Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T15:44:23.660Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - From the Neck Up: Facial Gestures in Dialogue

from Part I - Gestural Types: Forms and Functions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2024

Alan Cienki
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

Gestures of the face have a relatively limited presence in scholarly gesture discourses. The use of facial movements as intentional communication has been historically undermined in facial behavior research. The face has been primarily studied as expressions of emotion, traditionally theorized as involuntary signs of internal affective states. Emotion expressions are differentiated from facial movements that serve conversational functions in face-to-face dialogue. The facial gestures presented in this chapter illustrate the flexibility and diversity of meanings conveyed by facial communicative actions. Gestures can refer to affective events not present in the immediate here and now, communicate understanding of another individual’s affective experience, and convey information about a target referent. Other facial gestures have counterparts in hand gestures with similar pragmatic and semantic functions. The study of facial gestural components of linguistic communicative events is important to the construction of a comprehensive model of language.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Antas, J., & Gembalczyk, S. (2017). The bodily expression of negation in Polish. Journal of Multimodal Communication Studies, 4(1–2), 1622. https://ruj.uj.edu.pl/xmlui/bitstream/handle/item/55971/antas_gembalczyk_the_bodily_expression_of_negation_in_polish_2017.pdfGoogle Scholar
Antzakas, K. (2006). The use of negative head movements in Greek Sign Language. In Zeshan, U. (Ed.), Interrogative and negative constructions in sign language (pp. 258269). Nijmegen, the Netherlands: Ishara Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 168. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100619832930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel.” Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2018). Some pragmatic functions of conversational facial gestures. Gesture, 17(1), 98127. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.00012.bavCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19(2), 163194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X00019002001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2000). Listeners as co-narrators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 941952. https://doi/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.941CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavelas, J. B., & Gerwing, J. (2007). Conversational hand gestures and facial displays in face-to-face dialogue. In Fiedler, K. (Ed.) Social communication (pp. 283308). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bavelas, J., Gerwing, J., & Healing, S. (2014a). Including facial gestures in gesture-speech ensembles. In Seyfeddinipur, M. & Gullberg, M. (Eds.), From gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance: Essays in honor of Adam Kendon (pp. 1534). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Bavelas, J., Gerwing, J., & Healing, S. (2014b). Effect of dialogue on demonstrations: Direct quotations, facial portrayals, hand gestures, and figurative references. Discourse Processes, 51(8), 619655. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.883730CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J., Gerwing, G., & Healing, S. (2014c). Hand gestures and facial displays in conversational interaction. In Holtgraves, T. M. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of language and social psychology (pp. 111130). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Blyth, C., Recktenwald, S., & Wang, J. (1990) “I’m like, ‘Say what?!’”: A new quotative in American oral narrative. American Speech, 65(3), 215227. https://doi.org/10.2307/455910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D.L. (1946). Thoughts on “Yep” and “Nope”. American Speech, 21(2), 9095. https://doi.org/10.2307/486479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394402. https://doi/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chovil, N. (1989). Communicative functions of facial displays in conversation (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Victoria, Canada.Google Scholar
Chovil, N. (1991a). Discourse-oriented facial displays in conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 25(1–4), 163194. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351819109389361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chovil, N. (1991b). Social determinants of facial displays. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15(3), 141154. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01672216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chu, M., Meyer, A., Foulkes, L., & Kita, S. (2014). Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 694709. https://doi.org/10.1037/a00338CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Tree, J. E. (2002). Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition, 84(1), 73111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00017-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K., Abner, N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018). The palm-up puzzle: Meanings and origins of a widespread form in gesture and sign. Frontiers in Communication, 3, Article 23. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dachkovsky, S., & Sandler, W. (2009). Visual intonation in the prosody of a sign language. Language and Speech, 52(2–3), 287314. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830909103175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Debras, C. (2017). The shrug: Forms and meanings of a compound enactment. Gesture, 16(1), 134. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.16.1.01debCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S. D. (2002). Gesture, verb aspect, and the nature of iconic imagery in natural discourse. Gesture, 2(2), 183206. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.2.2.04dunCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duran, J. I., Reisenzein, R., & Fernandez-Dols, J. M. (2017). Coherence between emotions and facial expressions: A research synthesis. In Fernandez-Dols, J. M. & Russell, J. A., (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 107132). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1976). Movements with precise meanings. Journal of Communication, 26(3), 1426. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1976.tb01898.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1979). About brows: Emotional and conversational signals. In Aschoof, J., von Cranach, M., Foppa, K., Lepenies, W., & Ploog, D. (Eds.), Human ethology (pp. 169202). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies. New York, NY: Berkley Books.