Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T08:44:11.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Intelligence, language, nature, and nurture in young twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

J. Steven Reznick
Affiliation:
Yale University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The relation between intelligence and language is complex. If intelligence is defined as the ability to solve problems, generate creative ideas, recognize emergent categories and patterns, or simply as processing power or speed of neural conduction, then language can be viewed as a secondary process in that it merely serves to communicate the workings of the intelligent mind. This language-independent conceptualization of intelligence can be contrasted with a language-dependent perspective, in which verbal skill per se is an integral aspect of intelligence. From the language-dependent perspective, intelligence includes abilities such as knowledge of word meanings, mastery of grammar and syntax, and skilled articulation of ideas.

Verbal and nonverbal skills co-occur in normal adults, and efforts to separate them are problematic: Most verbal tasks tap underlying nonverbal skills, and tasks that seem nonverbal may be influenced by verbal mediation of instructions or ongoing processes. However, from a developmental perspective, separating verbal and nonverbal skills is obvious and necessary. In the first year, most human infants begin to babble, use gestures, and understand and say a few words, but these activities pale in comparison to the infant's interest in object manipulation, locomotion, and visual exploration. Our terminology for the first year of life evokes this relative lack of language – the word infant is from the Latin infans, meaning “incapable of speech.” Infants do communicate, but their linguistic investment seems relatively minor in comparison to their other abilities and to the prodigious advances in language that characterize the second year.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×