Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:12:13.644Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Relationship-Specific Intergenerational Family Ties: An Evolutionary Approach to the Structure of Cultural Transmission

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ute Schönpflug
Affiliation:
Freie Universität Berlin
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Cultural transmission encompasses those processes that transmit and modify beliefs, attitudes, and values in a population. To understand these processes, it might not be satisfactory merely to know how the existing structures of cultural transmission give rise to cultural change. Rather, we also need to understand why cultural transmission has the structures that it shows (Boyd & Richerson, 1985; Richerson & Boyd, 2003). Evolutionary theories of human behavior are indispensable for finding satisfactory answers to this “why” question. Such theories address the question of why the human mind (or that of other animals) holds the particular design features that it does, as opposed to other, nonexistent yet conceivable ones (Pinker, 1997; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). Because culture is a product of the mind, mind features can explain cultural phenomena to some extent (Dawkins, 1982; Miller, 2000), including cultural transmission. Evolutionary theories may facilitate understanding by guiding us to see the relevant aspects of natural phenomena or, as Socrates suggested and Plato wrote, to “carve nature at its joints” (Gangestad & Snyder, 1985, p. 317). Such guidance may help to steer us away from suboptimal concepts like those inspired by changing zeitgeist or individual predilections. With it, we avoid becoming entangled in an overabundance of unconnected middle-level theories, a state that characterizes several disciplines of human behavior and impedes scientific progress.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cultural Transmission
Psychological, Developmental, Social, and Methodological Aspects
, pp. 70 - 92
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alcock, J. (1998). Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach (6th ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., Lam, D., & Lancaster, J. B. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers I: Reports by Xhosa high school students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 433–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. B. (1999a). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers I: Reports from Albuquerque men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 405–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. B. (1999b). Paternity confidence and fitness outcomes: Abortion, divorce, and paternal investment. Paper presented at The Annual Meeting of the Human Behavior Evolution Society, June 2–6, Salt Lake City, UT.Google Scholar
Baker, R. R., & Bellis, M. A. (1995). Human sperm competition. Copulation, masturbation and infidelity. London, England: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R., & Lycett, J. (2002). Human evolutionary psychology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, C., & Schiefenhoevel, W. (1994, October). Adoption in Tauwema, Tobriand-Inseln. Poster presented at the Congress “Anthropologie Heute” of the Gesellschaft für Anthropologie. Humboldt-University at Berlin und University of Potsdam, Germany.Google Scholar
Birkhead, T. (2000). Promiscuity: An evolutionary history of sperm competition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bossong, B. (2001). Gender and age differences in inheritance patterns: Why men leave more to their spouses and women more to their children. An experimental analysis. Human Nature, 12, 107–122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brédart, S., & French, R. M. (1999). Do babies resemble their fathers more than their mothers? A failure to replicate Christenfeld and Hill (1995). Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 129–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressan, P. (2002). Why babies look like their daddies: Paternity uncertainty and the evolution of self-deception in evaluating family resemblance. Acta Ethologica, 4, 113–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressan, P., & Grassi, M. (2004). Parental resemblance in one-year-olds and the Gaussian curve. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 133–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (2003). Psychology and evolution: The origins of mind. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of mind (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.Google Scholar
Cartwright, J. (2000). Evolution and human behavior: Darwinian perspectives on human nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Christenfeld, N. J. S., & Hill, E. A. (1995). Whose baby are you? Nature, 378, 669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Vincent, A. J. C. (1991). Sexual selection and the potential reproductive rates of males and females. Nature, 351, 58–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, M., Salmon, C., & Wilson, M. (1997). Kinship: The conceptual hole in psychological studies of social cognition and close relationships. In Simpson, J. A. & Kenrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 265–296). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1980). Discriminative parental solicitude: A biological perspective. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 277–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1982). Whom are newborn babies said to resemble. Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 69–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The long reach of the gene. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeKay, W. T. (1995, July). Grandparent investment and the uncertainty of kinship. Paper presented at the Seventh Annual Meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, Santa Barbara, CA.
