Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:45:02.322Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Situated theory as a bridge between psychology, history, and educational practices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2011

Robert Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
Laura Martin
Affiliation:
Arizona Museum of Science and Technology
Katherine Nelson
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

The context of theoretical explanation in developmental psychology

The explanations offered in psychology for behavior and experience arise from a cultural activity. The adequacy of a theory's analytic categories is determined partly by the inherent structure of the phenomena we as theorists seek to explain – by the degree to which it succeeds in cutting the objective world “at its joints” (Boyd 1979) – and partly by the perspective that it adopts on a set of human concerns. The perspective originates from a position that is both socially defined and situationally motivated. The purpose for which we are attempting to analyze or to “cut up” the phenomena constitutes the situational motivation, and this in turn can only be understood with reference to the social relations between the author, her subject, and her audience – the context in which this purpose is formulated (Serpell 1990).

The inspiration for this chapter came from two papers by Sylvia Scribner (1976, 1985). The first is a methodological plea for distinguishing between issues of ecological validity and issues of experimental control. The second is an exploration of the role of history in explanations of cognitive development. I believe that the significance of her earlier methodological argument can only be fully appreciated if it is complemented with a sociopolitical insight implicit in her later theoretical analysis.

In the elegant paper “Situating the experiment in cross-cultural research” Scribner (1976) argues that the scientific advantages of experimental control do not carry with them a necessary implication of artificiality and lack of ecological validity. The term situated in the title of that paper carries two distinct but related connotations for me.

Type
Chapter
Information
Sociocultural Psychology
Theory and Practice of Doing and Knowing
, pp. 21 - 42
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×