Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2005), “From Language-specific to General Principles: the Development of Chomskyan Thought,” University of Essex, MA dissertation.
Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2007), “Linguistics and the Natural Sciences,” University of Essex, Mres dissertation.
Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2008), “Minimalism and the Road towards the Natural Sciences,” in Jenset, G. B., Heggelund, O., and Cardona, M. D. (eds.), Linguistics in the Making: Selected Papers from the Second Scandinavian PhD Conference in Linguistics, 97–114.
Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2011), “The Strong Minimalist Thesis: its Nature and Plausibility,” University of Essex, PhD dissertation.
Armstrong, D. M. (1968), A Materialist Theory of Mind, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Atkinson, M. (2000), “Minimalist Visions,” Essex Research Reports in Linguistics 34: 1–55.
Atkinson, M. (2005a), Unpublished lecture notes, University of Essex, UK.
Atkinson, M. (2005b), “Minimalist Visions,” in Saleemi, A. P., Bohn, O.-S., and Gjedde, A. (eds.), Search of a Language for the Mind-Brain, Denmark: Aarhus University Press, 200–284.
Atkinson, M. (2009), “Recursive Musings,” Unpublished ms., University of Essex, UK.
Atkinson, M. and Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2012), “UG or not UG: Where is Recursion?”, Iberia 4(1): 35–60.
Ayala, F. J. (2007), Darwin’s Gift to Science and Religion, Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.
Ayoun, D. (2003), Parameter Setting in Language Acquisition, London: Continuum.
Batterman, R. W. (2000), “Multiple Realizability and Universality,” British Society for the Philosophy of Science 51: 115–45.
Beightler, C. S., Philips, D. T., and Wilde, D. J. (1979), Foundations of Optimization, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bell, A. E. (1947), Christian Huygens and the Development of Science in the Seventeenth Century A. E, London: Edward Arnold and Co.
Berdichevsky, V. L. (2009), Variational Principles of Continuum Mechanics, New York: Springer.
Berwick, R. C. (1998), “Language Evolution and the Minimalist Program: the Origins of Syntax,” in Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (eds.), Approaches to the Evolution of Language, Cambridge University Press, 320–40.
Berwick, R. C. and Chomsky, N. (2011), “The Biolinguistic Program: the Current State of its Development,” in Di Sciullo, A. M. and Boeckx, C. (eds.), The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, Oxford University Press, 19–41.
Bickerton, D. (2003), “Symbol and Structure: a Comprehensive Framework for Language Evolution,” in Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language Evolution, New York: Oxford University Press, 77–93.
Block, N. (2004[1980]), “What is Functionalism?”, in Heil, J. (ed.), Philosophy of Mind: a Guide and Anthology, Oxford University Press, 183–99.
Block, N. (1995), “The Mind as the Software of the Brain,” in Smith, E. E. and Osherson, D. N. (eds.), An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Thinking (2nd edn), Vol. III, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 377–425.
Block, N. and Fodor, J. (1972), “What Psychological States are Not,” Philosophical Review 81: 159–81.
Bloom, P. and Markson, L. (1998), “Capacities Underlying Word Learning,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 67–73.
Boden, M. A. (2006), Mind as Machine: A History of Cognitive Science, 2 vols., Oxford University Press.
Boden, M. A. (2008), “Odd Man Out: Reply to Reviewers,” Artificial Intelligence 172: 1944–64.
Boeckx, C. (2006), Linguistic Minimalism, Oxford University Press.
Boeckx, C. (2011), “Approaching Parameters from Below,” in Di Sciullo, A. M. and Boeckx, C. (eds.), The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty, Oxford University Press, 205–21.
Boeckx, C. and Grohmann, K. K. (2007), “The Biolinguistics Manifesto,” Biolinguistics 1: 1–8.
Boeckx, C. and Hornstein, N. (2010), “The Varying Aims of Linguistic Theory,” in Bricmont, J. and Franck, J. (eds.), Chomsky Notebook, New York: Columbia University Press, 115–41.
Boeckx, C., and Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2005), “Language as a Natural Object: Linguistics as a Natural Science,” The Linguistic Review 22: 447–66.
Bouchard, D. (2006), “Beyond Descriptivism: Exaptation and Linguistic Explanation,” in Nishida, C. and Montreuil, J. P. (eds.), New Perspectives on Romance Linguistics, John Benjamins B. V., Vol. I, 27–50.
