Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-n7lw4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-07T06:36:40.737Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Second Language Phonology

Phonetic Variation and Phonological Representations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 March 2025

Ellen Simon
Affiliation:
Ghent University

Summary

This Element deals with the interplay between phonology, phonetics and acquisition. It addresses the question of whether and how phonological representations are acquired in adult second language (L2) learners in the face of phonetic variation inherent in speech. Drawing from a large number of empirical studies on the acquisition of L2 speech sounds, the Element outlines how phonetic or phonological representations develop in L2 learners on the basis of input in immersion and instructed language learning contexts. Taking in insights from sociophonetics and clinical linguistics, the Element further discusses how accent variation impacts second language phonological acquisition and what clinical studies on individuals with atypical language development can tell us about the nature of phonological representations. Finally, new avenues in the field of L2 phonology are explored, especially with regard to methodological challenges and opportunities related to the use of spontaneous speech and remote data collection.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009420648
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 27 March 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

All speech samples were drawn from talk shows and interviews that are publicly available online:

Speech sample 1: excised from www.youtube.com/watch?v=QgjkjsqAzvo.

Speech samples 2 and 3: excised from www.youtube.com/watch?v=raE87U2TLJo.

Speech samples 4 and 5: see open access data repository: Verbeke, G. & Simon, E. (2023b). Replication data for listening to accents: Comprehensibility, accentedness and intelligibility of native and non-native English speech. https://doi.org/10.18710/8F0Q0L, DataverseNO, V1.

Speech sample 6: Phonologie du français contemporain, available at FLORAL-PFC – Base de données sur le français oral contemporain dans l’espace francophone (projet-pfc.net). For this speaker, see: Coquillon, A. (2010). Conversation à Marseille (Bouches-du-Rhône): Un cuisinier dans la marine. In Detey, S., Durand, J., Laks, B. C. & Lyche, eds., Les variétés du français parlé dans l’espace francophone: Ressources pour l’enseignement. Paris: Ophrys, pp. 117129.

Aksu, B. (2022). The influence of L1 Turkish regional dialects on L2 English speech production. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Lancaster University. https://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/id/eprint/182235/2/Amendment2022BaharPhD.pdf.Google Scholar
Alcorn, S., Meemann, K., Clopper, C. G. & Smiljanic, R. (2020). Acoustic cues and linguistic experience as factors in regional dialect classification. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147, 657670. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4989083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alves, U. K. & Alcantara de Albuquerque, J. I. (2022). Second Language Pronunciation: Different Approaches to Teaching and Training. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110736120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aoyama, K., Flege, J. E., Guion, S. G., Akahane-Yamada, R. & Yamada, T. (2004). Perceived phonetic dissimilarity and L2 speech learning: The case of Japanese /r/ and English /l/ and /r/. Journal of Phonetics, 32(2), 233250. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(03)00036-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, J. (2023). Phonological redeployment and the mapping problem: Cross-linguistic E-similarity is the beginning of the story, not the end. Second Language Research, 39(1), 287297. https://doi.org/10.1177/02676583211066413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babel, M. (2012). Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. Journal of Phonetics, 40(1), 177189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.09.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baese-Berk, M. M., Chandrasekaran, B. & Roark, C. L. (2022). The nature of non-native speech sound representations. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 152(5), 30253034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baese-Berk, M. M., Kapnoula, E. C. & Samuel, A. G. (2024). The relationship of speech perception and speech production: It’s complicated. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02561-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baese-Berk, M. M., Levi, S. V. & Van Engen, K. J. (2023). Intelligibility as a measure of speech perception: Current approaches, challenges and recommendations. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 153(1), 6876. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0016806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrientos, F. (2021). On segmental representations in second language phonology: A perceptual account. Second Language Research, 39(1), 259285. https://doi.org/10.1177/02676583211030637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, K. (2014). (r) we there yet? The change of rhoticity in New York City English. Language Variation and Change, 26(2), 141168. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394514000064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bent, T. & Baese-Berk, M. M. (2021). Perceptual learning of accented speech. In Pardo, J. S., Nygaard, L. C., Remez, R. E. & Pisoni, D. B., eds., The Handbook of Speech Perception, 2nd edition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, pp. 428464. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119184096.CH16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bent, T. & Bradlow, A. R. (2003). The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114(3), 16001610. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1603234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, C. (1995). A direct realist view of crosslanguage speech perception. In Strange, E., ed., Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Theoretical and Methodological Issues. Timonium, MD: New York Press, pp. 171204.Google Scholar
