Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T12:55:08.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Emotional Convergence

A Case of Contagion?

from Part IV - Understanding Others

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2016

Sukhvinder S. Obhi
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Ontario
Emily S. Cross
Affiliation:
Bangor University
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Because they are likely to trigger convergent emotional reactions in observers, it has become extremely common to refer to emotions as ‘contagious’ elements. While it could have simply remained a metaphor, the concept of contagion has become central to account for situations of emotional convergence between individuals. In this respect, the primitive emotional contagion model introduced by Hatfield and colleagues in 1994 assumes that we have a strong tendency to mimic the facial, postural and vocal motor behaviour of the people we interact with. This claim resembles the shared representations framework, according to which we make use of our own motor resources to access and share others’ states. This chapter consists of a critical review of the primitive emotional contagion model and further discusses the legitimacy of considering emotions as contagious elements to describe situations of affective or emotional convergence.

Type
Chapter
Information
Shared Representations
Sensorimotor Foundations of Social Life
, pp. 417 - 436
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R. B. Jr, & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14(6), 644647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5(1), 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 4261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahs, F., Pissiota, A., Michelgard, A., Frans, O., Furmark, T., et al. (2009). Disentangling the web of fear: Amygdala reactivity and functional connectivity in spider and snake phobia. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 172, 103108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronfreed, J. (1970). The socialization of altruistic and sympathetic behavior: Some theoretical and experimental analyses. In J. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and Helping Behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 103126.Google Scholar
Balconi, M., & Bortolotti, A. (2013). Emotional face recognition, empathic trait (BEES), and cortical contribution in response to positive and negative cues. The effect of rTMS on dorsal medial prefrontal cortex. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 7(1), 1321. doi: 10.1007/s11571-012-9210-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1971-10097-000.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Williams, E. L. (2006). The automaticity of social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(1), 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartal, I. B.-A., Decety, J., & Mason, P. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science, 334(6061), 14271430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, T., Matthias, W., & Lutz, J. (2007). Modulation of corticospinal activity by strong emotions evoked by pictures and classical music: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. NeuroReport, 18, 261265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher–student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12(2), 120138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior, 401.Google Scholar
Brown, R. W. (1954). Mass phenomena. Handbook of Social Psychology, 2, 833876.Google Scholar
Bush, L. K., Barr, C. L., McHugo, G. J., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1989). The effects of facial control and facial mimicry on subjective reactions to comedy routines. Motivation and Emotion, 13(1), 3152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Cole, J., Campbell, R., & Young, A. W. (2000). Facial expression recognition by people with Möbius syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1–3), 7387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cappella, J. N. (1981). Mutual influence in expressive behavior: Adult–adult and infant–adult dyadic interaction. Psychological Bulletin, 89(1), 101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cappella, J. N. (1997). Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 100, 5497–502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chovil, N. (1991). Social determinants of facial displays. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15(3), 141154. doi: 10.1007/BF01672216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chovil, N. (1997). Facing others: A social communicative perspective on facial displays. Psychology of Facial Expression, 25, 321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coelho, C. M., Lipp, O. V., Marinovic, W., Wallis, G., & Riek, S. (2010). Increased corticospinal excitability induced by unpleasant visual stimuli. Neuroscience Letters, 481, 135138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coombes, S. A., Tandonnet, C., Fujiyama, H., Janelle, C. M., Cauraugh, J. H., & Summers, J. J. (2009). Emotion and motor preparation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of corticospinal motor tract excitability. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 380388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conty, L., Dezecache, G., Hugueville, L., & Grèzes, J. (2012). Early binding of gaze, gesture, and emotion: Neural time course and correlates. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(13), 45314539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coviello, L., Sohn, Y., Kramer, A. D. I., Marlow, C., Franceschetti, M., et al. (2014). Detecting emotional contagion in massive social networks. PLoS One, 9(3), e90315. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristinzio, C., N’Diaye, K., Seeck, M., Vuilleumier, P., & Sander, D. (2010). Integration of gaze direction and facial expression in patients with unilateral amygdala damage. Brain, 133(Pt 1), 248261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 577596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(2), 5458. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derks, D., Fischer, A. H., & Bos, A. E. (2008). The role of emotion in computer-mediated communication: A review. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(3), 766785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dezecache, G., Conty, L., Chadwick, M., Philip, L., Soussignan, R., et al. (2013a). Evidence for unintentional emotional contagion beyond dyads. PLoS One, 8(6), e67371. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dezecache, G., Jacob, P., & Grèzes, J. (2015). Emotional contagion: its scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(6), 297299. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.011.Google Scholar
