Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T14:40:05.143Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Basic Processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2023

Ursula Hess
Affiliation:
Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
Reginald B. Adams, Jr.
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Robert E. Kleck
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, New Hampshire
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Emotion Communication by the Aging Face and Body
A Multidisciplinary View
, pp. 1 - 82
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Adams, R. B. Jr., Ambady, N., Nakayama, K., & Shimojo, S. (2011). Social Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Effect of gaze on Amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science, 300, 15361537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., Nelson, A. J., Soto, J. A., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Emotion in the neutral face: A mechanism for impression formation? Cognition and Emotion, 26, 431441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antypa, D., Kafetsios, K., Simos, P., et al. (2022). Neural correlates of contextualized facial emotion perception and its links to personality. Paper presented at the Conference of the Consortium of European Research on Emotion (CERE), Granada, Spain, June 1011.Google Scholar
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 10501066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, A. P. (2013). Bodily expressions of emotion: Visual cues and neural mechanisms. In Vuilleumier, J. A. P. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience (pp. 198222). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1997). The emotions: An integrative approach. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J. A., & Briggs, S. R. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 513541). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, D. J., & Gariépy, J. L. (2001). The functions of freezing in the social interactions of juvenile high-and low-aggressive mice. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 463475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In Feldman, R. S. & Rimé, B. (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401432). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berry, D. S., & Brownlow, S. (1989). Were the physiognomists right? Personality correlates of facial babyishness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 266279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1985). Some components and consequences of a babyface. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 312323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biesanz, J. C., & Wallace, J. D. (2020). Accuracy in person perception. In Matthews, G. & Corr, P. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 4455). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77, 343352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender, emotion, and expression. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 447460). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1984). Nonverbal receiving ability. In Buck, R. (Ed.), The communication of emotion (pp. 209242). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bull, N., & Gidro-Frank, L. (1950). Emotions induced and studied in hypnotic subjects: Part II – The findings. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 112, 97120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cary, M. S. (1978). The role of gaze in the initiation of conversation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 41, 269271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coussi-Korbel, S. (1994). Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 164171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cracco, E., Genschow, O., & Baess, P. (2022). Top-down social modulation of perception-action coupling. Acta Psychologica, 222, 103481. DOI : https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103481CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuddy, A. J. C., & Fiske, S. T. (2002). Doddering but dear: Process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons (pp. 326). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. (Originally published, 1872)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 7388. http://10.1037/a0034251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3), 285290. http://10.1037/h0033731CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971 (pp. 207283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(3), 288298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203235. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M. G., Levesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7, 131146. http://10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Ross, L. (1975). Intimacy in response to direct gaze. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11(6), 592613. http://10.1016/0022-1031(75)90010-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Goldsmith, H., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of the affective sciences (pp. 572595). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, B. J., & Exline, R. V. (1987). Nonverbal behavior and communication In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (Eds.), Social visual interaction: A conceptual and literature review (pp. 225325). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Felson, R. B. (1979). “Are the good beautiful or the beautiful good?” The relationship between children’s perception of ability and perceptions of physical attractiveness. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42(4), 386392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism & Psychology, 3, 303318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foos, P. W., & Clark, M. C. (2011). Adult Age and gender differences in perceptions of facial attractiveness: Beauty is in the eye of the older beholder. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172(2), 162175. http://10.1080/00221325.2010.526154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freudenberg, M., Albohn, D. N., Kleck, R. E., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Hess, U. (2020). Emotional stereotypes on trial: Implicit emotion associations for young and old adults. Emotion, 20, 12441254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion: Cognitive structure and action readiness. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 115143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Shure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion appraisal and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, H., & Smith, P. (1979). Accommodation theory: Optimal levels of convergence. In Giles, H. & Clair, R. N. S. (Eds.), Language and social psychology (pp. 4565). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Garner interference reveals dependencies between emotional expression and gaze in face perception. Emotion, 7(2), 296313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2012). Neurocognitive mechanisms of gaze-expression interactions in face processing and social attention. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 553566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, McGhee, A. G., D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480.Google Scholar