Best, C. & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In Munro, M. J. & Bohn, O.-S., eds., Second Language Speech Learning: The Role of Language Experience in Speech Perception and Production. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858886. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordag, D., Gor, K. & Opitz, A. (2022). Ontogenesis model of the L2 lexical representation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 185201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breitbarth, A., Ghyselen, A.-S., van Hout, R. & Wieling, M. (2023). Big data: New perspectives for research on language variation and change. Taal en Tongval, 75(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.5117/TET2023.1.001.BREI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broersma, M. (2005). Perception of familiar contrasts in unfamiliar positions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117(6), 38903901. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1906060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman., C. & Goldstein, I. (1986). Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 219252.Google Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica, 49(3–4), 155180. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruggeman, L. & Cutler, A. (2019). No L1 privilege in talker adaptation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(3), 681693. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catanzariti, B. & Currie, M. eds. (2022). How to use Mechanical Turk ethically [how-to guide]. Sage Research Methods: Doing Research Online. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781529608403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B. (2012). Rapid and multifaceted effects of second-language learning on first-language speech production. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 249268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B. (2013). A novelty effect in phonetic drift of the native language. Journal of Phonetics, 41(6), 520533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B. (2019). Phonetic drift. In Schmid, M. S. & Köpke, B., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 191203.Google Scholar
Chappell, W. & Kanwit, M. (2022). Do learners connect sociophonetic variation with regional and social characteristics? The case of L2 perception of Spanish aspiration. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44(1), 185209. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263121000115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), 975979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chládková, K., Boersma, P. & Escudero, P. (2022). Unattended distributional training can shift phoneme boundaries. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(5), 827840. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chládková, K. & Podlipský, V. J. (2011). Native dialect matters: Perceptual assimilation of Dutch vowels by Czech listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(4), 186192. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3629135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claessen, M., & Leitão, S. (2012). Phonological representations in children with SLI. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 28(2), 211223. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265659012436851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C. G. (2004). Linguistic experience and the perceptual classification of dialect variation. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Indiana.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. (2021). Perception of dialect variation. In Pardo, J. S., Nygaard, L. C., Remez, R. E. & Pisoni, D. B., eds., The Handbook of Speech Perception, 2nd edition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119184096.ch13.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. & Bradlow, A. R. (2009). Free classification of American English dialects by native and nonnative listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 37(4), 436451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2009.07.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C. G. & Dossey, E. (2020). Phonetic convergence to Southern American English: Acoustics and perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(1), 671683. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, A. C., Fougeron, C. & Huffman, M. K. (2017). Laboratory phonology. In The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory, 1st edition. Abingdon: Routledge, pp. 504529. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315675428-18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, A. C. & Renwick, M. (2019). Doing phonology in the age of big data. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3725897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coquillon, A. (2007). Le français parlé à Marseille: Exemple d’un locuteur PFC. Bulletin PFC (Phonologie du Français Contemporain), 7, 145156.Google Scholar
Coquillon, A. (2010). Conversation à Marseille (Bouches-du-Rhône): Un cuisinier dans la marine. In Detey, S., Durand, J., Laks, B. & Lyche, C., eds., Les variétés du français parlé dans l’espace francophone: Ressources pour l’enseignement. Paris: Ophrys, pp. 117129.Google Scholar
Cox, F. & Palethorpe, S. (2007). Australian English. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 37(3), 341350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A. & Otake, T. (2004). Pseudo-homophony in non-native listening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115, 2392. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4780547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., Drucker, S. J. & Scarborough, R. A. (2008). Talker adaptation in speech perception: Adjusting the signal or the representations? Cognition, 108, 710718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dailey, M. (2024). Late Bilingual Perception of Phonetic and Sociophonetic Cues. Doctoral dissertation. Ecole normale supérieure – Paris Sciences et Lettres.Google Scholar