Dezecache, G., Mercier, H., & Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2013b). An evolutionary approach to emotional communication. Journal of Pragmatics, 59, 221233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 19(6), 643647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimberg, U., Hansson, G. Ö., & Thunberg, M. (1998). Fear of snakes and facial reactions: A case of rapid emotional responding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 7580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimberg, U., & Thunberg, M. (1998). Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 3945. doi: 10.1111/1467–9450.00054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11(1), 8689. doi: 10.1111/1467–9280.00221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & Van Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2008). Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. British Medical Journal, 337. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view (Vol. 38). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gallup, A. C., Hale, J. J., Sumpter, D. J. T., Garnier, S., Kacelnik, A., et al. (2012). Visual attention and the acquisition of information in human crowds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(19), 72457250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1116141109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelder, B. de. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(3), 242249. doi: 10.1038/nrn1872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelder, B. de, Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: A mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(47), 1670116706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Adenis, M. S., Pouga, L., & Armony, J. L. (2012). Self-relevance modulates brain responses to angry body expressions. Cortex, 49(8), 22102220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., & Dezecache, G. (2014). How do shared-representations and emotional processes cooperate in response to social threat signals? Neuropsychologia, 55, 105114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Philip, L., Chadwick, M., Dezecache, G., Soussignan, R., & Conty, L. (2013). Self-relevance appraisal influences facial reactions to emotional body expressions. PLoS One, 8(2), e55885. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. NeuroImage, 35(2), 959967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Wicker, B., Berthoz, S., & de Gelder, B. (2009). A failure to grasp the affective meaning of actions in autism spectrum disorder subjects. Neuropsychologia, 47, 18161825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosbras, M. H., & Paus, T. (2006). Brain networks involved in viewing angry hands or faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 10871096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadjikhani, N., Hoge, R., Snyder, J., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Pointing with the eyes: The role of gaze in communicating danger. Brain and Cognition, 68, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., & Hsee, C. K. (1995). The impact of vocal feedback on emotional experience and expression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 293313.Google Scholar
Hennenlotter, A., Dresel, C., Castrop, F., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O., Wohlschläger, A. M., & Haslinger, B. (2009). The link between facial feedback and neural activity within central circuitries of emotion: New insights from Botulinum toxin-induced denervation of frown muscles. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 537542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., Adams, R., & Kleck, R. (2007). Looking at you or looking elsewhere: The influence of head orientation on the signal value of emotional facial expressions. Motivation & Emotion, 31(2), 137144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review. doi: 10.1177/1088868312472607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hietanen, J. K., Surakka, V., & Linnankoski, I. (1998). Facial electromyographic responses to vocal affect expressions. Psychophysiology, 35(5), 530536. doi: 10.1017/S0048577298970445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, A. L., Rand, D. G., Nowak, M. A., & Christakis, N. A. (2010). Emotions as infectious diseases in a large social network: The SISa model. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, K. L., Gothard, K. M., Schmid, M. C., & Logothetis, N. K. (2007). Facial-expression and gaze-selective responses in the monkey amygdala. Current Biology, 17(9), 766772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., & Chemtob, C. (1992). Assessments of the emotional states of others: Conscious judgments versus emotional contagion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11(2), 119128. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1992.11.2.119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., et al. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 1045610459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilner, J., Friston, K., & Frith, C. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system, Cognitive Processes, 8(3), 159166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, J. D. (1984). The real role of facial response in the experience of emotion: A reply to Tourangeau and Ellsworth, and others. http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/47/4/909/.Google Scholar
Langford, D. J., Crager, S. E., Shehzad, Z., Smith, S. B., Sotocinal, S. G., et al. (2006). Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science, 312(5782), 19671970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanzetta, J. T., & Orr, S. P. (1980). Influence of facial expressions on the classical conditioning of fear. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Bon, G. (1896). Psychologie des foules. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, D. H., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Social transmission of the sensory benefits of eye widening in fear expressions. Psychological Science. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Likowski, K. U., Mühlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 10651072. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnée, M. J. C. M., Stekelenburg, J. J., Kemner, C., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Neuroreport, 18(4), 369372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R. E., Banks, J. H. Jr, & Ogawa, N. (1963). Role of facial expression in ‘cooperative-avoidance conditioning’ in monkeys. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(1), 24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moody, E. J., McIntosh, D. N., Mann, L. J., & Weisser, K. R. (2007). More than mere mimicry? The influence of emotion on rapid facial reactions to faces. Emotion, 7(2), 447457. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Strey, H. H., Frederick, B., Savoy, R., Cox, D., et al. (2009). Chemosensory cues to conspecific emotional stress activate amygdala in humans. PLoS One, 4(7), e6415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