Duvall, E. M. (1954). In-laws: Pro & con. New York: Association Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1988). Grandchildrens' perspectives on relationships with grandparents: The influence of gender across generations. Sex Roles, 19, 205–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Euler, H. A., Hoier, S., & Rohde, P. A. (2001). Relationship-specific closeness of intergenerational family ties: Findings from evolutionary psychology and implications for models of cultural transmission. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 163–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Euler, H. A., & Weitzel, B. (1996). Discriminative grandparental solicitude as reproductive strategy. Human Nature, 7, 39–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Fischer, L. R. (1983). Transition to grandmotherhood. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 16, 67–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S., & Snyder, M. (1985). “To carve nature at its joints”: On the existence of discrete classes in personality. Psychological Review, 92, 317–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C., & McBurney, D. H. (2001). Psychology: An evolutionary approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.Google Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C., McBurney, D. H., & Brakeman-Wartell, S. L. (1997). Matrilateral biases in the investment of aunts and uncles. Human Nature, 8, 139–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaulin, S. J. C., & Schlegel, A. (1980). Paternal confidence and paternal investment: A cross-cultural test of a sociobiological hypothesis. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1, 301–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, T. S., & Manaster, G. L. (1982). The relationship with grandparents: Contact, importance, role conceptions. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 15, 233–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of another hypothesis about men's foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 29–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E. (1978/1979). Young adults' relations with their grandparents: An exploratory study. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 10, 299–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoier, S., Euler, H. A., & Hänze, M. (2000). Diskriminative verwandtschaftliche Fürsorge von Onkeln und Tanten: Eine evolutionspsychologische Analyse [Discriminative solicitude of aunts and uncles: An evolutionary analysis]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 22, 206–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, R. W., Spilka, B., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (1996). The psychology of religion: An empirical approach (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature: Natural selection and the female of the species. London, England: Chatto & Windus.Google Scholar
Judge, D. S. (1995). American legacies and the variable life histories of women and men. Human Nature, 6, 291–323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahana, B., & Kahana, E. (1970). Grandparenthood from the perspective of the developing grandchild. Developmental Psychology 3, 98–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, G. E. (1989). Middleborns' perception of family relationships. Psychological Reports, 64, 755–760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, G. E. (1990). College students' expectations of grandparent and grandchild role behaviors. The Gerontologist, 30, 43–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidwell, J. S. (1981). Number of siblings, sibling spacing, sex, and birth order: Their effects on perceived parent–adolescent relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 315–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Littlefield, C. H., & Rushton, J. P. (1986). When a child dies: The sociobiology of bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 797–802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlowe, F. (1999). Showoffs or providers? The parenting effort of Hadza men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 391–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, S. H., & Sprey, J. (1985). Adolescents' relationships with grandparents: An empirical contribution to conceptual clarification. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 621–626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBurney, D., Simon, J., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Geliebter, A. (2001). Matrilateral biases in the investment of aunts and uncles: Replication in a population presumed to have high paternity certainty. Human Nature, 13, 391–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLain, D. K., Setters, D., Moulton, M. P., & Pratt, A. E. (2000). Ascription of resemblance of newborns by parents and nonrelatives. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 11–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mealey, L. (2000). Sex differences: Development and evolutionary strategies. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Pashos, A. (2000). Does paternal uncertainty explain discriminative grandparental solicitude? A cross-cultural study in Greece and Germany. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 97–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Penguin Putnam, Inc.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., Ashbury, K., & Dunn, J. (2001). Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 46, 225–233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & Rutter, M. (1997). Behavioral genetics (3rd ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Porter, R. (1987). Kin recognition: Functions and mediating mechanisms. In Crawford, C. B., Smith, M. S., & Krebs, D. (Eds.), Sociobiology and psychology: Ideas, issues and applications (pp. 175–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Regalski, J. M., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (1993). Whom are Mexican infants said to resemble? Monitoring and fostering paternal confidence in the Yucatan. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 97–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2003). The nature of cultures. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rohde, P. A., Atzwanger, K., Butovskaya, M., Lampert, A., Mysterud, I., Sanchez-Andres, A., et al. (2003). Perceived parental favoritism, closeness to kin, and the rebel of the family: The effects of birth order and sex. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 261–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent–child relations across the life course. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Russell, R. J. H., & Wells, P. A. (1987). Estimating paternity confidence. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, C. A. (1999). On the impact of sex and birth order on contact with kin. Human Nature, 10, 183–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, C. A., & Daly, M. (1996). On the importance of kin relations to Canadian women and men. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 289–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, C. A., & Daly, M. (1998). Birth order and familial sentiment: Middleborns are different. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 299–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schönpflug, U. (2001). Intergenerational transmission of values: The role of transmission belts. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 174–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M. S. (1988). Research in developmental sociobiology: Parenting and family behavior. In MacDonald, K. B. (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp. 271–292). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 171–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbach, I., & Henke, W. (1998). Grosselterninvestment: Eine empirische interkulturelle Vergleichsstudie [Grandparental investment: An empirical cross-cultural comparative study]. Anthropologie, 36, 293–301.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. (1996). Born to rebel: Birth order, family dynamics, and creative lives. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. (2001). Birth order, sibling competition, and human behavior. In Holcomb, H. R. III (Ed.), Conceptual challenges in evolutionary psychology: Innovative research strategies (Vol. 27, pp. 39–83). Dodrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Szydlik, M. (1995). Die Enge der Beziehung zwischen erwachsenen Kindern und ihren Eltern – und umgekehrt [The closeness of relationship between adult children and their parents – and vice versa]. Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 24, 75–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Tesser, A. (1993). The importance of heritability in psychological research: The case of attitudes. Psychological Review, 93, 129–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871–1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×