Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T04:23:17.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Mirrors and matchings: imitation from the perspective of mirror-self-recognition, and the parietal region's involvement in both

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 December 2009

Robert W. Mitchell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Eastern Kentucky University, USA
Chrystopher L. Nehaniv
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Kerstin Dautenhahn
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In the movie Duck Soup, the Marx Brothers Harpo and Groucho come upon each other, face to face. There would have been a mirror between them, but Harpo, in his attempt to avoid being seen by Groucho whom he had been impersonating, ran into it and broke it. The two men look alike; to avoid detection, Harpo imitates Groucho's movements to make it appear that Groucho is seeing himself in a mirror. Testing the “mirror” image, Groucho performs a series of bizarre actions that Harpo imitates simultaneously, thereby supporting the ridiculous conclusion that Groucho is indeed looking in a mirror. The sequence nicely illustrates the similarities between bodily imitation and mirror images. In both, actions of one entity closely mimic the actions of another, such that a resemblance is apparent. Consequently, it seems reasonable to expect that the same processes that allow someone to imitate another person's actions (or to recognize that one's actions are being imitated) might also be useful in determining that the image in the mirror is oneself.

Remarkably, the first explicit recognition of the similarity between bodily imitation and mirror-self-recognition was quite recent, in 1925 by French psychologist Paul Guillaume, in his book about the imitative development of his daughters. Guillaume (1926/1971, p. 152) argued “We must not hesitate to connect this exceptional mirror experiment [i.e., the child's infrequent interactions with mirrors] with the experience of normal imitation. Other adults and also other children in the society in which he lives are for the infant the natural mirror that reflects for him his own image when he proceeds to imitate.”

Type
Chapter
Information
Imitation and Social Learning in Robots, Humans and Animals
Behavioural, Social and Communicative Dimensions
, pp. 103 - 130
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. R. (1994). The monkey in the mirror: a strange conspecific. In Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. and Boccia, M. L. (eds.), Self-awareness in Animals and Humans. New York: Cambridge University Press, 315–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anisfeld, M. (1991). Neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 11, 60–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anisfeld, M., Turkewitz, G., Rose, S. A., Rosenberg, F. R., Sheiber, F. J., Couturier-Fagan, D. A., Ger, J. S. and Sommer, I. (2001). No compelling evidence that neonates imitate oral gestures. Infancy, 2, 111–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S. and Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. M. (1898/1902). The Story of the Mind. New York: D. Appleton and Co.Google Scholar
Bekkering, H. and Wohlschläger, A. (2002). Action perception and imitation: a tutorial. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 294–314.Google Scholar
Bonda, E., Frey, S. and Petrides, M. (1996). Evidence for a dorso-medial parietal system involved in mental transformations of the body. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76, 2042–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature, 39, 756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger., A. and Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44, 124–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugger, P., Kollias, S. S., Müri, R. M., Crelier, G., Hepp-Reymond, M.-C. and Regard, M. (2000). Beyond re-membering: phantom sensations of congenitally absent limbs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 6167–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G. and Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–4.Google Scholar
Castiello, U., Lusher, D., Mari, M., Edwards, M. and Humphreys, G. W. (2002). Observing a human or a robotic hand grasping an object: Differential motor priming effects. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 315–33.Google Scholar
Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A. N. and Decety, J. (2002). Does the end justify the means? A PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation. NeuroImage, 15, 318–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, M. M. (1967). Continuous versus terminal visual feedback in prism aftereffects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 24, 1295–1302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corso, J. F. (1967). The Experimental Psychology of Sensory Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Craig, J. C. and Rollman, G. B. (1999). Somesthesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 305–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crammond, D. J. (1997). Motor imagery: never in your wildest dream. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 54–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creem, S. H., Downs, T. H., Wraga, M., Harrington, G. S., Proffitt, D. R. and Downs, J. H. III. (2001). An fMRI study of imagined self-rotation. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 1, 239–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creem-Regehr, S. H. (in press). Body mapping and spatial transformations. In Dolins, F. and Mitchell, R. W. (eds.), Spatial Perception, Spatial Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dearborn, G. V. N. (1916). Movement, cenesthesia, and the mind. Psychological Review, 23, 190–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Chaminade, T., Grèzes, J. and Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). A PET exploration of the neural mechanisms involved in reciprocal imitation. NeuroImage, 15, 265–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F. and Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., Tadary, B., Woods, R., Mazzlotta, J. C. and Fazio, F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature, 371, 600–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demiris, J. and Hayes, G. (2002). Imitation as a dual-route process featuring predictive and learning components: a biologically plausible computational model. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 327–61.Google Scholar
