of emotion in India. In Lynch, O. M. (Ed.), Divine passions: The social construction of emotion in India (pp. 334). Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, M. L. (1995). Missing emotion: The limitations of cultural constructionism in the study of emotion. Cultural Anthropology, 10(2), 244263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, N. J. (1994). Symbolic interactionism as affect control. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Majeed, R. (2023). Does the problem of variability justify Barrett’s emotion revolution? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 14(4), 14211441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majid, A. (2012). Current emotion research in the language sciences. Emotion Review, 4(4), 432443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manokara, K., & Sauter, D. A. (forthcoming). Emotion universals: The foundation from which cultural variability of emotion emerges. In Leersnyder, J. De (Ed.), The socio-cultural shaping of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Manokara, K., Đurić, M., Fischer, A., & Sauter, D. (2021). Do people agree on how positive emotions are expressed? A survey of four emotions and five modalities across 11 cultures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 45(4), 601632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marvasti, A. B. (2019). Courage: It’s not all about overcoming fear. In Jacobsen, Emotions, everyday life and sociology, pp. 7187.Google Scholar
Mason, W. A., & Capitanio, J. P. (2012). Basic emotions: A reconstruction. Emotion Review, 4(3), 238244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities and differences in display rules. Motivation and Emotion, 14(3), 195214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (1992). American-Japanese cultural differences in the recognition of universal facial expressions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23(1), 7284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1989). American-Japanese cultural differences in intensity ratings of facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 13(2), 143157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (2019). Culture and emotion: Integrating biological universality with cultural specificity. In Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 361398). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2012). Culture and emotion: The integration of biological and cultural contributions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 91118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the agony of defeat: Spontaneous expression of medal winners of the 2004 Athens Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 568581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D., & Wilson, M. (2022). A half-century assessment of the study of culture and emotion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 53(7–8), 917934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsunaka, Y., Chen, Y., & Shinohara, K. (2023). Fluidity in Japanese emotion metaphors: A corpus study. The 16th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Düsseldorf, Germany. August 711, https://iclc16.github.io/abstracts/ICLC16_paper_185.pdf.Google Scholar
Matt, S. J., & Stearns, P. N. (Eds.). (2013). Doing emotions history. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthewson, L. (2003). Is the meta-language really natural? Theoretical Linguistics, 29(3), 263274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 209237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa: A psychological study of primitive youth for western civilisation. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1930). Growing up in New Guinea. New York: Blue Ribbon Books.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1935). Sex and temperament in three primitive societies. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Mendes, W. B. (2016). Emotion and the autonomic nervous system. In Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, Handbook of Emotions, pp. 166181.Google Scholar
Mendes, W. B., & Park, J. (2014). Neurobiological concomitants of motivational states. Advances in Motivational Science, 1, 233270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., et al. (2019). What are aesthetic emotions? Psychological Review, 26(2), 171195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural evolution: A review of theory, findings, and controversies. Evolutionary Biology, 43(4), 481497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B. (2001). Culture and emotion: Different approaches to the question. In Mayne, T. J., & Bonanno, G. A. (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 214250). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B. (2022). Between us: How cultures create emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A sociodynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 298302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 112(2), 179204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesquita, B., & Parkinson, B. (2024). Social constructionist theories of emotion. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 388407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B., Boiger, M., & De Leersnyder, J. (2016). The cultural construction of emotions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 3136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesquita, B., Boiger, M., & De Leersnyder, J. (2017). Doing emotions: The role of culture in everyday emotions. European Review of Social Psychology, 28(1), 95133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milton, K. (2005). Emotion (or life, the universe, everything). The Australian Journal of Anthropology, 16(2), 198211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montag, C., & Davis, K. L. (2020). Animal emotions: How they drive human behavior. Santa Barbara: Brainstorm Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montag, C., & Panksepp, J. (2016). Primal emotional-affective expressive foundations of human facial expression. Motivation and Emotion, 40(5), 760766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. C., Romney, A. K., Hsia, T.