Darcy., I. (2022). From fuzzy to fine-grained representations in the developing lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 206207. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Haes, H., Peperkamp, S., Crible, L. & Simon, E. (in prep.). Real-time reactions to sociophonetic features in French accents.Google Scholar
De Leeuw, E. & Chang, C. B. (2024). Phonetic and phonological L1 attrition and drift in bilingual speech. In Amengual, M., ed., The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Phonetics and Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 721745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delvaux, V. & Soquet, A. (2007). The influence of ambient speech on adult speech productions through unintentional imitation. Phonetica, 64(2–3), 145173. https://doi.org/10.1159/000107914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diehl, R. L. (1992). Articulatory Phonology, Special Topic Issue, 49(34).Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D. & Corbit, J. D. (1973). Selective adaptation of linguistic feature detectors. Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 99109. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90006-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenstein, M. (1982). A study of social variation in adult second language acquisition. Language Learning, 32, 367391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2022). Fuzzy representations. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(2), 210211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Diehl, R. L. & Buchwald, S. E. (1977). Perceptual switching in bilinguals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 62, 971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elvin, J., Escudero, P. & Vasiliev, P. (2014). Spanish is better than English for discriminating Portuguese vowels: Acoustic similarity versus vowel inventory size. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 103389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernestus, M. & Baayen, R. H. (2007). The comprehension of acoustically reduced morphologically complex words: The roles of deletion, duration, and frequency of occurrence. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. J., eds., Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Saarbrucken: Saarland University, pp. 773776.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. (2005). Linguistic perception and second language acquisition: Explaining the attainment of optimal phonological categorization. PhD thesis. LOT Dissertation Series 113. Utrecht University.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(4), 551585. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263104040021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P. & Hayes-Harb, R. (2022). The Ontogenesis Model may provide a useful guiding framework, but lacks explanatory power for the nature and development of L2 lexical representation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 212213. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Simon, E. & Mitterer, H. (2012). The perception of English front vowels by North Holland and Flemish listeners: Acoustic similarity predicts and explains cross-linguistic and L2 perception. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 280288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P. & Williams, D. (2012). Native dialect influences second-language vowel perception: Peruvian versus Iberian Spanish learners of Dutch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131, EL406–EL412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, B. G. & Iverson, P. (2007). Plasticity in vowel perception and production: A study of accent change in young adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(6), 38143826. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2722209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanelli, D. (2012). Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries. Scientometrics, 90(3), 891904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishero, S., Jongman, A. & Sereno, J. A. (2023). Perception and production of Mandarin-accented English: The effect of degree of accentedness on the Interlanguage Speech Intelligibility Benefit for Listeners (ISIB-L) and Talkers (ISIB-T). Journal of Phonetics, 99, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W., ed., Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 233277. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb01710.x.Google Scholar
Flege, J. (2021). New methods for second language (L2) speech research. In Wayland, R., ed., Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 119156. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108886901.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. & Bohn, O.-S. (2021). Second Language Speech learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress, ed. Wayland, R.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. & Eefting, W. (1987). Cross-language switching in stop consonant perception and production by Dutch speakers of English. Speech Communication, 6(3), 185202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J. & MacKay, I. (1995). Effects of second-language learning on the production of English consonants, Speech Communication, 16, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foulkes, P. & Docherty, G. (2006). The social life of phonetics and phonology. Journal of Phonetics, 34(4), 409438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2005.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foulkes, H. & Hay, J. B. (2015). The emergence of sociophonetic structure. In MacWhinney, B. & O’Grady, W., eds., The Handbook of Language Emergence. Wiley, pp. 292313. