Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T10:40:35.251Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Copying strategies by people with autistic spectrum disorder: why only imitation leads to social cognitive development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 December 2009

Justin H. G. Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Child Health, University of Aberdeen Medical School, UK
Chrystopher L. Nehaniv
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Kerstin Dautenhahn
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In neuroscience, if we wish to identify the brain areas that serve particular functions, a common approach is to study behaviour when part of the brain has been rendered inoperative as a result of injury or illness. By identifying the functional loss of a particular brain area, we can learn something about the role of that brain area in normal conditions. One problem with this approach is that it treats the brain as if its functional specialization was static rather than a dynamic and developing process. A brain area may feasibly serve a role in the development of a function, but lose that later in life. Therefore, we need to understand the processes that are necessary for the development of function and ask about the brain mechanisms that serve them. Then, if we can examine a condition in which those processes are impaired, and we can find the brain bases for that impairment, this might be a better way to appreciate the role of that brain area in serving the function in question. The way that we understand other people's thoughts may be a good case in point. It may be profitable to study the processes that contribute to the development of social understanding, if we are to understand its neural substrate in adulthood. Imitation seems likely to be one important developmental process, necessary for the acquisition of culture, language, knowledge and social cognitive abilities (Whiten et al., 2003; Meltzoff and Moore, 1999).

Type
Chapter
Information
Imitation and Social Learning in Robots, Humans and Animals
Behavioural, Social and Communicative Dimensions
, pp. 199 - 216
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avikainen, S., Wohlschlager, A., Liuhanen, S., Hanninen, R., Hari, R.et al. (2003). Impaired mirror-image imitation in Asperger and high-functioning autistic subjects. Current Biology, 13, 339–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U. and Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage, 12, 314–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Another look at imitation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 403–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrino, di G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S.et al. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of ‘theory of mind’ in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. and Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds – a biological basis. Science, 286, 1692–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. and Frith, U. (2000). The physiological basis of theory of mind: functional neuroimaging studies. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding Other Minds – Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 334–56.Google Scholar
Gallagher, H. L., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U. and Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. and Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, R. M. (1996). ‘Radical’ simulationism. In Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. K. (eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 11–21.CrossRef
Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A. L., Huber, J. and Henderson, S. E. (2002). The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's Syndrome: a comparison with Specific Developmental Disorder of Motor Function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 655–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grezes, J., Costes, N. and Decety, J. (1998). Top-down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a PET investigation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 553–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C.et al. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport, 8, 197–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermsdorfer, J., Goldenberg, G., Wachsmuth, C., Conrad, B., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O.et al. (2001). Cortical correlates of gesture processing: clues to the cerebral mechanisms underlying apraxia during the imitation of meaningless gestures. Neuroimage, 14, 149–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 253–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. and Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 649–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P., Lee, A. and Meyer, J. (2003). Identification in autism. Presentation at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, Florida.
Hughes, C. (1996). Brief report: planning problems in autism at the level of motor control. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 99–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Russell, J. and Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, S. and Chater, N. (eds.) (2004). Perspectives on Imitation: from Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science, Vols. 1 and 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. and Chater, N. (2004). Perspectives on Imitation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. C.et al. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 13995–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C. and Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M. and Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L.Jr., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A. and Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule – generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. and Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 358, 491–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. and Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism, 1st edn. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 335–66.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–92.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1999). Persons and representation: why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 7–35.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Prinz, W. (2002). The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (2002). The correspondence problem. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 41–61.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research and Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roeyers, H., Oost, P. and Bothuyne, S. (1998). Immediate imitation and joint attention in young children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 10, 441–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in Infancy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 254–83.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J. and Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 3, 137–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R. and Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, 2060–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russon, A. E. (1998). Exploiting the expertise of others. In Whiten, A. and Byrne, R. W. (eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence II – extensions and evaluations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 174–206.Google Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., , Volkmar F.et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Kleinman, J., , G. C., Marois, R.et al. (2003). The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 358, 415–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, I. M. and Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, I. M. and Bryson, S. E. (1998). Gesture imitation in autism I: nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, W. L., Lemanek, K. L., Fishel, P. T., Fernandez, M. C. and Altemeier, W. A. (1990). Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of autism in young children. Pediatrics, 86, 267–72.Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2002). Skill and cognition in stone tool production. An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 43, 693–722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. and Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toni, I. and Passingham, R. E. (1999). Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor associations: a PET study. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 19–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happe, F., Falkai, P.et al. (2001). Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage, 14, 170–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weeks, S. J. and Hobson, R. P. (1987). The salience of facial expression for autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 137–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (1996). When does smart behaviour-reading become mind-reading? In Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. K. (eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 277–92.CrossRef
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape?Learning and Behaviour, 32, 36–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Horner, V. and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2003). Cultural Panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 92–105.Google Scholar
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T. and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A. and Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 285–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I. and Whiten, A. (in press). An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×