Grumet, G. W. (1999). Kinesic cues: The body, eyes, and face. In Guerrero, L. K & DeVito, J. A. (Eds.), The nonverbal communication reader: Classic and contemporary readings (pp. 4889). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.Google Scholar
Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 336343. http://10.1177/1754073914534501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128140. http://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802613828CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, M. A., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License or obligation to smile: The effect of power and sex on amount and type of smiling. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(12), 13321342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex and nonverbal communication. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited: What do we know today? In Kalbfleisch, P. J. & Cody, M. J. (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 2761). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hess, U. (2017). Emotion Categorization. In Lefebvre, C. & Cohen, H. (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (2nd ed., pp. 107126). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2007). When two do the same it might not mean the same: The perception of emotional expressions shown by men and women. In Hess, U. & Philippot, P. (Eds.), Group dynamics and emotional expression (pp. 3352). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2008). The role of perceived emotion in first impressions. In Ambady, N. & Skowronski, J. J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 234254). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). Inter-group misunderstandings in emotion communication. In Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-P., & Dovidio, J. F. (Eds.), Intergroup misunderstandings: Impact of divergent social realities (pp. 85100). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Simard, A., Stevenson, M. T., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Smiling and sad wrinkles: Age-related changes in the face and the perception of emotions and intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 13771380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., Beaupré, M. G., & Cheung, N. (2002). Who to whom and why: Cultural differences and similarities in the function of smiles. In Abel, M. & Ceia, C. H. (Eds.), An empirical reflection on the smile (pp. 187216). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). The influence of expression intensity, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265283. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006623213355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Blaison, C., & Dandeneau, S. (2017). The impact of rewards on empathic accuracy and emotional mimicry. Motivation and Emotion, 41(1), 107112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9590-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2016). The impact of context on the perception of emotions. In Abell, C. & Smith, J. (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological, and legal perspectives (pp. 199218). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2017). The social signal value of emotions: The role of contextual factors in social inferences drawn from emotion displays. In Russell, J. A. & Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 375392). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2019). The Emotion-Based Inferences in Context (EBIC) Model. In The social nature of emotion expression (pp. 1–5). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, Huppertz, U., Mauersberger, D., H., & Kastendieck, T. (2023). Wrinkles are neither beautiful nor nice: The effect of facial wrinkles on person perception and interpersonal closeness. Manuscript submitted for publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Kafetsios, K. (2022). Infusing context into emotion perception impacts emotion decoding accuracy: A truth and bias model. Experimental Psychology, 68, 285294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science, 14, 640643. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1478.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchings, P. B., & Haddock, G. (2008). Look Black in anger: The role of implicit prejudice in the categorization and perceived emotional intensity of racially ambiguous faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 14181420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). Vocal expression of affect. In Harrigan, J. A., Rosenthal, R., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), The new handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 65135). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastendieck, T., Mauersberger, H., Blaison, C., Ghalib, J., & Hess, U. (2020). Laughing at funerals and frowning at weddings: Top-down influences of context-driven social judgments on emotional mimicry. Acta Psychologica, 212, 103195.Google ScholarPubMed
Keating, C. F., Mazur, A., & Segall, M. H. (1981). A cross-cultural exploration of physiognomic traits of dominance and happiness. Ethology and Sociobiology, 2, 4148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, C. F., Mazur, A., Segall, M. H., et al. (1981). Culture and the perception of social dominance from facial expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 615625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A. (2004). PERSON: A general model of interpersonal perception. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 265280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirouac, G., & Hess, U. (1999). Group membership and the decoding of nonverbal behavior. In Philippot, P., Feldman, R., & Coats, E. (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 182210). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 78100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02281954CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korthase, K. M., & Trenholme, I. (1982). Perceived age and perceived physical attractiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54(3 suppl.), 12511258. http://10.2466/pms.1982.54.3c.1251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaFrance, M., & Hecht, M. A. (1995). Why smiles generate leniency. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 207214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The Chameleon Effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 543554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.4.543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lass, N. J., Hughes, K. R., Bowyer, M. D., Waters, L. T., & Bourne, V. T. (1976). Speaker sex identification from voiced, whispered, and filtered isolated vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(3), 675678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavater, J. C. (1804). Essays on physiognomy: For the promotion of the knowledge and the love of mankind (Vol. 1). London: C. Whittingham.Google Scholar