-L., & Rusch, C. D. (1999). The universality of the semantic structure of emotion terms: Methods for the study of inter- and intra-cultural variability. American Anthropologist, 101(3), 529546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moors, A. (2014). Flavors of appraisal theories of emotion. Emotion Review, 6(4), 303307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moors, A. (2022). Demystifying emotions: A typology of theories in psychology and philosophy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mousley, A. (2007). Re-humanising Shakespeare: Literary humanism, wisdom and modernity. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Mousley, A. (Ed.). (2011). Towards a new literary humanism. New York: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(3), 207233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, F. R. (1979). Emotions and the self: A theory of personhood and political order among Pintupi Aborigines. Ethos, 7(4), 343370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, N., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5(1), 815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1(3), 261289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesse, R. M., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009). Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. American Psychology, 64(2), 129139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A., & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 763784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nummenmaa, L., & Saarimäki, H. (2019). Emotions as discrete patterns of systemic activity. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nussbaum, M. (2001). Upheavals of thought: The intelligence of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. (1993). Social construction in emotions. In Lewis, M., & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 341352). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (2006). Simulation of substance and shadow: Inner emotions and outer behavior in Shakespeare’s psychology of character. College Literature, 33(1), 1533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. (2011). Such stuff as dreams: The psychology of fiction. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. (2012). The passionate muse: Exploring emotion in stories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(8), 618628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2011). Basic emotions in social relationships, reasoning, and psychological illnesses. Emotion Review, 3(4), 424433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogren, M., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2022). Emotion words link faces to emotional scenarios in early childhood. Emotion, 22(1), 167178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortony, A. (2022). Are all “basic emotions” emotions? A problem for the (basic) emotions construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(1), 4161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., & Fainsilber, L. (1989). The role of metaphors in descriptions of emotions. In Wilks, Y. (Ed.), Theoretical issues in natural language processing (pp. 78182). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97(3), 315331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özkarar-Gradwohl, F. G. (2019). Cross-cultural affective neuroscience. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, article 794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(1), 3080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 17911804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (2012). My reflections on commentaries and concluding perspectives. In Zachar, P., & Ellis, R. D. (Eds.), Categorical versus dimensional models of affect: A seminar on the theories of Panksepp and Russell (pp. 301320). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotion. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Watt, D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emotion Review, 3(4), 387396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paredes-Canilao, N., Babaran-Diaz, Ma. A., Florendo, Ma. N. B., & Salinas-Ramos, T. (2015). Indigenous psychologies and critical-emancipatory psychology. In Parker, I. (Ed.), Handbook of critical psychology (pp. 356365). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2012). Piecing together emotion: Sites and time-scales for social construction. Emotion Review, 4(3), 291298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Vick, S. J., & Bard, K. A. (2007). Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion, 7(1), 172181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parrott, W. G. (2010). Ur-emotions and your emotions: Reconceptualizing basic emotion. Emotion Review, 2(1), 1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parrott, W. G. (2012). Ur-emotions: The common feature of animal emotions and socially constructed emotions. Emotion Review, 4(3), 247248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2005). Emotions and multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (Ed.). (2006). Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expression, and representation. Buffalo: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 147164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perovíc, S., & Vuković-Stanatović, M. (2021). Universality and cultural variation in the conceptualisation of love via metaphors, metonymies and cultural scripts: The case of Montenegrin. Círculo de Lingüistica Aplicada a la Comunicación, 85, 4560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16(2), 331348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plamper, J. (2015). The history of emotions: An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (1962). The emotions: Facts, theories, and a new model. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (2001). The nature of emotions. American Scientist, 89(4), 344350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plutchik, R. (2003). Emotions and life: Perspectives from psychology, biology, and evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R., & Conte, H. R. (Eds.). (1997). Circumplex models of personality and emotions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ponsonnet, M. (2022). Emotional linguistic relativity and cross-cultural research. In Schiewer, Alatarriba, & Ng, Language and emotion, 2:10331061.Google Scholar
Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 715734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, M. J., & Dalgleish, T. (2015). Cognition and emotion: From order to disorder. 3rd ed. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Printz, J. J. (2004). Gut feelings: A perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pritzker, S. E., Fenigsen, J., & Wilce, J. M. (Eds.). (2020). The Routledge handbook of language and emotion. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Quinn, N., & Mathews, H. F. (2016). Emotional arousal in the making of cultural selves. Anthropological Theory, 16(4), 359389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratner, C. (1989). A social constructionist critique of the naturalistic theory of emotion. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 10(3), 211230.Google Scholar
Raz, G., Touroutoglou, A., Wilson-Mendenhall, C., et al. (2016). Functional connectivity dynamics during film viewing reveal common networks for different emotional experiences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(4), 709723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, W. M. (1997). Against constructionism: The historical ethnography of emotions. Current Anthropology, 38(3), 327351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, W. M. (2001). The navigation of feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, W. M. (2009). Historical research on the self and emotions. Emotion Review, 1(4), 302315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, W. M. (2020). The unavoidable intentionality of affect: The history of emotions and the neurosciences of the present day. Emotion Review, 12(3), 168178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riemer, N. (2006). Reductive paraphrase and meaning: A critique of Wierzbickian semantics. Linguistics and Philosophy, 29(3), 347379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2017). Evolution of the emotional brain. In Watanabe, S., Hofman, M. A., & Shimizu, T. (Eds.), Evolution of the brain, cognition, and emotion in vertebrates (pp. 251272). Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romania, V. (2022). Vilfredo Pareto. In Cerulo, C., & Scribano, A. (Eds.), The emotions in the classics of sociology: A study in social theory (pp. 96112). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Romney, A. K., Moore, C. C., & Rusch, C. D. (1997). Cultural universals: Measuring the semantic structure of emotion terms in English and Japanese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(10), 54895494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rorty, A. O. (1980). Introduction. In Rorty, A. O. (Ed.), Explaining emotions (pp. 19). Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rosaldo, M. Z. (1980). Knowledge and passion: Ilongot notions of self & social life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosaldo, M. Z. (1984). Toward an anthropology of self and feeling. In Shweder, & LeVine, Culture theory, pp. 137157.Google Scholar
Rosaldo, R. (1984). Grief and a headhunter’s rage: On the cultural force of emotions. In Bruner, E. M. (Ed.), Text, play, and story: The construction and reconstruction of self and society (pp. 178195). Washington, DC: American Ethnological Society.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 1136.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (2011). Emotional behaviors, emotivational goals, emotion strategies: Multiple levels of organization integrate variable and consistent responses. Emotion Review, 3(4), 434443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (2013). Appraisal in the emotion system: Coherence in strategies for coping. Emotion Review, 5(2), 141149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenwein, B. H. (2001). Writing without fear about early medieval emotions. Early Medieval Europe, 10(2), 229234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenwein, B. H. (2010). Problems and methods in the history of emotions. Passions in Context, 1, 130.Google Scholar
Rosenwein, B. H. (2016). Generations of feeling: A history of emotions, 600–1700. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenwein, B. H. (2021). Emotions: Some historical observations. History of Psychology, 24(2), 107111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenwein, B. H., & Cristiani, R. (2017). What is the history of emotion? Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Roughley, N. (Ed.). (2000). Being humans: Anthropological universality and particularity in transdisciplinary perspectives. Berlin: Walter se Gruter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, B. P. (2009). Changing brains: The emergence of the field of adult neurogenesis. BioSocieties, 4, 407424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1979). Affective space is bipolar. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(3), 345356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 11611178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 426450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 102141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A. (2009). Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 12591283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(3), 223237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1994). Fuzzy concepts in a fuzzy hierarchy: Varieties of anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 186205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (1997). What does a facial expression mean? In Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 130). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saarimäki, H., Gotsopoulos, A., Jääskeläinen, I. P., et al. (2016). Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 25632573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarimäki, H., Ejtehadian, L. F., Glerean, E., et al. (2018). Distributed affective space represents multiple emotion categories across the human brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(5), 471482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarimäki, H., Glerean, E., Smirnov, D., et al. (2022). Classification of emotion categories based on functional connectivity patterns of the human brain. NeuroImage, 247, article 118800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, D., & Maia, B. (2018). Language, emotion, and the emotions: A computational introduction. Language and Linguistics Compass, 12(6), article e12279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E. (1921). Language, an introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Satpute, A. B., & Lindquist, K. A. (2021). At the neural intersection between language and emotion. Affective Science, 2(2), 207220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, G., Thalmayer, A. G., & Bel-Bahar, T. S. (2014). Human attribute concepts: Relative ubiquity across twelve mutually isolated languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 199216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauter, D. A., & Russell, J. A. (2024). What do nonverbal expressions tell us about emotion? In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 543560.Google Scholar
Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. PNAS, 107(6), 24082412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2015). Emotional vocalizations are recognized across cultures regardless of the valence of distractors. Psychological Science, 26(3), 354356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarantino, A. (2009). Core affect and natural affective kinds. Philosophy of Science, 76(5), 940957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2012a). Discrete emotions: From folk psychology to causal mechanisms. In Zachar, P., & Ellis, R. D. (Eds.), Categorical versus dimensional models of affect: A seminar on the theories of Panksepp and Russell (pp. 135154). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2012b). How to define emotions scientifically. Emotion Review, 4(4), 358368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2012c). Functional specialization does not require a one-to-one mapping between brain regions and emotions. Behavioral and Brain Science, 35(3), 161162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2014). The motivational theory of emotions. In D’arms, J., & Jacobson, D. (Eds.), Moral psychology and human agency: Philosophical essays on the science of ethics (pp. 156185). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2015). Basic emotions, psychological construction, and the problem of variability. In Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The psychological construction of emotion (pp. 334376). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2016). The philosophy of emotion and its impact on affective science. In Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, Handbook of emotions, pp. 348.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2018). Are LeDoux’s survival circuits basic emotions under a different name? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 24, 7582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (Ed.). (2024). Emotion theory: The Routledge comprehensive guide. 2 vols. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., & de Sousa, R. (2018). Emotion. In E. Zalta (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., & Griffiths, P. (2011). Don’t give up on basic emotions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 444454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1984). Emotion as a multicomponent process: A model and some cross-cultural data. Review of Personality & Social Psychology, 5, 3763.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1994). Toward a concept of “modal emotions.” In Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 2531). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1997). The role of culture in emotion-antecedent appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 902922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92120). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2009a). Modal emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 257258). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2009b). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component process model. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 13071351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 719745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiewer, G. L., Altarriba, J., & Ng, B. C. (Eds.). (2022–2023). Language and emotion: An international handbook. 3 vols. Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Schnell, R. (2021). Histories of emotion: Modern-premodern. Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Scott, B. (2019). Affective disorders: Emotion in colonial and postcolonial literature. Liverpool: Liverpool University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedgwick, E. K., & Frank, A. (1995). Shame in the cybernetic fold: Reading Silvan Tomkins. Critical Inquiry, 21(2), 496522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger. PNAS, 106(35), 1507315078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semin, G. R. (2012). Balancing emotions between constraints and construction: Comment on Boiger and Mesquita. Emotion Review, 4(3), 230231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shakuf, V., Ben-David, B., Wegner, T. G. G., et al. (2022). Processing emotional prosody in a foreign language: The case of German and Hebrew. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Studies, 6(3), 251268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 10611086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaver, P. R., Wu, S., & Schwartz, J. C. (1992). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation: A prototype approach. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 175212.Google Scholar
Shiota, M. N. (2024). Basic and discrete emotion theories. In Scarantino, Emotion theory, pp. 310330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiota, M. N., Camras, L. A., & Adolphs, R. (2023). The future of affective science: Introduction to the special issue. Affective Science, 4(3), 429442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, B. (2000). Human diversity and human nature: The life and times of a false dichotomy. In Roughley, Being humans, pp. 81104.Google Scholar
