Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T08:28:04.476Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The puzzle of nongenetic variance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Arthur R. Jensen
Affiliation:
University of California
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Within the past decade, empirical findings in behavior genetics have importantly changed how researchers in this field think heredity and environment affect individual differences in mental ability. These insights are hardly new. Some are even found in the writings of Sir Francis Galton (1822–1911), the father of behavior genetics. But the recognition, formalization, and empirical support given to them by behavior geneticists in recent years can be considered significant advances. The most surprising findings, only conjectured by Galton, concern the role of environment in the development of mental ability. The present picture is quite different from the beliefs generally held only a decade ago.

Genotype–environment covariance

One such idea is that the perceptible environment is like a cafeteria. People make different selections according to their genetic makeup, or genotype. The environment is not a “given” but is largely the person's own creation. This becomes increasingly true as persons develop from infancy to maturity.

Behavioral differences between persons that result from their self-selected and self-fashioned environments are the phenotypic expression of genotype–environment covariance. It accounts for more of the total variance (i.e., individual differences) in abilities and achievements than was formerly thought. Genotype–environment (GE) covariance is neither a strictly genetic nor a strictly environmental component of phenotypic variance but reflects the genetically driven differential selection of experiences from the available environment. It also includes the effects of differential treatment by parents, teachers, and peers, because their responses are largely evoked by the person's distinctive genotypic characteristics.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×