Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T01:03:38.474Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 January 2019

Tom Cochrane
Affiliation:
Flinders University of South Australia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Emotional Mind
A Control Theory of Affective States
, pp. 216 - 230
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41(1), 4967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, A. (2011). Attention and integration. In Mole, C., Smithies, D. & Wu, W. (eds.), Attention: Philosophical and Psychological Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2459.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(4), 245266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aristotle, (350 BCE/2000). Nicomachean Ethics. (Crisp, R., Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashton, M. C. & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 150166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aydede, M. (2014). How to unify theories of sensory pleasure: An adverbialist proposal. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 5(1), 119133.Google Scholar
Badre, D. (2013). Hierarchical cognitive control and the functional organization of the frontal cortex. In Ochsner, K. N. & Kosslyn, S. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience. Vol. 2: The Cutting Edges. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 300317.Google Scholar
Bain, D. (2013). What makes pains unpleasant? Philosophical Studies, 166(1), 6989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlassina, L. & Gordon, R. M. (2017). Folk psychology as mental simulation. In E. N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2017 Edition). Retrieved from: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/folkpsych-simulation/Google Scholar
Barlassina, L. & Newen, A. (2013). The role of bodily perception in emotion: In defense of an impure somatic theory. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 89(3), 637678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 2858.Google Scholar
Bastian, B., Jetten, J. & Hornsey, M. J. (2014). Gustatory pleasure and pain: The offset of acute physical pain enhances responsiveness to taste. Appetite, 72, 150155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H. & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K., Robinson, T. E. & Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 9, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthier, M., Starkstein, S. & Leiguarda, R. (1988). Asymbolia for pain: A sensory-limbic disconnection syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 24(1), 4149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billon, A. & Kriegel, U. (2015). Jaspers’ dilemma: The psychopathological challenge to subjectivity theories of consciousness. In Gennaro, R. (ed.), Disturbed Consciousness: New Essays on Psychopathology and Theories of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackburn, S. (2005). Success semantics. In Lillehammer, H. & Mellor, D. H. (eds.), Ramsey’s Legacy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Block, J. (2010). The five-factor framing of personality and beyond: Some ruminations. Psychological Inquiry, 21(1), 225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and Loss. Vol. 1: Attachment (2nd edn.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bradley, F. H. (1893). Appearance and Reality: A Metaphysical Essay. New York: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.Google Scholar
Brady, M. S. (2013). Emotional Insight: The Epistemic Role of Emotional Experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramble, B. (2013). The distinctive feeling theory of pleasure. Philosophical Studies, 162(2), 201217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bratman, M. E. (1993). Shared intention. Ethics, 104(1), 97113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brentano, F. (2014/1874). Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broad, C. D. (1954). Emotion and sentiment. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 13(2), 203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47(1–3), 139159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruder, M., Fischer, A. & Manstead, A. S. R. (2014). Social appraisal as a cause of collective emotions. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 141155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burge, T. (1979). Individualism and the mental. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 4(1), 73121.Google Scholar
Calì, G., Ambrosini, E., Picconi, L., Mehling, W. E. & Committeri, G. (2015). Investigating the relationship between interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive awareness, and emotional susceptibility. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. (1997). Interpreting the Personal: Expression and the Formation of Feelings. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 105110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2008). Expression and extended cognition. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 66(4), 329340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2009). Eight dimensions for the emotions. Social Science Information, 48(3), 379420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2010). A simulation theory of musical expressivity. The Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 88(2), 191207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2011). Using the persona to express complex emotions in music. Music Analysis, 29(3), 264275.Google Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2014). Narrative and character formation. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 72(3), 303315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. (2017). The double intentionality of emotional experience. European Journal of Philosophy, 25(4), 14541475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, T. & Rosset, O. (2012). Expressive dimensions in music. In Barthet, M. & Dixon, S. (eds.), Proceedings of the 9th International Symposium on Computer Music Modeling and Retrieval. London: Springer, 2028.Google Scholar