Doré, F. Y. (1986). Object permanence in adult cats (Felis catus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 340–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, M. and Brugger, P. (2002). Visual recognition of hands by persons born with only one hand. Cortex, 38, 860–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 334–55.Google Scholar
Gandevia, S. C. and Burke, D. (1992). Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 614–32.Google Scholar
Geldard, F. A. (1953). The Human Senses. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin – the representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, M. and Botvinick, M. (2002). How the brain represents the body: insights from neurophysiology and psychology. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 136–57.Google Scholar
Guillaume, P. (1925). L'imitation Chez L'Enfant [Imitation in Children]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Guillaume, P. (1926/1971). Imitation in Children (trans. E. P. Halperin). Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Harris, C. S. (1965). Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. Psychological Review, 72, 419–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. and Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iriki, A. (in press). “Understanding” of external space generated by bodily re-mapping: an insight from the neurophysiology of tool-using monkeys. In Dolins, F. and Mitchell, R. W. (eds.), Spatial Perception, Spatial Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., Obayashi, S. and Iwamura, Y. (2001). Self-images in the video monitor coded by monkey intraparietal neurons. Neuroscience Research, 40, 163–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (1986). Mechanisms of visuomotor coordination: a study in normal and brain-damaged subjects. Neuropsychologia, 24, 41–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H. and Westerberg, H. (2002). Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornheiser, A. S. (1976). Adaptation for laterally displaced vision: a review. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 783–816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. M., Digirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L. and Alpert, N. M. (1998). Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology, 35, 151–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krubitzer, L. and Disbrow, E. (in press). The evolution of the parietal areas involved in hand use in primates. In Dolins, F. and Mitchell, R. W. (eds.), Spatial Perception, Spatial Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lackner, J. R. (1988). Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. Brain, 111, 281–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B. and Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes, II: imitation and utilization behavior: a neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19, 335–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Major, D. R. (1906). First Steps in Mental Growth. New York: The Macmillan Co.Google Scholar
Maurer, D., Stager, C. L. and Mondloch, C. J. (1999). Cross-modal transfer of shape is difficult to demonstrate in one-month-olds. Child Development, 70, 1047–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: the role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. and Beeghly, M. (eds.), The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 139–64.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melzack, R. (1989). Phantom limbs, the self, and the brain. Canadian Psychology, 30, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menzel, E. W., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S.Jr. and Lawson, J. (1985). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) spatial problem solving with the use of mirrors and televised equivalents of mirrors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 211–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, H. L. (1994). ME CHANTEK: The development of self-awareness in a signing orangutan. In Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. and Boccia, M. L. (eds.), Self-awareness in Animals and Humans. New York: Cambridge University Press, 254–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, H. L., Mitchell, R. W. and Harper, S. (1996). Simon says: the development of imitation in an enculturated orangutan. In Russon, A., Bard, K. and Parker, S. T. (eds.), Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. New York: Cambridge University Press, 278–99.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1993a). Mental models of mirror-self-recognition: two theories. New Ideas in Psychology, 11, 295–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1993b). Recognizing one's self in a mirror? A reply to Gallup and Povinelli, De Lannoy, Anderson, and Byrne. New Ideas in Psychology, 11, 351–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1994a). The evolution of primate cognition: simulation, self-knowledge, and knowledge of other minds. In Quiatt, D. and Itani, J. (eds.), Hominid Culture in Primate Perspective. Boulder: University Press of Colorado, 177–232.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1994b). Are motor images based on kinesthetic–visual matching?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 214–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1997a). A comparison of the self-awareness and kinesthetic–visual matching theories of self-recognition: autistic children and others. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 818, 39–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (1997b). Kinesthetic–visual matching and the self-concept as explanations of mirror-self-recognition. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 27, 101–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (2002a). Imitation as a perceptual process. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 441–69.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (2002b). Subjectivity and self-recognition in animals. In Leary, M. R. and Tangney, J. (eds.), Handbook of Self and Identity. New York: Guilford Press, 567–93.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (2002c). Kinesthetic–visual matching, imitation, and self-recognition. In Bekoff, M., Allen, C. and Burghardt, G. (eds.), The Cognitive Animal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 345–51.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (2002d). Review of Imitation in Infancy, ed. Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G.. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 150–1.