Bricmont, J. and J. Franck (eds.) (2010), Chomsky Notebook, Columbia University Press: New York.
Brody, M. (2006), “‘Move’ in Syntax: Logically necessary or Undefinable in the Best Case?”, Lingua 116: 1553–60.
Burtt, E. A. (1924), The Metaphysical Foundations of Physical Science: A Historical and Critical Essay, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Cangelosi, A., Smith, A., and Smith, K. (eds.) (2006), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 6th International Conference (EVOLANG6), Singapore, World Scientific.
Carey, S. (1994), “Does Learning a Language Require the Child to Reconceptualize the World?”, Lingua 92: 143–67.
Carey, S. (2001), “Whorf versus Continuity Theories: Bringing Data to Bear on the Debate,” in Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. (eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, Cambridge University Press, 185–214.
Carnap, R. (1937), The Logical Syntax of Language, London: K. Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co.
Carnie, A. and Denton-Mendoza, N. (2003), “Functionalism is/n’t Formalism: an Interactive Review of Darnell et al. (1999),” Journal of Linguistics 39: 373–89.
Cela-Conde, C. J. and Marty, G. (1998), “Noam Chomsky’s Minimalist Program and the Philosophy of Mind: an Interview,” Syntax 1: 19–36.
Chabay, R. W. and Sherwood, B. A. (2002), Matter and Interactions, New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Chandra, P. (2007), “(DIS)AGREE: Movement and Agreement Reconsidered,” University of Maryland, PhD dissertation.
Cherniak, C. (1981), “Minimal Rationality,” Mind 90: 161–83.
Cherniak, C. (1990), “The Bounded Brain: Toward Quantitative Neuroanatomy,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2: 58–68.
Cherniak, C. (1992), “Local Optimization of Neuron Arbors,” Biological Cybernetics 66: 503–10.
Cherniak, C. (1994), “Philosophy and Computational Neuroanatomy,” Philosophical Studies 73: 89–107.
Cherniak, C. (1995), “Neural Component Placement,” Trends in Neuroscience 18: 522–7.
Cherniak, C. (2005), “Innateness and Brain-Wiring Optimization: Non-Genomic Nativism,” in Zilhao, A. (ed.), Cognition, Evolution, and Rationality, London: Routledge, 103–12.
Cherniak, C., Changizi, M., and Kang, D. W. (1999), “Large-scale Optimization of Neuron Arbors,” Physical Review E 59: 6001–9.
Cherniak, C., Mokhtarzada, Z., and Nodelman, U. (2002), “Optimal-wiring Models of Neuroanatomy,” in Ascoli, G. (ed.), Computational Neuroanatomy: Principles and Methods, Totowa, NJ: Humana.
Cherniak, C., Mokhtarzada, Z., and Rodriguez-Esteban, R. (2006), “Neural Wiring Optimization,” in Kaas, J. (ed.), Evolution of Nervous Systems, Vol. I, Academic Press, 269–72.
Cherniak, C., Mokhtarzada, Z., Rodriguez-Esteban, R., and Changizi, K. (2004), “Global Optimization of Cerebral Cortex Layout,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 101: 1081–6.
Chomsky, N. (1955), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, MIT Humanities Library, Microfilm.
Chomsky, N. (1956), “Three Models for the Description of Language,” in IRE Transactions on Information Theory, Vol. IT-2, no. 3, 113–24.
Chomsky, N. (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague: Mouton.
Chomsky, N. (1959), “Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behaviour,” Language 35: 26–58.
Chomsky, N. (1965), Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1966), Cartesian Linguistics, New York: Harper and Row.
Chomsky, N. (1968), Language and Mind, New York: Harcourt Brace and World, Inc.
Chomsky, N. (1971), “The Case Against B. F. Skinner,” The New York Review of Books, December 30.
Chomsky, N. (1972), Studies on Semantics in Generative Grammar, The Hague: Mouton.
Chomsky, N. (1973), “Conditions on Transformations,” in Kiparsky, P. and Peters, S. (eds.), Festschrift for Morris Halle, The Hague: Mouton.
Chomsky, N. (1975a), Reflections on Language, New York: Pantheon Books.
Chomsky, N. (1975b), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, New York: Plenum Press.
Chomsky, N. (1975c), “Language as a Key to Human Nature and Society,” in Otero, C. P. (ed.), Chomsky on Democracy and Education, RoutledgeFalmer, 58–64.
