Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T01:41:25.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Teaching as a Natural Cognitive Ability: Implications for Classroom Practice and Teacher Education

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 December 2009

Sidney Strauss
Affiliation:
School of Education, Tel Aviv University, Israel
David B. Pillemer
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Sheldon H. White
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Get access

Summary

This is a chapter about why we teach. I do not ask what the best way is to teach this subject matter or that. Nor do I ask how we can assess children's learning as a result of teaching. Instead, I ask a deceptively simple question: Why do we teach in the first place? The search for answers to that question takes us to the borders between our biological, psychological, and cultural endowment as humans.

Teaching, or folk pedagogy, the social transformation of knowledge from one person to another or the attempt to engender it in others, is one of the most remarkable of human enterprises. I propose that teaching, which is central to education in the broad sense of that term, can also be seen as an essential domain of inquiry for the cognitive sciences. This is so because, as I attempt to show, teaching may be a natural cognitive ability and is essential to what it means to be a human being. Furthermore, I believe that a search for the cognitive underpinnings of teaching may lead to a description of some of the fundamental building blocks of human cognition and its development.

Learning, teaching's mirror image, has been a major focus of the cognitive sciences, to be sure, but intentional pedagogy aimed to cause learning has, by and large, been flying below the cognitive sciences' radar.

Type
Chapter
Information
Developmental Psychology and Social Change
Research, History and Policy
, pp. 368 - 388
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashley, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development 7, 143–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Pelletier, J. (1996). The language of mind: Its role in teaching and learning. In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Handbook of Education and Human Development (pp. 593–620). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M., & Hauser, M. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals. The Quarterly Review of Biology 67, 151–174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Waal, F. (1996). Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Waal, F. (1998). Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University PressGoogle Scholar
Feldman, D. H. (1994). Beyond Universals in Cognitive Development. (2nd ed.). Norwood, NJ: AblexGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (2000). The Mind Doesn't Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Frye, D., & Ziv, M. (in press). Teaching and learning as intentional activities. In Strauss, S. (Ed.), Theories of Mind and Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. A., & Kearns, M. J. (1991). On the Complexity of Teaching. Paper presented at the Twenty-Eighth Annual Symposium on Foundations of Computer Science
Haim, O., Strauss, S., & Ravid, D. (in press). Relations between EFL teachers' formal knowledge of grammar and their in-action mental models of children's minds and learning. Teaching and Teacher Education
Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., & Furth, C. (1996). “Theory of mind” in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroReport 8, 197–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. (2000). Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think. New York: Henry HoltGoogle Scholar
Kruger, A. C., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Cultural learning and learning culture. In Olson, D. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Handbook of Human Development and Education (pp. 369–387). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development 73, 969–982CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parisi, D., & Schlesinger, M. (2002). Artificial life and Piaget. Cognitive Development 17, 1301–1321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, A. T. (1989). The Teacher: Theory and Practice in Teacher Education. New York: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1979). Formal models of language learning. Cognition 8, 217–283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 61, 2 (Serial no. 247)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1997). Specificity of gaze-following in young chimpanzees. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 15, 213–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (1984). Pedagogy and aesthetics as sources of culture. In Gazzaniga, M. (Ed.), Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 15–35). New York: PlenumCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (1991). The aesthetic basis of pedagogy. In Hoffman, R. R. & Palermo, D. S. (Eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes: Applied and Ecological Perspectives (pp. 303–325). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (1993). Prolegomenon to evolution of cognition. In Poggio, T. A. & Glaser, D. A. (Eds.), Exploring Brain Functions: Models in Neuroscience (pp. 269–290). New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1994). Why animals have neither culture nor history. In Ingold, T. (Ed.), Companion Encyclopedia of Anthropology: Humanity, Culture and Social Life (pp. 350–365). London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1996). Why animals lack pedagogy and some cultures have more of it than others. In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Handbook of Human Development and Education (pp. 302–344). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (2003). Original Intelligence: Unlocking the Mystery of Who We Are. New York: McGraw HillGoogle Scholar
Reddy, M. (1979). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in language about language. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (2nd ed., pp. 164–201). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural correlates of theory-of-mind reasoning: An event-related potential study. Psychological Science 11, 46–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayag, T., & Strauss, S. (2004). Teaching & artificial life. In Ravid, D. & Shyldkrot, H. Bat-Zeev (Eds.), Perspectives on Language and Language Development: Essays in Honor of Ruth A. Berman (pp. 159–171). Dodrecht, Holland: KluwerGoogle Scholar
Sfard, A. (1998). On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one. Educational Researcher 27, 4–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational Researcher 15, 4–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10, 640–656CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, S. (1993). Theories of learning and development for academics and educators. Educational Psychologist 28, 191–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, S. (2001). Folk psychology, folk pedagogy and their relations to subject matter knowledge. In Torff, B. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Understanding and Teaching the Intuitive Mind (pp. 217–242). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Strauss, S., Ravid, D., Magen, N., & Berliner, D. C. (1998). Relations between teachers' subject matter knowledge, teaching experience and their mental models of children's minds and learning. Teaching and Teacher Education 14, 579–595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss S., Ravid, D., Zelcer, H., & Berliner, D. C. (1999). Teachers' subject matter knowledge and their belief systems about children's learning. In Nunes, T. (Ed.), Learning to Read: An Integrated View from Research and Practice (pp. 259–282). London: KluwerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, S., & Shilony, T. (1994). Teachers' models of children's minds and learning. In Hirschfeld, L. and Gelman, S. (Eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain-Specificity in Cognition and Culture (pp. 455–473). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, S., & Ziv, M. (2001). Requests for words are a request for teaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, 1118–1119Google Scholar
Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers' developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development 17, 1473–1487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 495–511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, S. M., Shulman, L. S., & Richert, E. (1987). ‘150 ways of knowing’: Representations of knowledge in teaching. In Calderhead, J. (Ed.), Exploring Teachers' Thinking (pp. 104–124). London: CassellGoogle Scholar
Wood, D., Wood, H., Ainsworth, S., & Malley, C. (1995). On becoming a tutor: Toward an ontogenetic model. Cognition and Instruction 13, 565–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziv, M., Strauss, S., & Porat, A. (in preparation). Developmental Differences in Early Childhood Concerning Children's Understanding of Teaching, Play, and Theory of Mind

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×