Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T08:55:32.261Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Information is not enough

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2009

Caron Chess
Affiliation:
Rutgers University
Branden B. Johnson
Affiliation:
New Jersey Department of Environmental Protection
Susanne C. Moser
Affiliation:
National Center for Atmospheric Research, Boulder
Lisa Dilling
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Communication about climate change is as complex as the science. Among the most difficult communication challenges is persuading people to act on a problem such as climate change, which is not immediately relevant or easily solved. The challenge becomes even more difficult when you ask people to change routine behavior, such as how they get to work or set their home thermostat.

Not surprisingly, after years of schooling, many readers of this volume probably come to it believing that information changes attitudes and behaviors. Some may go so far as to think information is a cure. Filling eyes and ears with sufficient data may seem a means of cleansing the brain of misguided opinions and incorrect facts, and replacing them with “appropriate” beliefs. Many informational efforts to promote environmentally responsible behavior lean on an implicit theory of behavior (Costanzo et al., 1986) that “right” behavior naturally follows from “right” thinking.

However, more knowledge does not necessarily lead to more appropriate behavior, or even behavior deemed more appropriate by the educator or communicator (Johnson, 1993; Fischhoff, 1995). Social science theory and much empirical research show that links between information and behavior can be tenuous at best. Information is not entirely inconsequential, but it is much overrated as a change agent (see a discussion of the so-called “deficit model” in Sturgis and Allum, 2004; see also Tribbia, Chapter 15, this volume).

Advocates for precautionary action may find it difficult to accept the fragile link between information and behavior.

Type
Chapter
Information
Creating a Climate for Change
Communicating Climate Change and Facilitating Social Change
, pp. 223 - 234
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Alhakami, A. S. and Slovic, P. (1994). A psychological study of the inverse relationship between perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis, 14, 1085–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bator, R. J. and Cialdini, R. B. (2000). The application of persuasion theory to the development of effective proenvironmental public service announcements. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 527–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bord, R. J., Fisher, A., and O'Connor, R. E. (1997). Is accurate understanding of global warming necessary to promote willingness to sacrifice?Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 8, 339–54.Google Scholar
Bord, R. J., O'Connor, R. E., and Fisher, A. (2000). In what sense does the public need to understand global climate change?Public Understanding of Science, 9, 205–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostrom, A., Atman, C. J., Fischhoff, B., et al. (1994a). Evaluating risk communications: Completing and correcting mental models of hazardous processes, part II. Risk Analysis, 14, 789–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., et al. (1994b). What do people know about global climate change? 1. Mental models. Risk Analysis, 14, 959–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulkeley, H. (2000). Common knowledge? Public understanding of climate change in Newcastle, Australia. Public Understanding of Science, 9, 313–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. (2001). The science of persuasion. Scientific American, 284, 76–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clean Energy States Alliance (CESA) and Gardner Nelson and Partners (GNP) (2003). Message Development Focus Groups: Qualitative Analysis. New York, NY: CESA, GNP.
Costanzo, M., Archer, D., Aronson, E., and Pettigrew, T. (1986). Energy conservation behavior: the difficult path from information to action. American Psychologist, 41, 521–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, C. S. and McCann, J. M. (1978). Assessing communication effects on energy conservation. Journal of Consumer Research, 5, 82–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunlap, R. E. (1998). Lay perceptions of global risk: Public views of global warming in cross-national context. International Sociology, 13, 473–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1995). Risk perception and communication unplugged: Twenty years of process. Risk Analysis, 15, 137–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., and Lewis, F. M. (eds.) (2002). Health Behavior and Education: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3rd edn. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Gross, J. (2004). From guilt trip to hot wheels. The New York Times, June 13, ST-1, p. 14.Google Scholar
Halpern, D., Bates, C., Beales, G., and Heathfield, A. (2004). Personal Responsibility and Changing Behaviour: The State of Knowledge and Its Implications for Public Policy. London, UK: Cabinet Office, Prime Minister's Strategy Unit. Available at: http://www.pm.gov.uk/files/pdf/pr.pdf; accessed January 9, 2006.Google Scholar
Henry, A. D. (2000). Public perceptions of global warming. Human Ecology Review, 7, 25–30.Google Scholar
Johnson, B. B. (1993). Advancing understanding of knowledge's role in lay risk perception. Risk: Issues in Health & Safety, 4, 189–212.Google Scholar
Kempton, W. (1997). How the public views climate change. Environment, 39, 12–21.Google Scholar
Lacey, M. (2004). Genital cutting shows signs of losing favor in Africa. The New York Times, June 8, p. 3.Google Scholar
Leiserowitz, A. A. (2003). Affective Imagery and Risk Perceptions of Global Climate Change. Presentation given at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Risk Analysis, Baltimore, December 7–10.
Löfstedt, R. E. (1991). Climate change perceptions and energy-use decisions in northern Sweden. Global Environmental Change, 1, 321–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löfstedt, R. E. (1992). Lay perspectives concerning global climate change in Sweden. Energy and Environment, 3, 161–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löfstedt, R. E. (1993). Lay perspectives concerning global climate change in Vienna, Austria. Energy and Environment, 4, 140–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenzie-Mohr, D. (2000). Promoting sustainable behavior: An introduction to community based social marketing. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 543–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., and Atman, C. J. (2002). Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Prochaska, J. O. and Di Clemente, C. C. (1983). Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 390–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, D., Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., et al. (1994). What do people know about global climate change? 2. Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis, 14, 971–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reardon, K. E. (1991). Persuasion in Practice. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Rogers, E. M. (1962). Diffusion of Innovations. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Rowan, K. E. (1991). When simple language fails: Presenting difficult science to the public. Journal of Technical Writing and Communication, 21, 369–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: A field experiment of curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 25–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, P. W. and Tyra, A. (2000). A Field Study of Normative Beliefs and Environmental Behavior. Presentation given at the Western Psychological Association, Portland, OR, April.
Stamm, K. R., Clark, F., and Eblascas, P. R. (2000). Mass communication and public understanding of environmental problems: The case of global warming. Public Understanding of Science, 9, 219–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturgis, P. and Allum, N. (2004). Science in society: Re-evaluating the deficit model of public attitudes. Public Understanding of Science, 13, 55–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1988). The precaution adoption process. Health Psychology, 7, 355–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D. and Sandman, P. M. (1992). A model of the precaution adoption process: Evidence from home radon testing. Health Psychology, 11, 170–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D., Lyon, J. E., Sandman, P. M., et al. (1998a). Experimental evidence for stages of health behavior change: The precaution adoption process model applied to home radon testing. Health Psychology, 17, 445–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, N. D., Rothman, A. J., and Sutton, S. R. (1998b). Stage theories of health behavior: Conceptual and methodological issues. Health Psychology, 17, 290–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×