Cole, J. (1995). Pride and the Daily Marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2008). The somatic marker hypotheses, and what the Iowa Gambling Task does and does not show. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 59(1), 5171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2014). The Feeling Body: Affective Science Meets the Enactive Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G. & Krueger, J. (2015). Scaffoldings of the affective mind. Philosophical Psychology, 28(8), 11571176.Google Scholar
Colombetti, G. & Roberts, T. (2015). Extending the extended mind: The case for extended affectivity. Philosophical Studies, 172(5), 12431263.Google Scholar
Currie, G. & Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative Minds: Imagination in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Random House.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2000). The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M. & Batson, C. D. (1973). ‘From Jerusalem to Jericho: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(1), 100108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Arms, J. & Jacobson, D. (2000). The moralistic fallacy: On the ‘appropriateness’ of emotions. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 61(1), 6590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (3rd edn.). Ekman, P. (ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1987). Knowing one’s own mind. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, 60, 441458.Google Scholar
Davies, S. (2005). Artistic expression and the hard case of pure music. In Kieran, M. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Deigh, J. (2009). Concepts of emotions in modern philosophy and psychology. In Goldie, P. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1740.Google Scholar
De Sousa, R. (1987). The Rationality of Emotions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deonna, J. A. & Teroni, F. (2009). Taking affective explanations to heart. Social Science Information, 48(3), 359377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deonna, J. A. & Teroni, F. (2012). The Emotions: A Philosophical Introduction. Oxford: Routledge.Google Scholar
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. The Psychological Review, 3(4), 357370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1934/1980). Art as Experience. New York: Perigee Books.Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 12461256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Döring, S. (2007). Seeing what to do: Affective perception and rational motivation. Dialectica, 61(3), 363394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doris, J. (2002). Lack of Character: Personality and Moral Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. (1981). Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. (1995). Naturalizing the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1912/1995). The Elementary Forms of Religious Life. (Fields, K. E., Trans.). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Eickers, G., Loaiza, J. R. & Prinz, J. (2017). Embodiment, context-sensitivity, and discrete emotions: A response to Moors. Psychological Inquiry, 28(1), 3138.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W. & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221(4616), 12081210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, D. (2002). The search hypothesis of emotion. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 53(4), 497509.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454459.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In Pervin, L. A. (ed.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford, 244276.Google Scholar
Feldman, F. (2004). Pleasure and the Good Life. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J. M. & Crivelli, C. (2013). Emotion and expression: Naturalistic studies. Emotion Review, 5(1), 2429.Google Scholar
Field, T., Diego, M. & Hernandez-Reif, M. (2009). Infants of depressed mothers are less responsive to faces and voices: A review. Infant Behavior & Development, 32, 239244.Google Scholar
Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., … Kainbacher, M. (2011). The bystander-effect: A meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 517537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleeson, W., Furr, R. M., Jayawickreme, E., Helzer, E. G., Hartley, A. G. & Meindl, P. (2015). Personality science and the foundations of character. In Miller, C. B., Furr, R. M., Knobel, A. & Fleeson, W. (eds.), Character: New Directions from Philosophy, Psychology, and Theology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 4171.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. New York: Thomas Crowell.Google Scholar