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118346136.ch13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, R. A. & McGory, J. T. (2007). Second language acquisition of a regional dialect of American English by native Japanese speakers. In Bohn, O.-S. & Munro, M. J., eds., Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 117134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, S. A. (2017). Exemplar theories in phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. and Bosch, A. R. K., eds., The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory. Abingdon: Routledge, pp. 553568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganong, W. F. (1980). Phonetic categorization in auditory word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6(1), 110125. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.6.1.110.Google ScholarPubMed
Gnevsheva, K. (2022). Studying sociophonetics in second languages. In Geeslin, K., ed., The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition and Sociolinguistics. Routledge, pp. 189199. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003017325-18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gnevsheva, K., Szakay, A. & Jansen, S. (2021). Phonetic convergence across dialect boundaries in first and second language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 89, 101110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2021.101110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldrick, M. & Cole, J. (2023). Advancement of phonetics in the 21st century: Exemplar models of speech production. Journal of Phonetics, 99, 101254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guediche, S., Blumstein, S. E., Fiez, J. A. & Holt, L. L. (2014). Speech perception under adverse conditions: Insights from behavioral, computational, and neuroscience research. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7(126), 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guion, S. G. (2003). The vowel systems of Quichua–Spanish bilinguals. Phonetica, 60(2), 98128. https://doi.org/10.1159/000071449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunter, K. M., Vaughn, C. R., Tyler, M. & Kendall, S. (2022). Contextualising /s/ retraction: Sibilant variation and change in Washington D.C. African American language. Language Variation and Change, 33(3), 331357. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095439452100020X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes-Harb, R., Smith, B. L., Bent, T. & Bradlow, A. R. (2008). The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit for native speakers of Mandarin: Production and perception of English word-final voicing contrasts. Journal of Phonetics, 36(4), 664679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, T. (1983). Sound change in perception and production. Language, 59, 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ježek, M. (2021). Sociophonology of Received Pronunciation: Native and Non-native Environments. Brno: Masaryk University Press. https://digilib.phil.muni.cz/sites/default/files/pdf/143839-monography.pdf. https://doi.org/10.5817/CZ.MUNI.M210-9833-2021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, F. & Kennison, S. (2022). The impact of L2 English learners’ belief about an interlocutor’s English proficiency on L2 phonetic accommodation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 51, 217234. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-021-09835-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E., van Heugten, M. & Buckler, H. (2022). Navigating accent variation: A developmental perspective. Annual Review of Linguistics, 8, 365387. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-032521-053717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, K. (1997). Speech perception without speaker normalization: An exemplar model. In Johnson, K. & Mullennix, J. W., eds., Talker Variability in Speech Processing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 145166.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. (2007). Decisions and mechanisms in exemplar-based phonology. In Solé, M. J., Beddor, P. & Ohala, M., eds., Experimental Approaches to Phonology: In Honor of John Ohala. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 2540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, K. (2020). The ΔF method of vocal tract length normalization for vowels. Laboratory Phonology, 11(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kachru, B. B. (1985). Standards, codification and sociolinguistic realism: The English language in the outer circle. In Quirk, R. & Widdowson, H. G., eds., English in the World: Teaching and Learning the Language and Literatures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1130.Google Scholar
Kartushina, N. & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2013). On the role of L1 speech production in L2 perception: Evidence from Spanish learners of French. Paper presented at Interspeech 2013, Lyon, France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kattner, F. & Ellermeier, W. (2020). Distraction at the cocktail party: Attenuation of the irrelevant speech effect after a training of auditory selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(1), 1020. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, T. & Fridland, V. (2021). Sociophonetics (Key Topics in Sociolinguistics). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316809709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, T., Pharao, N., Stuart-Smith, J. & Vaughn, C. (2023). Advancements of phonetics in the 21st century: Theoretical issues in sociophonetics. Journal of Phonetics, 98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, S. & Trofimovich, P. (2008). Intelligibility, comprehensibility, and accentedness of L2 speech: The role of listener experience and semantic context. Canadian Modern Language Review, 64(3), 459489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2021). Ecological validity and ‘ecological validity’. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 466471. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriengwatana, B., Terry, J., Chladkova, K. & Escudero, P. (2016). Speaker and accent variation are handled differently: Evidence in native and non-native listeners. PLoS ONE, 11(6), e0156870. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0156870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K. (1992). Infants’ perception and representation of speech: Development of a new theory. Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (ICSLP), 449456. https://doi.org/10.21437/ICSLP.1992-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwon, H., MacLeod, B. & Nielsen, K. (2024). Phonetic imitation: Representation, sound change, and other theoretical implications. Satellite workshop at the 19th Conference on Laboratory Phonology: LabPhon 19, Hanyang University, Seoul, 27–29 June. https://labphon.org/labphon19/phonetic-imitation.Google Scholar
Laase, M. (2015). Vowel production in children and adults with Williams syndrome. Unpublished honours thesis. Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Lavan, N. (2023). Person perception from voices: Impressions of physical, trait, and social characteristics emerge with 800 ms of exposure. Psychological Science, 34(7), 771783. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7rzem.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, D. & Baese-Berk, M. M. (2021). Non-native English listeners’ adaptation to native English speakers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America: Express Letters, 1(10), 105201. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0006558.Google ScholarPubMed
Lemhöfer, K. (2022). What is fuzziness, and how much does it explain? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 222223. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi, S. V. (2021). Perception of indexical properties of speech by children. In Pardo, J. S., Nygaard, L. C., Remez, R. E. & Pisoni, D. B., eds., The Handbook of Speech Perception, 2nd edition. Wiley, pp. 465483. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119184096.ch17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, M. Y. C., Braze, D., Kukona, A., Johns, C. L., Tabor, W., van Dyke, J. A., Mencl, W. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Pugh, K. R. & Magnuson, J. S. (2019). Individual differences in subphonemic sensitivity and phonological skills. Journal of Memory & Language, 107, 195215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.03.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74(6), 431461. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21(1), 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippi-Green, R. (1994). Accent, standard language ideology, and discriminatory pretext in the courts. Language in Society, 23(2), 163198. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404500017826.Google Scholar
Lippi-Green, R. (2011). English with an Accent: Language, Ideology and Discrimination in the United States, 2nd edition. London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203348802.Google Scholar
Long, G. B., Fox, R. A. & Jacewicz, E. (2016). Dyslexia limits the ability to categorize talker dialect. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 59, 900914. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, J. & Ota, M. (2023). The effect of learning context on Mandarin listeners’ perception of English vowels. Proceedings of the 20th International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences, 25722575.Google Scholar
MacLeod, B. & Di Lonardo Burr, S. M. (2022). Phonetic imitation of the acoustic realization of stress in Spanish: Production and perception. Journal of Phonetics, 92, 101139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maillart, C., Schelstraete, M.-A. & Hupet, M. (2004). Phonological representations of children with SLI: A study of French. Journal of Speech, Hearing and Language Research, 47, 187198. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/016).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattys, S., Davis, M. H., Bradlow, A. R. & Scott, S. K. (2012). Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(7–8), 953978. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.705006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F. & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101B111. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00157-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melguy, Y. V. & Johnson, K. (2021). General adaptation to accented English: Speech intelligibility unaffected by perceived source of non-native accent. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 149(4), 26022614. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0004240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melguy, Y. V. & Johnson, K. (2022). Perceptual adaptation to a novel accent: Phonetic category expansion or category shift? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 152, 2090. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0014602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennen, I., Reubold, U., Endes, K. & Mayr, R. (2022). Plasticity of native intonation in the L1 of English migrants to Austria. Languages, 7(3), 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitterer, H. & McQueen, J. (2009). Foreign subtitles help but native-language subtitles harm foreign speech perception. PloS ONE, 4. e7785. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitterer, H. & Müsseler, J. (2013). Regional accent variation in the shadowing task: Evidence for a loose perception–action coupling in speech. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(3), 557575. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0407-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitterer, H., Reinisch, E. & McQueen, J. M. (2018). Allophones, not phonemes in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 98, 7792. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.09.