Chomsky, N. (1978), “On the Biological Basis of Language Capacities,” in Miller, G. A. and Lenneberg, E. (eds.), Psychology and Biology of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg, New York: Academic Press, XII, 199–220.
Chomsky, N. (1980a), Rules and Representations, New York: Columbia University Press and Oxford: Basil Blackwell Publisher.
Chomsky, N. (1980b), “On Cognitive Structures and their Development: a Reply to Piaget,” in Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.), Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky, New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 35–52.
Chomsky, N. (1981), Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures, Holland: Foris Publications.
Chomsky, N. (1986), Knowledge of Language: its Nature, Origin, and Use, New York: Praeger Publishers.
Chomsky, N. (1988), Language and the Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures, Cambridge, MA, and London: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1991), “Linguistics and Cognitive Science: Problems and Mysteries,” in Kasher, A. (ed.), The Chomskyan Turn, Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell, 26–53.
Chomsky, N. (1995a), The Minimalist Program, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1995b[1994]), “Bare Phrase Structure,” in Webelhuth, G. (ed.), Government and Binding and the Minimalist Program: Principles and Parameters in Syntactic Theory, Oxford: Blackwell, 383–439 (originally distributed as MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics 5, 1994).
Chomsky, N. (1996), Powers and Prospects: Reflections on Human Nature and the Social Order, London: Pluto Press.
Chomsky, N. (2000a), New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind, Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. (2000b), “Minimalist Inquiries: the Framework,” in Martin, R., Michaels, D., and Uriagereka, J. (eds.), Step by Step: Essays in Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 89–155.
Chomsky, N. (2000c), “Linguistic and Brain Science,” in Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., and O´Neil, W. (eds.), Image, Language, Brain, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 13–28.
Chomsky, N. (2001), “Derivation by Phase,” in Kenstowicz, M. (ed.), Ken Hale: a Life in Language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–52.
Chomsky, N. (2002), On Language and Nature, Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. (2003a), “Reply to Lycan,” in Antony, L. M. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Chomsky and his Critics, Blackwell Publishing, 255–63.
Chomsky, N. (2003b), “Reply to Egan,” in Antony, L. M. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Chomsky and his Critics, Blackwell Publishing, 268–74.
Chomsky, N. (2004a), “Beyond Explanatory Adequacy,” in Belletti, A. (ed.), Structures and Beyond, Oxford University Press, 104–31.
Chomsky, N. (2004b), The Generative Enterprise Revisited: Discussions with Riny Huybregts, Henk van Riemsdijk, Naoki Fukui and Mihoko Zushi, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Chomsky, N. (2005), “Three Factors in Language Design,” ms., Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2007a), Review of Boden (2006), Artificial Intelligence 171: 1094–103.
Chomsky, N. (2007b), “Approaching UG from Below,” ms., Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2008a), “On Phases,” in Freidin, R., Otero, C. P., and Zubizarreta, M.-L. (eds.), Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 133–66.
Chomsky, N. (2010), “Some Simple Evo Devo Theses: How True Might they be for Language?”, in Larson, R., Deprez, V., and Yamakido, H. (eds.), The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives, Cambridge University Press, 45–62.
Chomsky, N. (2013), “Problems of projection,” Lingua: 130: 33–49.
Chomsky, N. and Lasnik, H. (1993), “The Theory of Principles and Parameters,” in Jacobs, J., Stechow, A., Sternefeld, W., and Vennemann, T. (eds.), Syntax: an International Handbook of Contemporary Research, Vol. I, Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 506–69 (reprinted in Chomsky 1995a).
Chomsky, N. and Miller, G. A. (1963), “Introduction to the Formal Analysis of Natural Languages,” in Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (eds.), Handbook of Mathematical Psychology, Vol. II, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 269–321.
Chomsky, N., Hauser, M. D., and Fitch, W. T. (2004), “Appendix: the Minimalist Program,” unpublished ms., Harvard University.
Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S. (eds.) (2003), Language Evolution, New York: Oxford University Press.
Clark, R. and Roberts, I. (1993), “A Computational Model of Language Learnability and Language Change,” Linguistic Inquiry 24: 299–345.
Cowper, E. A. (1992), A Concise Introduction to Syntactic Theory: the Government-binding Approach, University of Chicago Press.