Foot, P. (1972). Morality as a system of hypothetical imperatives. The Philosophical Review, 81(3), 305316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human Facial Expression: An Evolutionary View. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. & Duchaine, B. (1996). ‘Facial expressions of emotion’ and the delusion of the hermetic self. In Harré, R. & Parrott, W. G. (eds.), The Emotions: Social, Cultural and Biological Dimensions. London: Sage Publications, 259284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural Differences in Facial Expressions in a Social Situation: An Experimental Test on the Concept of Display Rules [Doctoral dissertation]. San Francisco: University of California.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2007). The Laws of Emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2016). The evolutionary emergence of what we call “emotions”. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 609620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijda, N. H. & Sundararajan, L. (2007). Emotion refinement: A theory inspired by Chinese poetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 227241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froese, T. & Di Paolo, E. A. (2011). The enactive approach: Theoretical sketches from cell to society. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19(1), 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, R. G. & Sheehy-Skeffington, A. (1974). Effects of group laughter on responses to humourous material: A replication and extension. Psychological Reports, 35(1), 531534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M. & Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 911917.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The bodily self as power for action. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 746755.Google Scholar
García-Carpintero, M. (2015). De se thought. Oxford Handbooks Online. Retrieved from: www.oxfordhandbooks.comGoogle Scholar
Gerrans, P. (2014). The Measure of Madness: Philosophy of Mind, Cognitive Neuroscience, and Delusional Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerrans, P. (2015). All the self we need. In Metzinger, T. & Windt, J. M. (eds.). Open MIND: 15(T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group, 119.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Human Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (1989). On Social Facts. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (2000). Sociality and Responsibility: New Essays in Plural Subject Theory. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (2014). How we feel: Understanding everyday collective emotion ascription. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1731.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2000a). The Emotions: A Philosophical Exploration. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2000b). Explaining expressions of emotion. Mind, 109(433), 2538.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2004). On Personality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2012). The Mess Inside: Narrative, Emotion, and the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2011). Two routes to empathy: Insights from cognitive neuroscience. In Coplan, & Goldie, (eds.), Empathy: philosophical and psychological perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grahek, N. (2007). Feeling Pain and Being in Pain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, R. (2014). Pain, perception and the sensory modalities: Revisiting the intensive theory. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 5(1), 87101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, M. S. (2007). Self-Expression. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (1997). What Emotions Really Are. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 126.Google Scholar
Gunther, Y. H. (2004). The phenomenology and intentionality of emotion. Philosophical Studies, 117(1), 4355.Google Scholar
Hall, R. J. (2008). If it itches, scratch! Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 86(4), 525535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T. & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional Contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatzimoysis, A. (2003). Emotional feelings and intentionalism. In Hayzimoysis, A. (ed.), Philosophy and the Emotions: Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 52. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 105111.Google Scholar
Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511.Google Scholar
Heathwood, C. (2007). The reduction of sensory pleasure to desire. Philosophical Studies, 133(1), 2344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helm, B. W. (2001). Emotional Reason: Deliberation, Motivation, and the Nature of Value. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Helm, B. W. (2014). Emotional communities of respect. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 4760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O. & Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: An EEG study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65(3), 214227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, E. A. & Mathews, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: A special relationship? Emotion, 5(4), 489497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, E. A. & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(3), 349362.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R. (2005). The speaking image: Visual communication and the nature of depiction. In Kieran, M. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art. Oxford: Blackwell, 145159.Google Scholar
Howard, S. A. (2012). Lyrical emotions and sentimentality. The Philosophical Quarterly, 62(248), 546568.Google Scholar