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1997). Should we call it expression or communication? Innovation: The European Journal of Social Science Research, 10(4), 333344. https://doi.org/10.1080/13511610.1997.9968538Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (2004). Emotional and conversational nonverbal signals. In Larrazabal, J. M. & Perez Miranda, L. A. (Eds.), Language, knowledge, and representation (pp. 3950). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124129. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1977). Nonverbal behavior. In Ostwald, P. (Ed.), Communication and social interaction (pp. 3746). London, UK: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 4998. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1969.1.1.49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K. (1999). Do signers gesture? In Messing, L. and Campbell, R. (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 133159). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B. A., & Robles, J. (2010). It’s like mmm: Enactments with it’s like. Discourse Studies, 12(6), 715738. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445610381862CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J., & Gilbert, A. N. (1985). Emotions and facial expression. Science, 230, 607608. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.4048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fussell, S. R., & Moss, M. M. (1998). Figurative language in emotional communication. In Fussell, S. R. & Kreuz, R. J. (Eds.), Social and cognitive approaches to interpersonal communication (pp. 113141). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in making meaning of facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 211219. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417746794CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddard, C. (2014). Interjections and emotion (with special reference to “surprise” and “disgust”). Emotion Review, 6(1), 5363. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913491843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H, & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62, 5175. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1986.62.1-2.51Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9(1), 107131. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133369700900105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jehoul, A., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K. (2017). The shrug as marker of obviousness. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-0082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1917). Negation in English and other languages. Copenhagen, Denmark: Det Kgl. Danske Videnskabernes Selskap.Google Scholar
Jones, S. S., & Hong, H. W. (2001). Onset of voluntary communication: Smiling looks to mother. Infancy, 2(3), 353370. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0203_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. W. (1991). An audience effect on smile production in 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 2(1), 4549. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00095.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1988) How gestures can become like words. In Poyatos, F. (Ed.), Crosscultural perspectives in nonverbal communication (pp. 131141). Toronto, Canada: C. J. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H. (2014). Recurrent gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 15581575). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Liddell, S. K. (1980). American Sign Language syntax. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., & Fridlund, A. (1991). Analyzing nonverbal behavior. In Dorwick, P. (Ed.) Practical guide to using video in the behavioral sciences (pp. 153165). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Meir, I. (2004). Question and negation in Israeli Sign Language. Sign Language & Linguistics, 7(2), 97124. https://doi.org/10.1075/sll.7.2.03meiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D. (1994). Bodytalk: A world guide to gestures. London, UK: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Studtmann, M., & Horstmann, G. (2013). Coherence between emotion and facial expression: Evidence from laboratory experiments. Emotion Review, 5(1), 1623. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912457228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimé, B. (1995). The social sharing of emotion as a source for the social knowledge of emotion. In Russell, J. A., Fernandez-Dols, J. M., Manstead, A. S. R., & Wellenkamp, J. C. (Eds.), Everyday conceptions of emotion (pp. 475489). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1(1), 6085. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908097189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 2341. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., & Ebert, R. (1994). Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 870871. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, W. (2003). On the complementarity of signed and spoken languages. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of SLI (pp. 383409). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sandler, W. (2009). Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica, 174, 241275. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.2009.035Google Scholar
Sandler, W. (2018). The body as evidence for the nature of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1782. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01782CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stec, K., Huiskes, M., & Redeker, G. (2016). Multimodal quotation: Role shift practices in spoken narratives. Journal of Pragmatics, 104, 117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2016.07.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, J. E. (1977). Facial expressions of the neonate infant indicating the hedonics of food-related chemical stimuli. In Weiffenbach, J. M. (Ed.), Taste and development: the genesis of sweet preference (pp 173188). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. (2002). Grammars, words, and embodied meanings: On the uses and evolution of so and like. Journal of Communication, 52(3), 581596. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02563.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton-Spence, R., & Napoli, D. J. (2010). Anthropomorphism in sign languages: A look at poetry and storytelling with a focus on British Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 10(4), 442475. https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.0.0055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waksler, R. (2001). A new all in conversation. American Speech, 76(2), 128138. https://www.muse.jhu.edu/article/2817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 12301240. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130486CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiggins, S. (2014). Family mealtimes, yuckiness and the socialization of disgust responses by preschool children. In Szatrowski, P. E. (Ed.), Language and food: Verbal and nonverbal experiences (pp. 211231). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×