Darwin, C. (2003[1859]), On the Origin of Species: a Facsimile of the First Edition, Cambridge, MA, and London: Harvard University Press.
Dresher, B. E. and Kaye, J. D. (1990), “A Computational Learning Model for Metrical Phonology,” Cognition 34: 137–95.
Dugas, R. (1988), A History of Mechanics, Dover Science Books.
Duncan, A. M. (trans.) (1981), Kepler: Mysterium Cosmographicum: Secret of the Universe, New York: Abaris.
Eldredge, N. and Gould, S. J. (1972), “Punctuated Equilibria: an Alternative to Phyletic Gradualism,” in Schopf, T. J. M. (ed.), Models in Paleobiology, San Francisco: Freeman, Cooper and Company, 82–115.
Epstein, S. D. and Hornstein, N. (1999), Working Minimalism, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Epstein, S. D. and Seely, T. D. (2002), “Introduction: on the Quest for Explanation,” in Epstein, S. D. and Seely, T. D. (eds.), Derivation and Explanation in the Minimalist Program, Blackwell, 1–18.
Evans, N. and Levinson, S. C. (2009), “The Myth of Language Universals: Language Diversity and its Importance for Cognitive Science,” Behavioural and Brain Sciences 32: 429–92.
Everett, D. (2005), “Biology and Language: a Consideration of Alternatives,” Journal of Linguistics 41: 157–75.
Everett, D. (2009), “Pirahã Culture and Grammar: a Response to Some Criticisms,” Language 85(2): 405–42.
Feynman, R. P. (1985), QED: the Strange Theory of Light and Matter, Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.
Feynman, R. P., Leighton, R. B., and Sands, M. (1964), The Feynman Lectures on Physics: Mainly Electromagnetism and Matter, Vol. II, Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fitch, W. T. (2010), The Evolution of Language, Cambridge University Press.
Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (2004), “Computational Constraints on Syntactic Processing in a Nonhuman Primate,” Science 303: 377–80.
Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., and Chomsky, N. (2005), “The Evolution of the Language Faculty: Clarification and Implications,” Cognition 97: 179–210.
Fodor, J. (1974), “Special Sciences: or the Disunity of Science as a Working Hypothesis,” Synthese 28: 97–115.
Fodor, J. (1975), The Language of Thought, New York: Thomas Y. Crowell.
Fodor, J. (1983), Modularity of Mind: an Essay on Faculty Psychology, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1987), Psychosemantics: the Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1991), “You can Fool Some of the People All of the Time, Everything Else Being Equal; Hedged Laws and Psychological Explanations,” Mind 100: 19–34.
Fodor, J. (1997), “Special Sciences: Still Autonomous after All These Years,” Philosophical Perspectives 11: 149–63.
Fodor, J. (1999), “Let Your Brain Alone,” London Review of Books, Vol. XXI, no. 19, September.
Fodor, J. (2004[1981]), “The Mind-Body Problem,” in Heil, J. (ed.), Philosophy of Mind: a Guide and Anthology, Oxford University Press, 168–82.
Fodor, J. (2007), “Why Pigs Don’t Have Wings,” London Review of Books, Vol. XXIX, no. 20, October.
Frank, P. (1961), Modern Science and its Philosophy, Collier Books: New York.
Freidin, R. (2007), Generative Grammar: Theory and its History, Routledge: New York.
Freidin, R. and Vergnaud, J. R. (2001), “Exquisite Connections: Some Remarks on the Evolution of Linguistic Theory,” Lingua 111: 639–66.
Fukui, N. (1996), “On the Nature of Economy in Language,” Ninti Kagaku [Cognitive Studies] 3: 51–71.
Galileo, G. (1967), Dialogue Concerning the Two Chief World Systems (2nd edn), Drake, S. (ed. and trans.), Berkeley: University of California Press.
Gallistel, C. R., Gelman, R., and Cordes, S. (2006), “The Cultural and Evolutionary History of the Real Numbers,” in Levinson, S. C. and Jaisson, P. (eds.), Evolution and Culture, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 247–74.
Gambell, T. and Yang, C. (2003), “Scope and Limits of Statistical Learning in Word Segmentation,” ms., New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Giblin, I. (2008), “Music and the Generative Enterprise,” doctoral dissertation, University of New South Wales.
Gibson, E. and Wexler, K. (1994), “Triggers,” Linguistic Inquiry 25: 407–54.