Hufendiek, R. (2016). Embodied Emotions: A Naturalist Approach to a Normative Phenomenon. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1739/1978). A Treatise of Human Nature. Selby-Bigge, L. A. & Nidditch, P. H. (eds.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huntsinger, J. R. (2013). Does emotion directly tune the scope of attention? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 265270.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. (2008). The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(1), 122.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds Without Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The Face of Emotion. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Jäger, C. & Bänninger-Huber, E. (2015). Looking into meta-emotions. Synthese, 192(3), 787811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9(34), 188205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology (2 vols.). New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Jones, K. (2008). How to change the past. In Mackenzie, C. & Atkins, K. (eds.), Practical Identity and Narrative Agency. London: Routledge, 269288.Google Scholar
Kamath, V., Moberg, P. J., Kohler, C. G., Gur, R. E. & Turetsky, B. I. (2011). Odor hedonic capacity and anhedonia in schizophrenia and unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(1), 5967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, I. (1785/1993). Grounding for the Metaphysics of Morals. (Ellington, J. W., Trans.). Cambridge: Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1788/2002). Critique of Practical Reason. (Pluhar, W. S., Trans.). Cambridge: Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. R. (2011). Yuck! The Nature and Moral Significance of Disgust. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kenny, A. (1963). Action, Emotion and Will. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Kever, A., Pollatos, O., Vermeulen, N. & Grynberg, D. (2015). Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 2023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivy, P. (1999). Feeling the musical emotions. British Journal of Aesthetics, 38(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, C. 2007. An imperative theory of pain. Journal of Philosophy, 104(10), 517522.Google Scholar
Klein, C. (2015a). What the Body Commands: The Imperative Theory of Pain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, C. (2015b). What pain asymbolia really shows. Mind, 124(494), 493516.Google Scholar
Klein, C. & Martínez, M. (forthcoming). Imperativism and degrees of pain. In D. Bain, M. Brady & J. Corns. (eds.), The Nature of Pain. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Koelsch, S., Jacobs, A. M., Menninghaus, W., Liebal, K., Klann-Delius, G., von Scheve, C. & Gebauer, G. (2015). The quartet theory of human emotions: An integrative and neurofunctional model. Physics of Life Reviews, 13, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolodny, N. (2003). Love as valuing a relationship. The Philosophical Review, 112(2), 135189.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 333347.Google Scholar
Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394421.Google Scholar
Kristjánsson, K. (2003). On the very idea of “negative emotions”. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 33(4), 351364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kristjánsson, K. (2010). The Self and Its Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Krueger, J. (2014). Dewey’s rejection of the emotion/expression distinction. In Solymosi, T. & Shook, J. R. (eds.), Neuroscience, Neurophilosophy and Pragmatism. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 140161.Google Scholar
Kupperman, J. (1995). Character. London: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacewing, M. (2007). Do unconscious emotions involve unconscious feelings? Philosophical Psychology, 20(1), 81104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laird, J. (2007). Feelings: The Perception of Self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lamm, C. & Silani, G. (2014). Insights into collective emotions from the social neuroscience of empathy. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 6377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laurence, S. & Margolis, E. (1999). Concepts and cognitive science. In Margolis, E. & Laurence, S. (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books, 381.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39(2), 124129.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Levinson, J. (2005). Musical expressiveness as hearability-as-expression. In Kieran, M. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art. Oxford: Blackwell, 192206.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 169194.Google Scholar
Loeser, J. D. & Melzack, R. (1999). Pain: An overview. The Lancet, 353(9164), 16071609.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. (1984). After Virtue (2nd edn.). Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Magai, C. & Haviland-Jones, J. (2002). The Hidden Genius of Emotion: Lifespan Transformations of Personality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. (2013). Fears and Phobias. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Martin, M. G. F. (2010). Getting on top of oneself: Comments on self-expression. Acta Analytica, 25(1), 8188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez, M. (2011). Imperative content and the painfulness of pain. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 6790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez, M. (2014). Pains as reasons. Philosophical Studies, 172(9), 22612274.Google Scholar
Mathews, K. E. & Canon, L. K. (1975). Environmental noise level as a determinant of helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(4), 571577.Google Scholar
McCormick, M. (2016). Rational hope. Philosophical Explorations, 20(Suppl. 1), 127147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in Adulthood: A Five-factor Theory Perspective (2nd edn). London: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McElroy, P. (2011). On Redemption: The Value of Putting Bad Things to Good Use [Doctoral dissertation]. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from: https://searchworks.stanford.edu/view/9082842Google Scholar
