Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T00:27:11.168Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The discovery of cerebral diversity: an unwelcome scientific revolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2011

Todd M. Preuss
Affiliation:
University of Louisiana
Dean Falk
Affiliation:
State University of New York, Albany
Kathleen R. Gibson
Affiliation:
University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston
Get access

Summary

Studies of mammalian and primate brain evolution have traditionally focused on changes in encephalization, that is, changes in brain size statistically adjusted to compensate for changes in body size, rather than on changes in the internal organization of the brain. There are some very sound reasons for stressing size. Mammals do indeed vary dramatically in absolute and relative brain size: at a given body weight, brain weight can vary more than five-fold across species (Stephan et al., 1988). Moreover, brain size changes can have profound consequences for the developmental biology and ecology of mammalian taxa, because larger-brained taxa grow more slowly and live longer than do smaller-brained taxa of comparable body size (Sacher, 1982; Finlay & Darlington, 1995) and because brain tissue is energetically very demanding (Aiello & Wheeler, 1995). Conveniently, brain size is relatively tractable empirically, which is to say that one can measure it with reasonable precision in all sorts of living and extinct taxa, whereas the internal features of brain organization can be examined only with difficulty in extant taxa and not at all in extinct forms. Finally, there can be little doubt that variations in brain size are in some way related to variations in cognitive and behavioral abilities.

But in precisely what ways are brain size, cognition, and behavior related? Harry Jerison has ably articulated the view that encephalization serves as an index of general animal intelligence (see especially Jerison, 1961, 1973), and in doing so has provided the underpinning for modern brain allometry studies.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×