Golumbia, D. (2010), “Minimalism is Functionalism,” Language Sciences 32: 28–42.
Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C. (1979), “The Spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: a Critique of the Adaptationist Program,” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 205: 581–98.
Haegeman, L. (1994), Introduction to Government and Binding Theory, Oxford: Blackwell.
Hamilton, W. R. (1967[1833]), “On a General Method of Expressing the Paths of Light, and the Planets, by the Coefficients of a Characteristic Function,” in Halberstam, H. and Ingram, R. E. (eds.), The Mathematical Papers of Sir William Rowan Hamilton, Vol. III, New York: Cambridge University Press, 311–32.
Hankins, T. L. (1985), Science and the Enlightenment, Cambridge History of Science Series, University of Cambridge: The Press Syndicate.
Hauser, M. D. (2009), “The Mind,” Scientific American, September, 44–51.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002), “The Faculty of Language: What is it, Who has it, and How did it Evolve?”, Science 298: 1569–79.
Hesse, M. (1961), Forces and Fields: the Concept of Action at a Distance in the History of Physics, London: Nelson.
Hinzen, W. (2006a), Mind Design and Minimal Syntax, Oxford University Press.
Hinzen, W. (2006b), “Minimalist Foundations of Language: on the Question of Why Language is the Way it is,” in Cangelosi, A., Smith, A., and Smith, K. (eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 6th International Conference (EVOLANG6), Singapore, World Scientific, 115–22.
Hinzen, W. (2009), “Hierarchy, Merge, and Truth,” in Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J., and Salaburu, P. (eds.), On Minds and Language, Oxford University Press, 123–40.
Hinzen, W. and Uriagereka, J. (2006), “On the Metaphysics of Linguistics,” Erkenntnis 65: 71–96.
Hiraiwa, K. (2005), “Dimensions of Symmetry in Syntax: Agreement and Clausal Architecture,” doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hofstadter, D. R. (1999[1979]), Gödel, Escher, Bach: an Eternal Golden Braid, Basic Books.
Hornstein, N. (1995), Logical Form: from GB to Minimalism, Oxford: Blackwell.
Hornstein, N. (2009), A Theory of Syntax: Minimal Operations and Universal Grammar, Cambridge University Press.
Hornstein, N., Nunes, J., and Grohmann, K. K. (2005), Understanding Minimalism: an Introduction to Minimalist Syntax, Cambridge University Press.
Hull, D. (1972), “Reduction in Genetics – Biology or Philosophy?”, Philosophy of Science 39: 491–9.
Huntley-Fenner, G., Carey, S., and Solimando, A., (2002), “Objects are Individuals but Stuff doesn’t Count: Perceived Rigidity and Cohesiveness Influence Infants’ Representations of Small Groups of Discrete Entities,” Cognition 85: 203–21.
Imai, M. and Gentner, D. (1997), “A Cross-linguistic Study of Early Word Meaning: Universal Ontology and Linguistic Influence,” Cognition 62: 169–200.
Ingram, J. (2007), Neurolinguistics: an Introduction to Spoken Language Processing and its Disorders, Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics, Cambridge University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1983), Semantics and Cognition, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. and Pinker, S. (2005), “The Nature of the Language Faculty and its Implications for Evolution of Language,” reply to Fitch et al. 2005, Cognition 97: 211–25.
Jenkins, L. (2000), Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language, Cambridge University Press.
Johansson, S. (2006), “Constraining the Time When Language Evolved,” in Cangelosi, A., Smith, A., and Smith, K. (eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 6th International Conference (EVOLANG6), Singapore, World Scientific, 152–9.
Johnson, D. and Lappin, S. (1997), “A Critique of the Minimalist Program,” Linguistics and Philosophy 20: 273–333.
Johnson, D. and Lappin, S. (1999), Logical Constraints vs. Economy, Stanford, CA: Center for Study of Language and Information.
Jowett, B. (trans.) (1961), “Plato: Timaeus,” in Hamilton, E. and Cairns, H. (eds.), The Collected Dialogues of Plato, New York: Bollingen.
Kant, I. (1997), Lectures on Metaphysics, Ameriks, K. and Naragon, S. (trans. and ed.), Cambridge University Press.
Katz, J. J. and Postal, P. M. (1964), An Integrated Theory of Linguistic Descriptions, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kim, J. (1992), “Multiple Realization and the Metaphysics of Reduction,” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 52: 1–26.