McNally, R. J. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 938946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehi, M. R., Bollich, K. L., Doris, J. M., Vazire, S. (2015). Character and coherence. In Miller, C. B., Furr, R. M., Knobel, A. & Fleeson, W. (eds.), Character: New Directions from Philosophy, Psychology, and Theology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 630646.Google Scholar
Mendelssohn-Bartholdy, F. (1842/1945). Letter to Marc-André Souchay, October 15, 1842. In Selden-Goth, G. (ed.), Letters. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(2), 5774.Google Scholar
Meyer, W. U., Reisenzein, R. & Schützwohl, A. (1997). Towards a process analysis of emotions: The case of surprise. Motivation and Emotion, 21(3), 251274.Google Scholar
Miceli, M. & Castelfranchi, C. (2015). Expectancy and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Kedem, P. & Paz, D. (1990a). The impact of trait anxiety and situational stress on the categorization of natural objects. Anxiety Research, 2(2), 85101.Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Kedem, P. & Paz, D. (1990b). Anxiety and categorization. Part 1: The structure and boundaries of mental categories; Part 2: Hierarchical levels of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences, 8(11), 805821.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. (1989). Biosemantics. The Journal of Philosophy, 86(6), 281297.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. (1995). Pushmi-pullyu representations. Philosophical Perspectives, 9, 185200.Google Scholar
Montero, B. G. (2016). Thought in Action: Expertise and the Conscious Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moors, A. (2012). Comparison of affect program theories, appraisal theories, and psychological construction theories. In Zachar, P. & Ellis, R. D. (eds.), Categorical versus Dimensional Models of Affect: A Seminar on the Theories of Panksepp and Russell. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 257278.Google Scholar
Morsella, E., Dennehy, T. C. & Bargh, J. A. (2013). Voluntary action and the three forms of binding in the brain. In Clark, A., Kiverstein, J. & Vierkant, T. (eds.), Decomposing the Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 183198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, A. (2013). Emotion and Imagination. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. (eds.), Social Perception in Infants. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 101125.Google Scholar
Nanay, B. (2013). Success semantics: The sequel. Philosophical Studies, 165(1), 151165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P., Wood, A. & Rychlowska, M. (2014). Embodied emotion concepts. In The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition. London: Routledge, 240249.Google Scholar
Nietzsche, F. (1887/1989). On the Genealogy of Morality. (Kaufman, W. & Hollingdale, R. J., Trans.). Kaufmann, W. (ed.), New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Noë, A. (2004). Action in Perception (Representation and Mind). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R. & Hietanen, J. K. (2014). Bodily maps of emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 646651.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2003). Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Páez, D. & Rimé, B. (2014). Collective emotional gatherings: Their impact upon identity fusion, shared beliefs and social integration. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 204216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parfit, D. (1971). Personal identity. Philosophical Review, 80(1), 327.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H. & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotions in Social Relations: Cultural, Group and Interpersonal Processes. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papineau, D. (1993). Philosophical Naturalism. Oxford: Blackwell Press.Google Scholar
Pietroski, P. M. (1992). Intentionality and teleological error. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 73(3), 267282.Google Scholar
Ploner, M., Freund, H. J. & Schnitzler, A. (1999). Pain affect without pain sensation in a patient with a postcentral lesion. Pain, 81(1/2), 211214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, C. (2006). Affect without object: Moods and objectless emotions. European Journal of Analytic Philosophy, 2(1), 4968.Google Scholar
Price, C. (2015). Emotion. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2004). Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2012). The Conscious Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puglisi-Allegra, S. & Andolina, D. (2015). Serotonin and stress coping. Behavioural Brain Research, 277, 5867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of meaning. In Philosophical Papers. Vol. II: Mind, Language, and Reality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental Imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157182.Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J. & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106(4), 835.Google Scholar
Rainville, P., Carrier, B., Hofbauer, R., Bushnell, M. & Duncan, G. (1999). Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation. Pain, 82(2), 159171.Google Scholar