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, C. & Moore, E. F. (2018). Evaluating s(c)illy voices: The effects of salience, stereotypes, and co-present language variables on real-time reactions to regional speech. Language, 94(3), 629661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mora, J. C. (2007). Methodological issues in assessing L2 perceptual phonological competence. In Proceedings of the PTLC 2007 Phonetics Teaching and Learning Conference. London: Department of Phonetics and Linguistics, University College London, pp. 15.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology. The critical factors of age, motivation, and instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 81108. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263199001035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munro, M. J. (2008). Foreign accent and speech intelligibility. In Edwards, J. G. H. & Zampini, M. L. (eds.), Phonology and Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 193218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munro, M. J. & Derwing, T. M. (1995a). Foreign accent, comprehensibility, and intelligibility in the speech of second language learners. Language Learning, 45, 7397. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1995.tb00963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munro, M. J. & Derwing, T. M. (1995b). Processing time, accent, and comprehensibility in the perception of native and foreign-accented speech. Language and Speech, 38, 289306. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383099503800305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munro, M. J. & Derwing, T. M. (2020). Foreign accent, comprehensibility and intelligibility, redux. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 6(3), 283309. https://doi.org/10.1075/jslp.20038.mun.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagle, C. L. & Baese-Berk, M. M. (2022). Advancing the state of the art in L2 speech perception-production research: Revisiting theoretical assumptions and methodological practices. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44(2), 580605. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263121000371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W. & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 119. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J. (2022). How fuzzy are L2 phonological representations? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 230231. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, K. (2011). Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 132142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M. & Cutler, A. (2003). Perceptual learning in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 204238. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00006-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nycz, J. (2013). Changing words or changing rules? Second dialect acquisition and phonological representation. Journal of Phonetics, 52, 4962. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.12.014.Google Scholar
Nycz, J. (2015). Second dialect acquisition: A sociophonetic perspective. Language and Linguistic Compass, 9(11), 469482. https://doi.org/10.1080/23268263.2020.1723359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., Berkowitz, M. & Bachorowski, J. A. (2007). Listeners judge talker sex more efficiently from male than from female vowels. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(6), 930941. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, N. & Chen, L. (2005). Phonological/phonemic awareness and reading: A crosslinguistic perspective. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3, 145152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14769670500066271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pardo, J. S., Gibbons, R., Suppes, A. & Krauss, R. M. (2012). Phonetic convergence in college roommates. Journal of Phonetics, 40(1), 190197. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.WOCN.2011.10.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrachione, T. K., Del Tufo, S. N. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Human voice recognition depends on language ability. Science, 333, 595. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1207327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phonology du Français Contemporain, corpus of spoken French available at FLORAL-PFC – Base de données sur le français oral contemporain dans l’espace francophone (projet-pfc.net).Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. (2016). Phonological representation: Beyond abstract versus episodic. Annual Review of Linguistics, 2, 3352. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-125050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421435.Google ScholarPubMed
Rato, A. & Oliveira, D. (2022). Assessing the robustness of L2 perceptual training: A closer look at generalization and retention of learning. In Alves, U. & Albuquerque, J., eds., Second Language Pronunciation: Different Approaches to Teaching and Training. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 369396. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110736120-014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebernik, T., Jacobi, J., Jonkers, R., Noiray, A. & Wieling, M. (2021). A review of data collection practices using electromagnetic articulography. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology, 12(1.6), 142. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodd, J. M. (2024). Moving experimental psychology online: How to obtain high quality data when we can’t see our participants. Journal of Memory and Language, 134, 104472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K., Trofimovich, P. & Isaacs, T. (2017). Using listener judgements to investigate linguistic influences on L2 comprehensibility and accentedness: A validation and generalization study. Applied Linguistics, 38(4), 439462. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amv047.Google Scholar