N’Diaye, K., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: Gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion, 9(6), 798806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicol, J. R., Perrotta, S., Caliciuri, S., & Wachowiak, M. P. (2013). Emotion-specific modulation of early visual perception. Cognition & Emotion, 27(8), 14781485. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.793654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). Face to face: Blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 167178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveri, M., Babiloni, C., Filippi, M. M., Caltagirone, C., Babiloni, F., et al. (2003). Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues. Experimental Brain Research, 149, 214221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158. doi: 10.1038/nrn2317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48(2), 175187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychological Science, 17(4), 292299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., & Grèzes, J. (2008). Emotional modulation of visual and motor areas by dynamic body expressions of anger. Social Neuroscience, 3, 199212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., (2009). Two different faces of threat: Comparing the neural systems for recognizing fear and anger in dynamic body expressions. NeuroImage, 47, 18731883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., (2012). Threat prompts defensive brain responses independently of attentional control. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 274285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pouga, L., Berthoz, S., de Gelder, B., & Grèzes, J. (2010). Individual differences in socioaffective skills influence the neural bases of fear processing: The case of alexithymia. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 14691481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prehn-Kristensen, A., Wiesner, C., Bergmann, T. O., Wolff, S., Jansen, O., et al. (2009). Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety. PLoS One, 4(6), e5987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provine, R. R. (2001). Laughter: A scientific investigation. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2005). Yawning: The yawn is primal, unstoppable and contagious, revealing the evolutionary and neural basis of empathy and unconscious behavior. American Scientist, 93(6), 532539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qin, S., Young, C. B., Supekar, K., Uddin, L. Q., & Menon, V. (2012). Immature integration and segregation of emotion-related brain circuitry in young children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 79417946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, E., De Haans, S., & Denys, D. (2013). Social affordances in context: What is it that we are bodily responsive to? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 436.Google Scholar
Roy, A. K., Shehzad, Z., Margulies, D. S., Kelly, A. M. C., Uddin, L. Q., et al. (2009). Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 45, 614626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Yoshikawa, S., Naito, E., & Matsumura, M. (2004). Enhanced neural activity in response to dynamic facial expressions of emotion: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 20, 8191.Google ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S., & Laird, J. D. (2007). Facing fear: Expression of fear facilitates processing of emotional information. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 35(4), 513–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutter, D. J. L. G., Hofman, D., & van Honk, J. (2008). Fearful faces selectively increase corticospinal motor tract excitability: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Psychophysiology, 45, 345348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sighele, S. (1901). La foule criminelle: Essai de psychologie collective. F. Alcan.Google Scholar
Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5(1), 136150. doi: 10.1037/h0031066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A. (1759 [2010]). The theory of moral sentiments. London: Penguin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and autonomic reactivity: A test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Emotion, 2(1), 52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soussignan, R., Chadwick, M., Philip, L., Conty, L., Dezecache, G., & Grèzes, J. (2013). Self-relevance appraisal of gaze direction and dynamic facial expressions: Effects on facial electromyographic and autonomic reactions. Emotion, 13(2), 330337. doi: 10.1037/a0029892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2010). Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions. Social Neuroscience, 4, 401416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 211220. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11(7), 843850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Geminiani, G., et al. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(42), 1766117666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarde, G. (1890). Les lois de l’imitation: étude sociologique. Félix Alcan.Google Scholar
Uchino, B., Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., Carlson, J. G., & Chemtob, C. (1991). The effect of expectations on susceptibility to emotional contagion. Unpublished manuscript, University of Hawaii, Hawaii. www2.hawaii.edu/~elaineh/83.pdf.Google Scholar
Van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Sorger, B., Pichon, S., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2011). Cortico-subcortical visual, somatosensory, and motor activations for perceiving dynamic whole-body emotional expressions with and without striate cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 1618816193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeulen, N., Godefroid, J., & Mermillod, M. (2009). Emotional modulation of attention: Fear increases but disgust reduces the attentional blink. PLoS One, 4(11), e7924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vignemont, F. de, & Jacob, P. (2012). What is it like to feel another’s pain? Philosophy of Science, 79(2), 295316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voon, V., Brezing, C., Gallea, C., Ameli, R., Roelofs, K., et al. (2010). Emotional stimuli and motor conversion disorder. Brain, 133, 15261536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 12711278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, J. E., Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Wiland, J., Dresner, M. A., et al. (2006). Positive emotions preferentially engage an auditory-motor mirror system. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1306713075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., Wright, C. I., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion, 1, 7083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×