Ramsey, F. P. (1927/1990). Facts and propositions. Reprinted in Mellor, D. H. (ed.), Philosophical Papers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. J. (2008). Feelings of Being: Phenomenology, Psychiatry, and the Sense of Reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ravenscroft, I. (2016). Folk psychology as a theory. In E. N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2016 edn.). Retrieved from: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2016/entries/folkpsych-theory/Google Scholar
Reid, T. (1785/1850). Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man. Walker, J. (ed.), Cambridge, MA: J. Bartlett.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Jackson, J. J., Fayard, J. V., Edmonds, G. & Meints, J. (2009). Conscientiousness. In Leary, M. R. & Hoyle, R. H. (eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York: Guilford Press, 369381.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. C. (2003). Emotions: An Essay in Aid of Moral Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. C. (2009). Emotional consciousness and personal relationships. Emotion Review, 1(3), 281288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. (1995). Startle. The Journal of Philosophy, 92(2), 5374.Google Scholar
Robinson, J. (2005). Deeper Than Reason: Emotion and Its Role in Literature, Music, and Art. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Roessler, J. (2005). Joint attention and the problem of other minds. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T. & Roessler, J. (eds.), Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 245246.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (2011). Emotional behaviors, emotivational goals, emotion strategies: Multiple levels of organization integrate variable and consistent responses. Emotion Review, 3(4), 434443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, S. E. (2011). Pain and itch: Insights into the neural circuits of aversive somatosensation in health and disease. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21(6), 880887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Lowery, L. & Ebert, R. (1994). Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 870881.Google Scholar
Ruff, C. C. (2011). A systems-neuroscience view of attention. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., Wu, W. (eds.), Attention: Philosophical and Psychological Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 123.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 11611178.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 426450.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmela, M. (2011). Can emotion be modelled on perception? Dialectica, 65(1), 129.Google Scholar
Salomons, T. V., Johnstone, T., Backonja, M. M. & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Perceived controllability modulates the neural response to pain. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(32), 71997203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarantino, A. (2010). Insights and blindspots of the cognitivist theory of emotions. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 61(4), 729768.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2014). The motivational theory of emotions. In Jacobson, D. & D’Arms, J. (eds.), Moral Psychology and Human Agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 156185.Google Scholar
Schablitzky, S. & Pause, B. M. (2014). Sadness might isolate you in a non-smelling world: Olfactory perception and depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 45. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schechtman, M. (2011). The narrative self. In Gallagher, S. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of the Self. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 394416.Google Scholar
Scheler, M. (1954). The Nature of Sympathy (Heath, P., Trans.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. (2005). What are emotions and how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44(4), 695729.Google Scholar
Schilder, P. & Stengel, E. (1928). Schmerzasymbolie. Zeitschrift für die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 113, 143158.Google Scholar
Schmid, H. B. (2014). The feeling of being a group: Corporate emotions and collective consciousness. In Von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (eds.), Collective Emotions: Perspectives from Psychology, Philosophy, and Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 316.Google Scholar
Schroeder, T. (2016). A sentimentalist theory of mind, by Michael Slote. Mind, 125(497), 228231.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1995). The Construction of Social Reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D. & O’Connor, G. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 10611086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiner, R. & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 232.Google Scholar
Siemer, M. (2009). Mood experience: Implications of a dispositional theory of moods. Emotion Review, 1(3), 256263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439(7075), 466469.Google Scholar
Slaby, J. (2012). Affective self-construal and the sense of ability. Emotion Review, 4(2), 151156.Google Scholar
Slote, M. (2014). A Sentimentalist Theory of the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, J. (2015). The phenomenology of face-to-face mindreading. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 90(2), 274293.Google Scholar
Smuts, A. (2011). The feels good theory of pleasure. Philosophical Studies, 155(2), 241265.Google Scholar
