Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T01:22:16.951Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - The Executive Functions of Language in Preschool Children

from Part I - Theoretical and Biological Foundations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we examine how language relates to executive function (EF) in preschoolers. EF consists of higher cognitive functions that are involved in the control of thought, action, and emotion and that have been linked to neural systems involving the prefrontal cortex. Despite its importance, the meaning of EF has remained elusive (Lehto, Juujäarvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, 2003). Broadly defined, EF is “an umbrella term for all of the complex set of cognitive processes that underlie flexible goal-directed responses to novel or difficult situations” (Hughes & Graham, 2002, p. 131). Recent empirical work has clarified the composition and development of EF. Factor analytic studies have identified working memory, flexibility, and inhibition as three key components of executive function (Lehto et al., 2003; Miyake et al., 2000).

EF and its development has recently received considerable attention in developmental psychology, and several studies have charted the developmental trajectory of children's performance on numerous EF tasks (see Diamond, 2002; Zelazo & Müller, 2002, for reviews). This interest has been driven by findings that (a) EF is implicated in various developmental disorders such as autism (e.g., Hill, 2004; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Russell, 1997) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (e.g., Barkley, 2004; Mulas, Capilla, & Fernández, 2006), and (b) the development of executive function is significantly correlated with the development of social understanding, specifically false belief understanding (e.g., Frye, Zelazo, & Palfai, 1995; Perner & Lang, 1999; Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses, & Lee, 2006).