Shott, S. (1979). Emotion and social life: A symbolic interactionist analysis. American Journal of Sociology, 84(6), 13171334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – what is it? In Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A., & Herdt, G. (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 144). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (2004). Deconstructing the emotions for the sake of comparative research. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 8197). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (2007). An anthropological perspective: The revival of cultural psychology – Some premonitions and reflections. In Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 821836). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., & LeVine, R. A. (Eds.). (1984). Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., et al. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 343393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, D. (2002). Culture and psychology: Perspectives of cross-cultural psychology. Psychology and Developing Societies, 14(1), 1125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2021). The sympathetic plot, its psychological origins, and implications for the evolution of fiction. Emotion Review, 13(3), 183198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smaldino, P. E., & Schank, J. C. (2012). Invariants of human emotion. Behavioral Brain Science, 35(3), 164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith-Lovin, L., & Winkielman, P. (2010). The social psychologies of emotion: A bridge that is not too far. Social Psychology Quarterly, 73(4), 327332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (1984). Getting angry: The Jamesian theory of emotion in anthropology. In Shweder, & LeVine, Culture theory, pp. 238256.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (1995). The cross-cultural comparison of emotion. In Marks, J., & Ames, R. T., (Eds.), Emotions in Asian thought (pp. 253294). Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (2002). Back to basics: On the very idea of “basic emotions.” Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 32(2), 115144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (Ed.). (2004). Thinking about feeling: Contemporary philosophers on emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, D., & Davies, J. (Eds.). (2010). Anthropological fieldwork: A relational process. Newcastle Upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Spiro, M. E. (1984). Some reflections on cultural determinism and relativism with special reference to emotion and reason. In Shweder, & LeVine, Culture theory, pp. 323346.Google Scholar
Stets, J. E. (2010). Future directions in the sociology of emotion. Emotion Review, 2(3), 265268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stets, J. E. (2012). Current emotion research in sociology: Advances in the discipline. Emotion Review, 4(3), 326334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stets, J. E., & Turner, J. H. (Eds.). (2006). The handbook of the sociology of emotions. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stets, J. E., & Turner, J. H. (2008). The sociology of emotion. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), The handbook of emotion, third edition (pp. 3246). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Stets, J. E., & Turner, J. H. (Eds.). (2014). Handbook of the sociology of emotions: Volume II. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stodulka, T. (2017). Towards an integrative anthropology of emotion: A case study from Yogyakarta. In Storch, A. (Ed.), Consensus and dissent: Negotiating emotion in the public space (pp. 934). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stodulka, T., Selim, N., & Mattes, D. (2018). Affective scholarship: Doing anthropology with epistemic affects. Ethos, 46(4), 519536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stodulka, T., Dinkelaker, S., & Thajib, F. (Eds.). (2019). Affective dimensions of fieldwork and ethnography. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundararajan, L. (2015). Understanding emotion in Chinese culture. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svašek, M. (2005). Introduction: Emotions in anthropology. In Milton, K., & Svašek, M. (Eds.), Mixed emotions: Anthropological studies of feeling (pp. 123). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tappolet, C. (2023). Philosophy of emotion: A contemporary introduction. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Taylor, H. (2020). Emotions, concepts and the indeterminacy of natural kinds. Synthese, 197(5), 20732093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tepora, T. (2020). What, if anything, can the history of emotions learn from the neurosciences? Cultural History, 9(1), 93105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thamm, R. (2004). Towards a universal power and status theory of emotion. Advances in Group Processes, 21, 189222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, P. A. (1985). Self-labeling processes in mental illness: The role of emotional deviance. American Journal of Sociology, 91(2), 221249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, P. A. (1989). The sociology of emotions. Annual Review of Sociology, 15, 317342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tissari, H. (2017). Current emotion research in English linguistics: Words for emotions in the history of English. Emotion Review, 9(1), 8694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomkins, S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol 1: The positive affects. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. (2020). Evolutionary psychology as the crystalizing core of a unified modern social science. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 14(4), 390403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990a). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11(4–5), 375424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990b). On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality, 58(1), 1767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), The handbook of emotion, third edition (pp. 114137). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2015). The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology: Foundations (pp. 387). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Touroutoglou, A., Lindquist, K. A., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). Intrinsic connectivity in the human brain does not reveal networks for “basic” emotions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(9), 12571265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J. L. (2014). An evolutionary approach to understanding distinct emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 308312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., Freire-Bebeau, L., & Przymus, D. E. (2002). Emotional expression and physiology in European Americans and Hmong Americans. Emotion, 2(4), 380397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, J. H. (1996). The evolution of emotions in humans: A Darwinian-Durkheimian analysis. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 26(1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H. (1999). Toward a general sociological theory of emotions. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 29(2), 133161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H. (2000). On the origins of human emotions: A sociological inquiry into the evolution of human affect. Stanford: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H. (2002). Face to face: Toward a sociological theory of interpersonal behavior. Stanford: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H. (2007). Human emotions: A sociological theory. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H. (2021). On human nature: The biology and sociology of what made us human. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Turner, J. H., & Stets, J. E. (2005). The sociology of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, V. (1967). The forest of symbols: Aspects of Ndembu ritual. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Vick, S. - Waller, J., Parr, B. M., Smith Pasqualini, L. A., M. C., & Bard, K. A. (2007). A cross-species comparison of facial morphology and movement in humans and chimpanzees using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 28642885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wager, T. D., Kang, J., Johnson, T. D., et al. (2015). A Bayesian model of category-specific emotional brain responses. PLos Computational Biology, 11(4), article e1004066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watzl, S. (2019). Culture or biology? If this sounds interesting, you might be confused. In Valsiner, J. (Ed.), Social philosophy of science for the social sciences (pp. 4571). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissbourd, E. (2023). Shakespeare from the bottom: Transnationalism, unfounded whiteness, and the First Folio. Shakespeare Quarterly, 74(3), 204216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherell, M. (2012). Affect and emotion: A new social science understanding. Los Angeles: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1986). Human emotions: Universal or cultural-specific? American Anthropologist, 88(3), 584594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Semantics, culture, and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1998). “Sadness” and “anger” in Russian: The non-universality of the so-called “basic human emotions.” In Athanasiadou, A., & Tabakowska, E. (Eds.), Speaking of emotions: Conceptualisation and expression (pp. 328). Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (2009). Language and metalanguage: Key issues in emotion research. Emotion Review, 1(1), 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (2014). Imprisoned in English: The hazards of English as a default language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wilce, J. M. (2009). Language and emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilce, J. M. (2014). Current emotion research in linguistic anthropology. Emotion Review, 6(1), 7785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkowski, B. M., Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., Carter, M. S., & Feltman, R. (2009). “Hot-headed” is more than an expression: The embodied representation of anger in terms of heat. Emotion, 9(4), 464477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science, 24(6), 947956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, B., & Matlock, T. (2017). Primary metaphors are both cultural and embodied. In Hampe, B. (Ed.), Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse (pp. 99115). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witkower, Z., Hill, A. K., Koster, J., & Tracy, J. L. (2021). Beyond face value: Evidence for the universality of bodily expressions of emotion. Affective Science, 2(3), 221229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkower, Z., Rule, N. O., & Tracy, J. L. (2023). Emotions do reliably co-occur with predicted facial signals. Comment on Durán and Fernández-Dols (2021). Emotion, 23(3), 903907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wnuk, E., & Ito, Y. (2021). The heart’s downward path to happiness: Cross-cultural diversity in spatial metaphors of affect. Cognitive Linguistics, 32(2), 195218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wojciehowski, H., & Gallese, V. (2011). How stories make us feel: Toward an embodied narratology. California Italian Studies, 2(1), https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3jg726c2.Google Scholar
Wojciehowski, H., & Gallese, V. (2018). Narrative and the biocultural turn. Costellazioni, 5, 921.Google Scholar
Wojciehowski, H., & Gallese, V. (2022). Embodiment: Embodied simulation and emotional engagement with fictional characters. In Hogan, Irish, & Hogan, Routledge companion, pp. 6173.Google Scholar
Wood, A., & Coan, J. A. (2023). Beyond nature versus nurture: The emergence of emotion. Affective Science, 4(3), 443452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ye, Z. (2001). An inquiry into “sadness” in Chinese. In Harkins, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds.), Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective (pp. 359404). Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeng, Y. (2022). Universality in Emily Brontë’s “remembrance.” Advances in Literary Study, 10(4), 313320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, F., Zhao, W., Qi, Z., et al. (2021). A distributed fMRI-based signature for the subjective experience of fear. Nature Communications, 12(1), article 6643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zunshine, L. (Ed.). (2015). The Oxford handbook of cognitive literary studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

The Universality of Emotion
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

The Universality of Emotion
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

The Universality of Emotion
Available formats
×