Sancier, M. L. & Fowler, C. A. (1997). Gestural drift in a bilingual speaker of Brazilian Portuguese and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 421436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarampalis, A., Kalluri, S., Edwards, B. & Hafter, E. (2009). Objective measures of listening effort: Effects of background noise and noise reduction. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 52, 12301240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, R. W. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scobbie, J., Turk, A., Geng, C., King, S., Lickley, R. & Richmond, K. (2013). The Edinburgh Speech Production Facility DoubleTalk corpus. In Proceedings Interspeech 2013. www.isca-archive.org/interspeech_2013/scobbie13_interspeech.pdf.Google Scholar
Shea, C. E. (2021). Commentary: L2 phonology: Where theory, data, and methods meet. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.774721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, A. & Strange, W. (1982). The acquisition of /r/ and /l/ by Japanese learners of English: Evidence that speech production can precede speech perception. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 243261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E. (2010). Voicing in Contrast: Acquiring a Second Language Laryngeal System. Ginkgo: Academia Press. https://doi.org/10.26530/OAPEN_377758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E., Debaene, M. & Van Herreweghe, M. (2015). The effect of L1 regional variation on the perception and production of standard L1 and L2 vowels. Folia Linguistica, 49(2), 521553. https://doi.org/10.1515/flin-2015-0018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E., De Clercq, B. & Degrave, P. (in prep.). Listening to regional accents: Dutch and French listeners’ perception of phonetic vowel variation in Belgian Dutch.Google Scholar
Simon, E., De Clercq, B., Degrave, P. & Decourcelle, Q. (2022). On the robustness of high variability phonetic training effects: A study on the perception of non-native Dutch contrasts by French-speaking learners. In Alves, U. K. & Imaregna Alcantara de Albuquerque, J. (eds.), Second Language Pronunciation, Vol. 64. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 315344. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110736120-012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E., Lybaert, C. & Plevoets, K. (2022). Social attitudes, intelligibility and comprehensibility: The role of the listener in the perception of non-native speech. Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 19, 177221. https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i19.3763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E., Sjerps, M. & Fikkert, P. (2014). Phonological representations in children’s native and non-native lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 321. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solon, M. & Kanwit, M. (2022). New methods for tracking development of sociophonetic competence: Exploring a preference task for Spanish /d/ deletion. Applied Linguistics, 43(4), 805825. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amac009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solon, M., Linford, B. & Geeslin, K. L. (2018). Acquisition of sociophonetic variation: Intervocalic /d/ reduction in native and non-native Spanish. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics, 31, 309344. https://doi.org/10.1075/resla.16028.sol.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, C. (1997). The unknown Englishes? Testing German students’ ability to identify varieties of English. In Schneider, E. W., ed., Englishes around the World. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 93108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, N., Chandler, J. & Paolacci, G. (2017). Crowdsourcing samples in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(10), 736748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strycharczuk, P. & Scobbie, J. M. (2017). Fronting of Southern British English high-back vowels in articulation and acoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142 (1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4991010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swan, D. & Goswami, U. (1997). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66(1), 1841. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1997.2375. PMID: 9226932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, E. R. (2013). Sociophonetics. In Chambers, J. K. & Schilling-Estes, N., eds., The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 2nd edition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 108127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilsen, S. (2016). Selection and coordination: The articulatory basis for the emergence of phonological structure. Journal of Phonetics, 55, 5377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.11.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, B. V. & Mukai, Y. (2023). Spontaneous Speech. Cambridge Elements in Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108943024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. (2022). Analysing the relationship between L2 production and different stages of L2 processing: Eye-tracking and acoustic evidence for a novel contrast. Journal of Phonetics, 91, 101134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. (2024). The role of L2 input in developing a novel L2 contrast phonetically and phonologically: Production evidence from a residence abroad context. Second Language Research, 0(0). https://doi.org/10.1177/02676583231217166.Google Scholar
Tyler, M. (2019). PAM-L2 and phonological category acquisition in the foreign language classroom. In Nyvad, A. M., Hejná, M., Højen, A., Jespersen, A. B. & Sørensen, M. H. (eds.), A Sound Approach to Language Matters: In Honor of Ocke-Schwen Bohn. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press, pp. 607630.Google Scholar