Type
Chapter

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: Evidence for task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641–657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P.Kim, J. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language?Brain and Language, 92, 240–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Thought and things, Vol. 1: Functional logic. New York: The Macmillan Company.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (2004). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and self-regulation: Taking an evolutionary perspective on executive functioning. In Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 301–323). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beaudichon, J. (1973). Nature and instrumental function of private speech in problem solving situations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 119–135.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1992). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blair, C. (2003). Behavorial inhibition and behavioral activation in young children: Relations with self-regulation and adaptation to preschool in children attending Head Start. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 301–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bühler, K. (1982). The axiomatization of the language sciences. In Innis, R. E. (Ed.), Karl Bühler: Semiotic foundation of language theory (pp. 91–164). New York: Plenum. (Original work published 1933)Google Scholar
Bühler, K. (1990). Theory of language: The representational function of language (Goodwin, D. F., Trans.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (Original work published 1934)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunge, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and panning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J. (2001). Disruption of inhibitory control in developmental disorders: A mechanistic model of implicated frontostriatal circuitry. In McClelland, J. & Siegler, R. S. (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (pp. 327–349). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cinan, S., & Tanör, Ö. Ö. (2002). An attempt to discriminate different types of executive functions in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Memory, 10, 277–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1995). The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D. C., Squire, L. R., Bihrle, A., & Massman, P. (1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting test. Neuropsychologia, 30, 683–697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). London: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, K., & Sussman, D. (1995). Toward a cognitive account of frontal lobe function: Simulating frontal lobe deficits in normal subjects. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 289–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, R. M., & Cheyne, J. A. (2002). Private speech in young adults: Task difficulty, self-regulation, and psychological predication. Cognitive Development, 16, 889–906.Google Scholar
Duncan, R. M., & Pratt, M. (1997). Microgenetic change in the quality and quantity of preschoolers' private speech. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 367–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5, Whole No. 270).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, M. J., & Drasgow, J. (1951). A Visual-Verbal Test for schizophrenia. Psychiatric Quarterly, 25(Suppl.), 55–64.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1970). Developmental studies of mediated memory. In Reese, H. W. & Lipsett, L. P. (Eds.), Advances in child development and child behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 983–1059). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Freund, L. S. (1990). Maternal regulation of children's problem-solving behavior and its impact on children's performance. Child Development, 61, 113–126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations between inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frye, D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2003). The development of young children's action control and awareness. In Roessler, J., & Eilan, N. (Eds.), Agency and self-awareness: Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 244–262). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauvain, M., & Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children's planning skills. Developmental Psychology, 25, 139–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Weisberg, R. W. (1966). Verbal behavior and problem solving: Some effects of labeling in a functional fixedness problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 659–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task set switching. In Monsell, S. & Driver, J. (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 331–355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hala, S., Hug, S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Executive function and false-belief understanding in preschool children: Two tasks are harder than one. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 275–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happaney, K. R., & Zelazo, P. D. (2003). Inhibition as a problem in the psychology of behavior. Developmental Science, 6, 468–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hegel, G. W. F. (1977). Phenomenology of spirit (A. V. Miller, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1807)Google Scholar
Herder, J. G. (1966). Essay on the origin of language. In Moran, J. H. & Gode, A. (Eds.), On the origin of language (pp. 87–166). New York: Frederick Ungar. (Original work published 1772)Google Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
House, B. J. (1989). Some current issues in children's selective attention. In Reese, H. W. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 21, pp. 91–119). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, C. (1998a). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C. (1998b). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind?Developmental Psychology, 34, 1526–1539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair?Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 645–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions?Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S. (2005). Some (verbal) thoughts on Language in Mind. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 458–463.Google Scholar
Jacques, S., Shiffman, M. A., & Parker, J. A. (2006). The role of language on cognitive flexibility in adults: A dual-task investigation. Unpublished manuscript.
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 573–591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005a). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: Implications for theory of mind. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 144–162). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005b). On the possible roots of cognitive flexibility. In Homer, B. D. & Tamis-Lemonda, C. S. (Eds.), The development of social understanding and communication (pp. 53–81). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jacques, S., Zelazo, P. D., Kirkham, N. Z., & Semcesen, T. K. (1999). Rule selection and rule execution in preschoolers: An error-detection approach. Developmental Psychology, 35, 770–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S., Zelazo, P. D., Lourenco, S. F., & Sutherland, A. E. (2006). The roles of labeling and abstraction in the development of cognitive flexibility. Unpublished manuscript.
Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive attention in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, T. (1998). Memory span as a predictor of false belief understanding. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 27, 36–43.Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H., & Kendler, T. S. (1961). Effect of verbalization on reversal shifts in children. Science, 134, 1619–1620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L. M., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 15–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, B., & Perner, J. (2002). Understanding of intention and false belief and the development of self-control. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehto, J. E., Juujäarvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, B., Stuss, D. T., & Milberg, W. P. (1995). Concept generation: Validation of a test of executive functioning in a normal aging population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 740–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1959). The directive function of speech in development and dissolution, Part I. Word, 15, 341–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour (Tizard, J., Ed.). New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M., Dodd, M. D., Sheard, E. D., Wilson, D. E., & Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 163–214). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115, 123–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montero, I., & Dios, M. J. (2006). Vygotsky was right. An experimental approach to the relationship between private speech and task performance. Estudios de Psicología, 27, 175–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulas, F., Capilla, A., & Fernández, S. (2006). Shifting-related brain magnetic activity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 373–379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 1594–1609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., & Imrisek, S. (2005). Executive function and children's understanding of false belief: How specific is the relation?Cognitive Development, 20, 173–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Lurye, L., & Liebermann, D. (2008). The effect of labeling on preschoolers' performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Cognitive Development, 23, 395–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, D. J. (1967). The role of speech responses in short-term memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 21, 263–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 337–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1937)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. (Original work published 1936)Google Scholar
Roebers, C. M., & Schneider, W. (2005). Individual differences in young children's suggestibility: Relations to event memory, language abilities, working memory, and executive functioning. Cognitive Development, 20, 427–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. (Ed.). (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and Theory of Mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 74–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saeki, E., & Saito, S. (2004a). Effect of articulatory suppression on task-switching performance: Implications for models of working memory. Memory, 12, 257–271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saeki, E., & Saito, S. (2004b). The role of the phonological loop in task switching performance: The effect of articulatory suppression in the alternating runs paradigm. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 47, 35–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soffer, G. (2000). Language and the formation of general concepts: The second logical investigation in a genetic light. Paper presented at the Boston Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science, Boston.Google Scholar
Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive Development, 15, 347–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1994). The problem of the cultural development of the child. In Veer, R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 57–72). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. (Original work published 1929)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In Wertsch, J. V. (Ed. & Trans.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 144–188). Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L., & Luria, A. (1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In Veer, R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 99–174). Oxford. UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1915). Begriffspsychologische Untersuchungen. Archiv für systematische Philosophie, 21, 162–172.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1926). Einführung in die Entwicklungspsychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., León, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behavior. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerys, B. E., & Munakata, Y. (2006). When labels hurt but novelty helps: Children's perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Development, 77, 1589–1607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (1999). Language, levels of consciousness, and the development of intentional action. In Zelazo, P. D., Astington, J. W., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 95–117). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 12–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1997). Cognitive complexity and control: A theory of the development of deliberate reasoning and intentional action. In Stamenov, M. (Ed.), Language structure, discourse, and the access to consciousness (pp. 113–153). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). II. Cognitive complexity and control: The development of executive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 121–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Jacques, S. (1996). Children's rule use: Representation, reflection and cognitive control. Annals of Child Development, 12, 119–176.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3, Serial No. 274).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×