Kim, J. (1998), Mind in a Physical World: an Essay on the Mind-Body Problem and Mental Causation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kinsella, A. R. (2009), Language Evolution and Syntactic Theory, Cambridge University Press.
Kitcher, P. (1984), “1953 and All That: a Tale of Two Sciences,” Philosophical Review 93: 335–73.
Koster, J. (2007), “Structure Preservingness, Internal Merge, and the Strict Locality of Triads”, in Karimi, S., Samiian, V., and Wilkins, W. K. (eds.), Phrasal and Clausal Architecture: Syntactic derivation and interpretation [Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 101], Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 188–205.
Koyré, A. (1957), From the Closed World to the Infinite Universe, Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins Press.
Lanczos, C. (1970[1949]), The Variational Principles of Mechanics, University of Toronto Press.
Langendoen, D. T. (2003), “Merge,” in Carnie, A., Harley, H., and Willie, M. (eds.), Formal Approaches to Function in Grammar, Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Co., 307–18.
Lappin, S., Levine, R., and Johnson, D. (2000), “The Structure of Unscientific Revolutions,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 18: 665–71.
Lappin, S., Levine, R., and Johnson, D. (2001), “The Revolution Maximally Confused,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 19: 901–19.
Larson, R., Deprez, V., and Yamakido, H. (eds.) (2010), The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives, Cambridge University Press.
Lees, R. B. (1957), “Review of Chomsky (1957),” Language 33: 375–408.
Legate, J. and Yang, C. (2002), “Empirical Re-assessment of Stimulus Poverty Arguments,” The Linguistic Review 19: 151–62.
Lenneberg, E. (1967), Biological Foundations of Language, New York: Wiley.
LePore, E. and Loewer, B. (1989), “More on Making Mind Matter,” Philosophical Topics 17: 175–91.
Levine, R. (2002), “Review of Uriagereka (1998),” Language 78: 325–30.
Lewis, D. (1972), “Psychophysical and Theoretical Identification,” Australasian Journal of Philosophy 50: 249–67.
Mahajan, N., Barnes, J., Blanco, M., and Santos, L. (2009), “Enumeration of Objects and Substances in Non-human Primates: Experiments with Brown Lemurs (Eulemur Fulvus),” Developmental Science 12: 920–8.
Mandelbrot, B. (1982), The Fractal Geometry of Nature, New York: W. H. Freeman.
Marantz, A. (1995), “The Minimalist Program,” in Webelhuth, G. (ed.), Government and Binding Theory and the Minimalist Program, Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 349–82.
Markson, L. and Bloom, P. (1997), “Evidence Against a Dedicated System for Word Learning in Children,” Nature 385: 813–15.
Marr, D. (1982), Vision: a Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information, New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Martin, R. and Uriagereka, J. (2000), “Some Possible Foundations of the Minimalist Program,” in Martin, R., Michaels, D., and Uriagereka, J. (eds.), Step by Step: Essays in Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–30.
Maxwell, J. C. (2003[1876]), “On Action at a Distance,” in Niven, W. D. (ed.), The Scientific Papers of James Clerk Maxwell, Vol. II, Dover Phoenix Editions, 311–23.
Maynard Smith, J., Burian, R., Kauffman, S., Alberch, R., Campbell, J., et al. (1985), “Developmental Constraints and Evolution,” The Quarterly Review of Biology 60: 265–87.
Mayr, E. (1982), The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance, Cambridge, MA, and London: Harvard University Press.
McKeon, R. (ed.) (1941), The Basic Works of Aristotle, New York: Random House, Inc.
Mirowski, P. (1989), More Heat than Light: Economics as Social Physics, Physics as Nature’s Economics, Cambridge University Press.
Nahin, P. J. (2004), When Least is Best, Princeton University Press.
Narita, H. (2009), “How Syntax Naturalizes Semantics: Review of Uriagereka (2008),” Lingua 119(11): 1767–75.
Neuenschwander, D. (2010), Emmy Noether’s Wonderful Theorem, Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D., and Rodrigues, C. (2009), “Pirahã Exceptionality: a Reassessment,” Language 85(2): 355–404.
Newmeyer, F. J. (2006), “A Rejoinder to ‘On the Role of Parameters in Universal Grammar: a Reply to Newmeyer’ by Ian Roberts and Anders Holmberg,” ms., University of Washington.
