Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T01:25:01.135Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Genetic and Environmental Influences on Continuity and Change in Reading Achievement in the Colorado Adoption Project

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2009

Sally J. Wadsworth
Affiliation:
Senior Research Associate at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder
Robin Corley
Affiliation:
Senior Research Associate at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder
Robert Plomin
Affiliation:
Professor and Deputy Director in Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry, Research Centre Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London
John K. Hewitt
Affiliation:
Professor and Director of the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder
John C. DeFries
Affiliation:
Professor at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder
Aletha C. Huston
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
Marika N. Ripke
Affiliation:
University of Hawaii, Manoa
Get access

Summary

Mastery of reading skills in childhood has far-reaching implications for development. Serving as a foundation for all academic learning, reading performance affects not only educational attainment, but also employment options, interpersonal relationships, and emotional well-being. Although this essential skill continues to develop throughout adolescence and adulthood, measures of reading achievement are highly stable, with correlations ranging from .23 to .96 over intervals of 1 to 8 years (e.g., Bast & Reitsma, 1998; DeFries, 1988; DeFries & Baker, 1983; Shaywitz, Escobar, Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Makuch, 1992; Wadsworth, Corley, Hewitt, & DeFries, 2001; Wagner et al., 1997; Williams & McGee, 1996). Results obtained from both twin and adoption studies have provided compelling evidence for substantial genetic influences on individual differences in reading performance from age 7 to adulthood, with heritability estimates ranging from .18 to .81 (e.g., Alarcón & DeFries, 1995; Cardon, DiLalla, Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1990; Knopik & DeFries, 1999; Stevenson, Graham, Fredman, & McLoughlin, 1987; Wadsworth et al., 2001). Despite the striking evidence for stability and heritability, reading achievement also shows some change as a function of environmental influences. All of these factors – genes and environment, continuity and change – are needed for a complete understanding of the development of reading achievement.

The evidence that individual differences in reading achievement are both stable and heritable throughout development suggests that genetic influences contribute to continuity. Genetic influences that are important at an early age may also be important at later ages.

Type
Chapter
Information
Developmental Contexts in Middle Childhood
Bridges to Adolescence and Adulthood
, pp. 87 - 106
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alarcón, M., & DeFries, J. C. (1995). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability: An alternative test [Abstract]. Behavior Genetics, 25, 253.Google Scholar
Bast, J., & Reitsma, P. (1998). Analyzing the development of individual differences in terms of Matthew effects of reading: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1373–1399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardon, L. R., DiLalla, L. F., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1990). Genetic correlations between reading performance and IQ in the Colorado Adoption Project. Intelligence, 14, 245–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherny, S. S., Fulker, D. W., & Hewitt, J. K. (1997). Cognitive development from infancy to middle childhood. In Sternberg, R. & Grigorenko, E. (Eds.) Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 463–482). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
DeFries, J. C. (1988). Colorado Reading Project: Longitudinal analyses. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 120–130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFries, J. C., & Baker, L. A. (1983). Parental contributions to longitudinal stability of cognitive measures in the Colorado Family Reading Study. Child Development, 54, 388–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & Fulker, D. W. (1994). Nature and nurture during middle childhood. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & LaBuda, M. (1987). Genetic stability of cognitive development from childhood to adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 23, 4–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dibble, E., & Cohen, D. J. (1974). Companion instruments for measuring children's competence and parental style. Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 805–815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970). Examiner's Manual: Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E., & DeFries, J. C. (2002). Developmental dyslexia: Genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 767–782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulker, D. W., & Cherny, S. S. (1995). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition during childhood. Population Research and Policy Review, 14, 283–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulker, D. W., Cherny, S. S., & Cardon, L. R. (1993). Continuity and change in cognitive development. In Plomin, R. & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 99–120). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Jimerson, S., Egeland, B., & Teo, A. (1999). A longitudinal study of achievement trajectories: Factors associated with change. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 116–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knopik, V. S., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). Etiology of covariation between reading and mathematics performance: A twin study. Twin Research, 2, 226–234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koutsoulis, M. K., & Campbell, J. R. (2001). Family processes affect students' motivation, and science and math achievement in Cypriot high schools. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 108–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective. In Plomin, R. & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.) Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 59–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). The Family Environmental Scale manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. NATO ASI series. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2002). Mx: Statistical modeling. (6th ed.) [Computer software], Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University.Google Scholar
Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. C. (2003). Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and nurture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1979). Multivariate behavioral as genetic analysis of twin data on scholastic abilities. Behavior Genetics, 9, 505–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R. & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado Adoption Project. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Corley, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1997). Nature, nurture, and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: A parent-offspring adoption study. Psychological Science, 8, 442–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhea, S. A., & Corley, R. (1994). Applied Issues. In DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & Fulker, D. W. (Eds.) Nature and nurture during middle childhood (pp. 295–309). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Robinson, N. M., Lanzi, R. G., Weinberg, R. A., Ramey, S. L., & Ramey, C. T. (2002). Family factors associated with high academic competence in former head start children at third grade. Gifted Child Quarterly, 46, 278–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makuch, R. (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 144–150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, J., Graham, P., Fredman, G., & McLoughlin, V. (1987). A twin study of genetic influences on reading and spelling ability and disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 229–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoolmiller, M. (1998). Correcting estimates of shared environmental variance for range restriction in adoption studies using a truncated multivariate normal model. Behavior Genetics, 28, 429–441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 392–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadsworth, S. J., Corley, R., Hewitt, J. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2001). Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7, 12, and 16 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. Behavior Genetics, 31, 353–359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadsworth, S. J., & DeFries, J. C. (2003). Etiology of the stability of reading performance from 7 to 12 years of age and its possible mediation by IQ. In Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.) Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence (pp. 49–61). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadsworth, S. J., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7 and 12 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 319–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, et al . (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. W. (1988). Walker-McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Widaman, K., & O'Neil, R. (2001). Linkages between children's social and academic competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 463–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1974). Examiner's manual: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Williams, S., & McGee, R. (1996). Reading in childhood and mental health in early adulthood. In Beitchman, J. H., Cohen, N.J., Konstantareas, M. M., & Tannock, R. (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives (pp. 530–554). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×