Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:34:09.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Global Literacy

Patterns and Variations

from Part II - Neurobiological and Ecological Markers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2023

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Sonali Nag
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

In this final chapter, a wide-angle perspective is provided on the major issues that arise when one thinks globally about literacy. Despite many important differences across the globe, literacy development suggests some general patterns that reflect nearly universal phases and shared operating principles. Although the “development” of literacy begins early through natural processes of language development and conceptual development, it is school experiences, explicitly designed to teach reading, that we expect to ensure the achievement of literacy. Each classroom within a school is its own literacy ecosystem, with one or more teachers, students, literacy curriculum materials, assesments, and regulated interactions. The classroom itself is embedded within other systems – the school, the community, the larger school administrative units, and local, regional, and national government control agents. A Global Literacy Framework is presented, showing that literacy development is embedded in language development and can be predicted by (a) system factors, referring to variations in the linguistic and writing systems, (b) child factors, associated with the neurobiological foundation of children’s learning capacity, and (c) support factors, associated with processes in the home and at school. All these influences exist within a sociopolitical context that exerts influence broadly across the system.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Australian Dyslexia Association. (2021). What is dyslexia? (web page). Sydney: ADA. https://dyslexiaassociation.org.au/what-is-dyslexia/.Google Scholar
Bender, P., Dutcher, N., Klaus, D., Shore, J., & Tesar, C. (2005). In Their Own Language: Education for All. Education Notes. Washington, DC: World Bank. http://hdl.handle.net/10986/10331.Google Scholar
Best, M., Knight, P., Lietz, P., Lockwood, C., Nugroho, D., & Tobin, M. (2013). The Impact of National and International Assessment Programmes on Education Policy, Particularly Policies Regarding Resource Allocation and Teaching and Learning Practices in Developing Countries. Camberwell, VIC: ACEReSearch. https://research.acer.edu.au/ar_misc/16/.Google Scholar
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65(1), 121. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/1170476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, L.-Y., Chen, Y.-C., & Perfetti, C. A. (2017). GraphCom: A multidimensional measure of graphic complexity applied to 131 written languages. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 427449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T. (1992). The syllabic origin of writing and the segmental origin of the alphabet. In Downing, P. A., Lima, S. D., & Noonan, M. (eds.), The Linguistics of Literacy (pp. 83110). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T., & Share, D. L. (2018). Writing system variation and its consequences for reading and dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 101116. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1379082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children’s kindergarten and fourth‐grade language and reading abilities. Child Development, 82(3), 870886. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01576.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowd, A. J., Friedlander, E. W., Jonason, C. et al. (2017). Lifewide learning for early reading development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 155, 3149. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, J. G., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). The Dyslexia Debate. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139017824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 263279. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hair, N. L., Hanson, J. L., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2015). Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement.JAMA Pediatrics, 169(9), 822829. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, J., Albert, W., Skinner, A., Shen, S., Dodge, K., & Lansford, J. (2019). Resting state coupling between the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex is related to household income in childhood and indexes future psychological vulnerability to stress. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 10531066. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579419000592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.Google Scholar
International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement. (2013). The Impact of National and International Assessment Programmes on Education Policy, Particularly Policies Regarding Resource Allocation and Teaching and Learning Practices in Developing Countries. Amsterdam: IEA. www.iea.nl/publications/study-reports/international-reports-iea-studies/impact-national-and-international.Google Scholar
Leyva, D., Weiland, C., Barata, M. et al. (2015). Teacher–child interactions in Chile and their associations with prekindergarten outcomes. Child Development, 86(3), 781799. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride, C., Pan, D. J., & Mohseni, F. (2021). Reading and writing words: A cross-linguistic perspective.Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(2), 114. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1920595.Google Scholar
Merz, E. C., Wiltshire, C. A., & Noble, K. G. (2019). Socioeconomic inequality and the developing brain: Spotlight on language and executive function. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 1520. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, S. (2007). Early reading in Kannada: The pace of acquisition of orthographic knowledge and phonemic awareness. Journal of Reading in Research, 30, 722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, S, Vagh, S. B., Dulay, K. & Snowling, M. J. (2019). Context and implications: Home language, school language and children’s literacy attainments: A systematic review of evidence from low- and middle-income countries. Review of Education, 7(1), 151155. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/rev3.3132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Center for Education Statistics. (2022). Education Expenditures by Country. Condition of Education. US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences. https://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator/cmd.Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H. et al. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 773778. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3983CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, R. K., Keenan, J. M., Byrne, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2019). Etiology of developmental dyslexia. In Verhoeven, L., Perfetti, C., & Pugh, K. (eds.), Developmental Dyslexia across Languages and Writing Systems (pp. 391412). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(1), 324. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization and universals in the development of reading skill: How Chinese research informs a universal science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 521. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.689786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. A., & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9(4), 296316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 2237. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.827687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., Verhoeven, L., & Pugh, K. (2019). Developmental dyslexia across languages and writing systems: The big picture. In Verhoeven, L., Perfetti, C. A., & Pugh, K. (eds.), Developmental Dyslexia across Languages and Writing Systems (pp. 441461). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Program for International Student Assessment (PISA). (n.d.). www.oecd.org/pisa/.Google Scholar
Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS). (n.d.). https://nces.ed.gov/surveys/pirls/.Google Scholar
Rowan, B., Correnti, R., & Miller, R. J. (2002). What Large-Scale, Survey Research Tells Us about Teacher Effects on Student Achievement: Insights from the Prospectus Study of Elementary Schools. Philadelphia: CPRE Research Reports. https://repository.upenn.edu/cpre_researchreports/31.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In McCardle, P., Ren, J., Tzeng, O., & Miller, B. (eds.), Dyslexia across Languages: Orthography and the Brain-Gene-Behavior Link (pp. 146168). New York: Brookes.Google Scholar
Shure, D., Parameshwaran, M., Nag, S. & Snowling, M. J. (2014). Economic and Social Factors related to Literacy and Foundation Learning, Technical Report No. 5: Literacy, Foundation Learning and Assessment in Developing Countries. London: DfID.Google Scholar
Taylor, J., Roehrig, A. D., Soden Hensler, B., Connor, C. M., & Schatschneider, C. (2010). Teacher quality moderates the genetic effects on early reading. Science, 328(5977), 512514. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1186149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UNESCO. (2019). Out of school children and youth. http://uis.unesco.org/en/topic/out-school-children-and-youth.Google Scholar
Vágvölgyi, R., Coldea, A., Dresler, T., Schrader, J., & Nuerk, H. C. (2016). A review about functional illiteracy: Definition, cognitive, linguistic, and numerical aspects.Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1617. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011). Introduction to this special issue: Vocabulary growth and reading skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (eds.). (2017).Learning to Read across Languages and writing systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., Perfetti, C.A., & Pugh, K. (eds.) (2019). Developmental Dyslexia across Languages and Writing Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2021). Universals in learning to read across languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 115. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1938575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., Perfetti, C., & Pugh, K. (2019). Cross-linguistic perspectives on second language reading. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 16. DOI: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Snow, C. E. (eds.). (2001). Introduction: Literacy and motivation: Bridging cognitive and sociocultural viewpoints. In Verhoeven, L. & Snow, C. E. (eds.), Literacy and Motivation: Reading Engagement in Individuals and Groups. New York: Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410601735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickins, C., & Sandlin, J. (2007). Literacy for what? Literacy for whom? The politics of literacy education and neocolonialism in UNESCO- and World Bank sponsored literacy programs. Adult Education Quarterly, 57, 275292. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741713607302364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading to the amount and breadth or their reading.Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 420. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×