van de Velde, H., Pinget, A., Voeten, C. & Demolin, D. (2022). Laboratory sociolinguistics. In Kristiansen, G., Franco, K., de Pascale, S., Rosseel, L. & Zhang, W., eds., Cognitive Sociolinguistics Revisited. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 557571. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110733945-045.Google Scholar
van de Velde, H. & van Hout, R. (2000). N-deletion in reading style. In H. de Hoop & T. van der Wouden, eds., Linguistics in the Netherlands 2000. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 209219.Google Scholar
van Heugten, M. & Johnson, E. K. (2014). Learning to contend with accents in infancy: Benefits of brief speaker exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 340350. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Heugten, M. & Johnson, E. K. (2016). Toddlers’ word recognition in an unfamiliar regional accent: The role of local sentence context and prior accent exposure. Language and Speech, 59(3), 353363. https://10.1177/0023830915600471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Heugten, M., Paquette-Smith, M., Krieger, D. R. & Johnson, E. K. (2018). Infants’ recognition of foreign-accented words: Flexible yet precise signal-to-word mapping strategies. Journal of Memory and Language, 100, 5160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.01.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leussen, J.-W. & Escudero, P. (2015). Learning to perceive and recognize a second language: The L2LP model revised. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 103694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbeke, G. (2024). On the role of ecological validity in language and speech research. In Buysschaert, J. & Lefèvre, A., eds., Taalkunde nu. Gent: Skribis. Series Studia Germanica Gandensia (Libri) & Spieghel Historiael, pp. 6995.Google Scholar
Verbeke, G., Mitterer, H. & Simon, E. (2025). Phonetic reduction in native and non-native English speech: Assessing the intelligibility for L2 listeners. Bilingualism, Language and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728925000021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbeke, G. & Simon, E. (2023a). Listening to accents: Comprehensibility, accentedness and intelligibility of native and non-native English speech. Lingua, 292, 103572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2023.103572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbeke, G. & Simon, E. (2023b). Replication data for Listening to Accents: Comprehensibility, accentedness and intelligibility of native and non-native English speech, DataverseNO, V1. https://doi.org/10.18710/8F0Q0L.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vriesendorp, H. (2021). How is the social meaning of linguistic variation stored in memory? Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Sheffield. https://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/29285.Google Scholar
Wagner, P., Trouvain, J. & Zimmerer, F. (2015). In defense of stylistic diversity in speech research. Journal of Phonetics, 48, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watt, D., Levon, E. & Ilbury, Ch. (2023). Accent bias. In Beal, J., Lukač, M. & Straaijer, R., eds., Routledge Handbook of Linguistic Prescriptivism. London: Routledge, pp. 3153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F. (2018). Perceptual beginnings to language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 703728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, K. S. & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Adaptation to novel accents by toddlers. Developmental Science, 14(2), 372384. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467!7687.2010.00986.X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, K. S., St. Pierre, T. & Johnson, E. K. (2024). In support of varying approaches to the study of variation. Language Learning and Development, 20(1), 6264. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2023.2239787.Google Scholar
Winke, P., Gass, S. & Myford, C. (2013). Raters’ L2 background as a potential source of bias in rating oral performance. Language Testing, 30(2), 231252. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532212456968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wojtkowiak, E. (2022). L2-induced phonetic drift in the speech of Polish learners of English: Phonological implications. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Adam Mickiewicz University.Google Scholar
Xie, X. & Fowler, C. A. (2013). Listening with a foreign-accent: The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit in Mandarin speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 41(5), 369378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, X., Liu, L. & Jaeger, T. F. (2021). Cross-talker generalization in the perception of nonnative speech: A large-scale replication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(11), e22. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y. (2010). In defense of lab speech. Journal of Phonetics, 38, 329336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.04.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X., Cheng, B., Qin, D. & Zhang, Y. (2021). Is talker variability a critical component of effective phonetic training for nonnative speech? Journal of Phonetics, 87, 101071, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2021.101071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y. & Wang, Y. (2007). Neural plasticity in speech acquisition and learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(2), 147160. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907002908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Second Language Phonology
  • Ellen Simon, Ghent University
  • Online ISBN: 9781009420648
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Second Language Phonology
  • Ellen Simon, Ghent University
  • Online ISBN: 9781009420648
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Second Language Phonology
  • Ellen Simon, Ghent University
  • Online ISBN: 9781009420648
Available formats
×