Newton, I. (1999), The Principia: Mathematical Principles of Natural Philosophy, Cohen, I. and Whitman, A. (trans.), Berkeley: University of California Press.
Newton, I. (2004), Philosophical Writings, Janiak, A. (ed.), Cambridge University Press.
O’Keefe, T. (2005), Epicurus on Freedom, Cambridge University Press.
Ott, D. (2007), “Reverse-engineering the Language Faculty: Origins and Implications of the Minimalist Program,” Harvard Working Papers in Linguistics 12: 77–90.
Penrose, R. (1989), The Emperor’s New Mind, Oxford University Press.
Perruchet, P. and Rey, A. (2005), “Does the Mastery of Center-embedded Linguistic Structures Distinguish Humans from Nonhuman Primates?”, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12: 307–13.
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.) (1980), Language and Learning: the Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky, New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (1998), “Foreword,” in Uriagereka, J. (ed.), Rhyme and Reason: an Introduction to Minimalist Syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J., and Salaburu, P. (eds.) (2009), Of Minds and Language: a Dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country, Oxford University Press.
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990), “Natural Language and Natural Selection,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–84.
Pinker, S. and Jackendoff, R. (2005), “The Faculty of Language: What’s Special about it?”, ms., Harvard University and Brandeis University. Modified version published in: Cognition 95: 201–36.
Popper, K. (1945), The Open Society and Its Enemies, 2 vols., London: Routledge.
Popper. K (1959[1935]), The Logic of Scientific Discovery, London: Hutchinson.
Popper. K (1961), The Poverty of Historicism (2nd edn), London: Routledge.
Popper. K (1963), Conjectures and Refutations, New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Popper. K (1976), Unended Quest: an Intellectual Autobiography, London: Fontana.
Popper, K. and Eccles, J. (1977), The Self and its Brain: an Argument for Interactionism, New York: Springer.
Post, E. (1943), “Formal Reductions of the General Combinatory Decision Problem,” American Journal of Mathematics 65: 197–215.
Postal, P. (2003), “(Virtually) Conceptually Necessary,” Journal of Linguistics 39: 599–620.
Progovac, L. (2010), “When Clauses Refuse to be Recursive: an Evolutionary Perspective,” in van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and Human Language, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 193–211.
Pullum, G. and Scholz, B. (2002), “Empirical Assessment of Stimulus Poverty Arguments,” The Linguistic Review 19: 9–50.
Pullum, G. and Scholz, B. (2010), “Recursion and Infinitude Claim,” in van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and Human Language, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 113–38.
Putnam, H. (1967), “The Nature of Mental States,” in Capitan, W. and Merrill, D. (eds.), Arts, Mind, and Religion, Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Putnam, H. (1988), Representation and Reality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Quine, W. V. O. (1950), Methods of Logic, New York: Holt.
Raj, M. (1996), Fundamentals of Optics, New Delhi: Anmol.
Ramón y Cajal, S. (1913–14), Estudios Sobre la Degeneración y Regeneración del Sistema Nervioso, Madrid: Moya.
Ringo, J. (1991), “Neuronal Interconnection as a Function of Brain Size,” Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 38: 1–6.
Rizzi, L. (1990), Relativized Minimality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rizzi, L. (2004), “On the Study of the Language Faculty: Results, Developments, and Perspectives,” The Linguistic Review 21: 323–44.
Sagan, H. (1969), Introduction to the Calculus of Variations, New York: McGraw-Hill.
Sakel, J. and Stapert, E. (2010), “Pirahã – in need of recursive syntax?”, in van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and Human Language, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 3–16.
Samuels, B. D. (2009), “The Structure of Phonological Theory,” Harvard University, PhD dissertation.
Samuels, B. D. (2011), Phonological Architecture: a Biolinguistic Perspective, Oxford University Press.
Samuels, B. D. and Boeckx, C. (2009), “What Emerges from Merge in Phonology?”, paper presented at OCP 6, University of Edinburgh, 22–4 January.
Scheer, T. (2004), A Lateral Theory of Phonology: What is CVCV, and Why Should it Be?, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Schreuder, M. and Gilbers, D. (2004), “Recursive Patterns in Phonological Phrases,” in Bel, B. and Marlien, I. (eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2004, Nara, Japan: ISCA (SproSig), 341–4.