Smuts, A. (2014). Normative reasons for love, Part I. Philosophy Compass, 9(8), 507517.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (2005). What’s character got to do with it? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 71(3), 648656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (2006). Against valence (‘positive’ and ‘negative’ emotions). In Not Passion’s Slave. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (2007). True to our Feelings: What our Emotions Are Really Telling Us. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Starkey, C. (2015). Emotion and character. In Miller, C. B., Furr, R. M., Knobel, A. & Fleeson, W. (eds.), Character: New Directions from Philosophy, Psychology, and Theology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 192209.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 465481.Google Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L. & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768777.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2003). Mental ballistics or the involuntariness of spontaneity. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 103(3), 227257.Google Scholar
Tappolet, C. (2000). Emotions et Valeurs. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tappolet, C. (2016). Emotions, Value, and Agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. & Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 377401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennov, D. (1979). Love and Limerence: The Experience of Being in Love. New York: Stein and Day.Google Scholar
Terasawa, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Tochizawa, S. & Umeda, S. (2014). Interoceptive sensitivity predicts sensitivity to the emotions of others. Cognition and Emotion, 28(8), 14351448.Google Scholar
Teroni, F. (2007). Emotions and formal objects. Dialectica, 61(3), 395415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (2008). Connectionist models of cognition. In Sun, R. (ed.) The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2358.Google Scholar
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Todd, C. (2012). Attending emotionally to fiction. Journal of Value Inquiry, 46(4), 449465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (2003). Things still to be done on the still-face effect. Infancy, 4, 475482.Google Scholar
Tuomela, R. (1991). We will do it: An analysis of group-intentions. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 51(2), 249277.Google Scholar
Van der Kam, E. L., Vry, J. D., Schiene, K. & Tzschentke, T. M. (2008). Differential effects of morphine on the affective and the sensory component of carrageenan-induced nociception in the rat. Pain, 136, 373379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velleman, J. D. (2000). The Possibility of Practical Reason. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Velleman, J. D. (2003). Narrative explanation. The Philosophical Review, 112(1), 125.Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L., Gronau, Q. F., Acosta, A., Adams, R. B. Jr., … Zwaan, R. A. (2016). Registered replication report: Strack, Martin & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(6), 917928.Google Scholar
Walton, K. (1978). Fearing fictions. The Journal of Philosophy, 75(1), 527.Google Scholar
Webber, J. (2015). Character, attitude and disposition. European Journal of Philosophy, 23(4), 10821096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, M. (2005). Reconstructing the Cognitive World: The Next Step. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wiech, K., Kalisch, R., Weiskopf, N., Pleger, B., Stephan, K. E. & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Anterolateral prefrontal cortex mediates the analgesic effect of expected and perceived control over pain. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(44), 1150111509.Google Scholar
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. Paris: Hermann.Google Scholar
Williams, B. (2007). Life as narrative. European Journal of Philosophy, 17(2), 305314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winkielman, P. & Berridge, K. (2015). Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 120123.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1980). Remarks on the Philosophy of Psychology. Vol. I. (Anscombe, G. E. M., Trans.). Anscombe, G. E. M. & von Wright, G. H. (eds.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wolf, S. (2007). Moral Psychology And The Unity Of The Virtues. Ratio, 20(2), 145167.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2012). Explaining schizophrenia: Auditory verbal hallucination and self‐monitoring. Mind & Language, 27(1), 86107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, W. (2013). Mental action and the threat of automaticity. In Clark, A., Kiverstein, J. & Vierkant, T. (eds.), Decomposing the Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 244261.Google Scholar
Yiend, J. (2009). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24(1), 347.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39(2), 117123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeman, A., Dewar, M. & Della Sala, S. (2016). Reflections on aphantasia. Cortex, 74, 336337.Google Scholar
Zhong, C. & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19(9), 838842.Google Scholar
Zhong, C. & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 14511452.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Tom Cochrane, Flinders University of South Australia
  • Book: The Emotional Mind
  • Online publication: 04 January 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108579056.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Tom Cochrane, Flinders University of South Australia
  • Book: The Emotional Mind
  • Online publication: 04 January 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108579056.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Tom Cochrane, Flinders University of South Australia
  • Book: The Emotional Mind
  • Online publication: 04 January 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108579056.012
Available formats
×