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. (2006). Pretense in Animals: the continuing relevance of children's pretense. In Göncü, A. and Gaskins, S. (eds.), Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 51–76.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. and Anderson, J. R. (1993). Discrimination learning of scratching, but failure to obtain imitation and self-recognition in a long-tailed macaque. Primates, 34, 301–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.) (1999). Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (2002). The correspondence problem. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 41–61.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Collier-Baker, E., Davis, J. and Suddendorf, T. (2005). Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 8, 31–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, J. A. (1985). Autotopagnosia: occurrence in a patient without nominal aphasia and with an intact ability to point to parts of animals and objects. Brain, 108, 1009–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: a study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 45–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. and Boccia, M. L. (eds.) (1994). Self-awareness in Animals and Humans. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one's body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 709–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Parsons, L. M. and Fox, P. T. (1998). The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 583–615.Google Scholar
Parsons, L. M., Fox, P. T., Downs, J. H., Glass, T., Hirsch, T. B., Martin, C. C., Jerabek, P. A. and Lancaster, J. L. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature, 375, 54–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfield, W. (1950). The Cerebral Cortex of Man: A Clinical Study of Localization of Function. New York: Hafner Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1945/1962). Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posada, S. and Colell, M. (2005). Mirror responses in a group of Mitopithecus talopoin. Primates, 46, 165–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulson, C. L., Nunes, L. R. de P. and Warren, S. F. (1989). Imitation in infancy: a critical review. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 22, 271–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). The perception of phantom limbs. Brain, 121, 1603–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S. and Blakesee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the Brain. New York: Morrow.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (1997). Adaptive Spatial Alignment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Redon, C., Hay, L., Rigal, R. and Roll, J. P. (1994). Contribution of the propriomuscular channel to movement coding in children: a study involving the use of vibration-induced kinaesthetic illusion. Human Movement Science, 13, 95–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riopelle, A. J., Nos, R. and Jonch, A. (1971). Situational determinants of dominance in captive young gorillas. Proceedings of the 3rd International Congress of Primatology, 3, 86–91.Google Scholar
Rivera, S. M., Wakeley, A. and Langer, J. (1999). The drawbridge phenomenon: representational reasoning or perceptual preference?Developmental Psychology, 35, 427–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J. A., Connell, S., McKenzie, B. E. and Day, R. H. (1990). Do infants use their own images to locate objects reflected in a mirror?Child Development, 61, 1558–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumiati, R. I. (2002). Processing mechanisms and neural structures involved in the recognition and production of actions: introduction to Section III. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 289–93.Google Scholar
Schiffman, H. R. (1976). Sensation and Perception. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Sekiyama, K. (1982). Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 89–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekiyama, K. (1983). Mental and physical movements of hands: kinesthetic information preserved in representational systems. Japanese Psychological Research, 25, 95–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999). The Primacy of Movement. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiffrar, M. and Pinto, J. (2002). The visual analysis of bodily movement. In Prinz, W. and Hommel, B. (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 381–99.Google Scholar
Sirigu, A. and Duhamel, J. R. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 910–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, I. M. (1995). Imitation and Gestural Representation in Autism. Poster presented at meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, Indiana.Google Scholar
Smith, I. M. and Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, B. E., Wallace, M. T. and Stanford, T. R. (1999). Development of multisensory integration: transforming sensory input into motor output. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5, 72–85.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J. F. (1992). The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 691–700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, G. F. (1901). A Manual of Psychology. London: University Tutorial Press.Google Scholar
Streri, A. and Gentaz, E. (2003). Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 20, 13–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G. and Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1746–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Tracy, F. (1901). The Psychology of Childhood, 5th edn. Boston: D. C. Heath and Co.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. (1986). Properties, parts, and objects. In Boff, K. R., Kaufman, L. and Thomas, J. P. (eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, Vol. II: Cognitive Processes and Performance. New York: Wiley, Chap. 35, 1–70.Google Scholar
Užgiris, I. Č. (1999). Imitation as activity: its developmental aspects. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ventre, J., Flandrin, J. M. and Jeannerod, M. (1984). In search for the egocentric reference: a neurophysiological hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 22, 797–806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fieandt, K. (1966). The World of Perception. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
Wu, C. W.-H. and Kaas, J. H. (2002). The effects of long-standing limb loss on anatomical reorganization of the somatosensory afferents in the brainstem and spinal cord. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 19, 153–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×