Schwartz, E. L. (ed.) (1990), Computational Neuroscience, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Tamariz, M., Cartmill, E. A., and Hurford, J. R. (eds.) (2012), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 9th International Conference (EVOLANG 2012), New Jersey, World Scientific.
Semenza, C., Delazer, M., Bertella, L., Granà, A., Mori, I., et al. (2006), “Is Math Lateralised on the Same Side as Language? Right Hemisphere Aphasia and Mathematical Abilities,” Neuroscience Letters 406: 285–8.
Shapiro, L. A. (2004), The Mind Incarnate, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Siegal, M. L. and Bergman, A. (2006), “Canalization,” in Fox, C. W. and Wolf, J. B. (eds.), Evolutionary Genetics: Concepts and Case Studies, New York: Oxford University Press, 235–51.
Smith, N. (2000), “Foreword,” in Chomsky, N., New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind, Cambridge University Press.
Smith, N. (2004), Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals (2nd edn), Cambridge University Press.
Smith, N. and Law, A. (2007), “Twangling Instruments: Is Parametric Variation Definitional of Human Language?”, UCL Working Papers in Linguistics 19: 1–28.
Smith, N and Law, A. (2009), “On Parametric (and Non-Parametric) Variation,” Biolinguistics 3/4: 332–43.
Sober, E. (1999), “The Multiple Realizability Argument Against Reductionism,” Philosophy of Science 66: 542–64.
Stahnisch, F. W. and Nitsch, R. (2002), “Santiago Ramón y Cajal’s Concept of Neuronal Plasticity: the Ambiguity Lives On,” Trends in Neuroscience 25: 589–91.
Tomalin, M. (2007), “Reconsidering Recursion in Syntactic Theory,” Lingua 117: 1784–1800.
Uriagereka, J. (1998), Rhyme and Reason: an Introduction to Minimalist Syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Uriagereka, J. (2000), “On the Emptiness of ‘Design’ Polemics,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 18: 863–71.
Uriagereka, J. (2001), “Cutting Derivational Options,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 19: 891–900.
Uriagereka, J. (2002), Derivations: Exploring the Dynamics of Syntax, London: Routledge.
Uriagereka, J. (2008), Syntactic Anchors: on Semantic Structuring, Cambridge University Press.
Vallauri, E. L. (2004), “The Relation between Mind and Language: the Innateness Hypothesis and the Poverty of Stimulus,” The Linguistic Review 21: 345–87.
Van der Hulst, H. (2010), “A Note on Recursion in Phonology,” in van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and Human Language, Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 301–41.
Varley, R., Klessinger, N., Romanowski, C., and Siegal, M. (2005), “Agrammatic but Numerate,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102: 3519–24.
Waddington, C. H. (1940), Organisers and Genes, Cambridge University Press.
Waddington, C. H. (1942), “Canalization of Development and the Inheritance of Acquired Characters,” Nature 150: 563–5.
Walton, M. T. and Walton, P. J. (1997), “The Geometrical Kabbalahs of John Dee and Johannes Kepler: the Hebrew Tradition and the Mathematical Study of Nature,” in Theerman, P. H. and Parshall, K. H. (eds.), Experiencing Nature: Proceedings of a Conference in Honor of Allen G. Debus, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 43–59.
Webelhuth, G. (1995), “Introduction,” in Webelhuth, G. (ed.), Government and Binding Theory and the Minimalist Program, Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 1–14.
Weinberg, S. (1976), “The Forces of Nature,” Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 29 (4): 13–29.
Whitehead, A. N. (1997[1925]), Science and the Modern World, New York: Pelican Mentor Books.
Wilks, Y. (1971), “Decidability and Natural Language,” Mind 80: 497–520.
Williams, E. (1987), “Introduction,” in Roeper, T. and Williams, E. (eds.), Parameter Setting, Dordrecht: D. Riedel.
Wilson, M. (1985), “What is This Thing Called ‘Pain’? The Philosophy of Science Behind the Contemporary Debate,” Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 66: 227–71.
Yang, C. (2010), “Three Factors in Language Variations,” Lingua 120: 1160–77.
Yoshimoto, Y. (2001), “A Review of the History of Constraints in the Pre-Minimalist Transformational Generative Grammar,” Scripsimus 10: 143–70.
Yourgrau, W. and Mandelstam, S. (1960), The Variational Principles in Dynamics and Quantum Theory, London: Pitman and Sons.