Likowski, K. U., Mühlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 10651072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Siegel, E. H., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: Are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on Lench, Flores, and Bench (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 139, 255263. http://10.1037/a0029038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linville, S. E. (1996). The sound of senescence. Journal of Voice, 10(2), 190200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(96)80046-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C., & Mason, M. F. (2002). Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychological Science, 13, 460464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnée, M. J. C. M., Stekelenburg, J. J., Kemner, C., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 18, 369372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32801776e6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malatesta, C. Z., Izard, C. E., Culver, C., & Nicolich, M. (1987). Emotion communication skills in young, middle-aged, and older women. Psychology and Aging, 2, 193203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2015). Can we undo our first impressions? The role of reinterpretation in reversing implicit evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(6), 823849. http://10.1037/pspa0000021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221232). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5, 119124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauersberger, H., Kastendieck, T. M., Hetmann, A., Schöll, A., & Hess, U. (2022). The different shades of laughter: When do we laugh and when do we mimic other’s laughter? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 1863, 20210188.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. Jr. (1973). Leadership and communication in young chimpanzees. In Menzel, J. E. W. (Ed.), Precultural primate behavior (pp. 192225). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. Jr. (1974). A group of young chimpanzees in a one-acre field. In Schrier, A. M. & Stollnitz, F. (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (pp. 83153). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1993). A cross-cultural comparison of impressions created by age-related variations in gait. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(1), 5568. http://10.1007/BF00987008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motley, M. T., & Camden, C. T. (1988). Facial expression of emotion: A comparison of posed expressions versus spontaneous expressions in an interpersonal communications setting. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 52, 122. http://10.1080/10570318809389622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Social Forces, 74, 823850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 259285. http://10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkinson, B. (1999). Relations and dissociations between appraisal and emotion ratings of reasonable and unreasonable anger and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 347385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2001). Putting appraisal in context. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 173186). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reis, H. T., Wilson, I. M., Monestere, C., & Bernstein, S. (1990). What is smiling is beautiful and good. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20(3), 259267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 161200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., & Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: Experimental evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., Spindel, M. S., & Jose, P. E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), 899915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2019). Affective pragmatics extended: From natural to overt expressions of emotions. In The social nature of emotion expression (pp. 49–81). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2022). Emotional expressions as appeals to recipients. Emotion, 22, 18561868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1997). Profiles of emotion-antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical predictions across cultures. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 113150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Grandjean, D. (2008). Facial expressions allow inference of both emotions and their components. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 789801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, S., Christensen, A., Häußinger, F. B., et al. (2013). Show me how you walk and I tell you how you feel: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study on emotion perception based on human gait. NeuroImage, No Pagination Specified. http://10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.078Google Scholar
Schuller, B., Batliner, A., Steidl, S., & Seppi, D. (2011). Recognising realistic emotions and affect in speech: State of the art and lessons learnt from the first challenge. Speech Communication, 53(9–10), 10621087. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.01.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Barnes, J., & David, A. S. (1999). An investigation into the perception of dominance from schematic faces: A study using the World-Wide Web. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 31, 341346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Showers, C., & Cantor, N. (1985). Social cognition: A look at motivated strategies. Annual Review of Psychology, 36(1), 275305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, N., Bard, C., Dadouchi, F., et al. (1992). Posture and elderly persons: Evidence for deficits in the central integrative mechanisms. In Tutorials in motor behavior, 2 (pp. 917931). Oxford: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Thibault, P., Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2006). The effect of group-identification on emotion recognition: the case of cats and basketball players. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 676683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, G. R. (1943). The effect upon judgements of personality traits of varying a single factor in a photograph. Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 127148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todorov, A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors’ faces: Evidence from a false recognition paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 10511065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A., & Uleman, J. S. (2003). The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors’ faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 549562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(33), 1165511660. www.pnas.org/content/105/33/11655.abstractCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Schalk, J., Fischer, A., Doosje, B. J., et al. (2011). Convergent and divergent responses to emotional displays of ingroup and outgroup. Emotion, 11(2), 286298. http://10.1037/a0022582CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walter, H. (2012). Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Empathy: Concepts, Circuits, and Genes. Emotion Review, 4(1), 917. http://emr.sagepub.com/content/4/1/9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webbink, P. (1986). The power of the eyes. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Weyers, P., Mühlberger, A., Kund, A., Hess, U., & Pauli, P. (2009). Modulation of facial reactions to avatar emotional faces by nonconscious competition priming. Psychophysiology, 46, 328335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J., Mignault, A., & Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: Evidence from connectionist modeling. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 194215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2006). The ecological approach to person perception: Evolutionary roots and contemporary offshoots. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A., & Kendrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 81114). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar

References

Aboim, S., Hearn, J., & Howson, R. (2016). Hegemonic masculinity. In The Blackwell Encyclopedia of Sociology (pp. 14). Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405165518.wbeosh022.pub2Google Scholar
Adams, R. B., Albohn, D. N., Hedgecoth, N., Garrido, C. O., & Adams, K. D. (2022). Angry White faces: A contradiction of racial stereotypes and emotion-resembling appearance. Affective Science, 3, 4661 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-021-00091-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B., Garrido, C. O., Albohn, D. N., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2016). What facial appearance reveals over time: When perceived expressions in neutral faces reveal stable emotion dispositions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 986. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00986CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alaei, R., Ma, M., Bjornsdottir, R. T., Krendl, A., & Rule, N. O. (2022a). Facial appearance elicits stable and valid social judgments across the life span. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Alaei, R., Schwartzman, E., & Rule, N. O. (2022b). Accuracy and consistency in social categorization across context, motivation, and time. Social Cognition, 41(2), 103–136. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2023.41.2.103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alley, Z. M., Kerr, D. C. R., Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2019). Prospective associations between boys’ substance use and problem behavior histories and their facial trustworthiness in adulthood. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(8), 647670. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2019.38.7.647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 10541068. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, S. J., Lee, H., Becerra, J., & Tate, C. C. (2019). Emotional expressions affect perceptions of younger and older adults’ everyday competence. Psychology and Aging, 34(7), 9911004. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected: Divergent goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 248264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjornsdottir, R. T., & Rule, N. O. (2017). The visibility of social class from facial cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(4), 530546. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjornsdottir, R. T., & Rule, N. O. (2020a). Emotion and gender typicality cue sexual orientation differently in women and men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(7), 25472560. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-020-01700-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjornsdottir, R. T., & Rule, N. O. (2020b). Negative emotion and perceived social class. Emotion, 20(6), 10311041. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjornsdottir, R. T., & Rule, N. O. (2021). Perceiving acculturation from neutral and emotional faces. Emotion, 21(4), 720729. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blashill, A. J., & Powlishta, K. K. (2012). Effects of gender-related domain violations and sexual orientation on perceptions of male and female targets: An analogue study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(5), 12931302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9971-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkenau, P., Brecke, S., Möttig, C., & Paelecke, M. (2009). Extraversion is accurately perceived after a 50-ms exposure to a face. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(4), 703706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brody, L. R., Lovas, G. S., & Hay, D. H. (1995). Gender differences in anger and fear as a function of situational context. Sex Roles, 32(1), 4778. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01544757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, E., Lee, T., & Gross, J. (2007). Emotion regulation and culture: Are the social consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion, 7(1), 3048. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, P., Rodgers, B., & Windsor, T. D. (2009). Financial hardship, socio-economic position and depression: Results from the PATH Through Life Survey. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 69(2), 229237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carré, J. M., McCormick, C. M., & Mondloch, C. J. (2009). Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 20(10), 11941198. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chien, A. L., Qi, J., Cheng, N., et al. (2016). Perioral wrinkles are associated with female gender, aging, and smoking: Development of a gender-specific photonumeric scale. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 74(5), 924930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaad.2015.11.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2012). Who’s stressed? Distributions of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples from 1983, 2006, and 2009. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(6), 13201334. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00900.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coles, N. A., Larsen, J. T., & Lench, H. C. (2019). A meta-analysis of the facial feedback literature: Effects of facial feedback on emotional experience are small and variable. Psychological Bulletin, 145(6), 610651. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connell, R. W., & Messerschmidt, J. W. (2005). Hegemonic masculinity: Rethinking the concept. Gender & Society, 19(6), 829859. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243205278639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, A. N., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2021). Adultification, anger bias, and adults’ different perceptions of Black and White children. Cognition and Emotion, 35(7), 14161422. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2021.1950127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czopp, A. M., Lasane, T. P., Sweigard, P. N., Bradshaw, S. D., & Hammer, E. D. (1998). Masculine styles of self-presentation in the classroom: Perceptions of Joe Cool. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 13(2), 281294.Google Scholar
Dietl, E., Rule, N., & Blickle, G. (2018). Core self-evaluations mediate the association between leaders’ facial appearance and their professional success: Adults’ and children’s perceptions. The Leadership Quarterly, 29(4), 476488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1993). What is hegemonic masculinity? Theory and Society, 22(5), 643657. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00993540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duarte, J., Siegel, S., & Young, L. (2012). Trust and credit: The role of appearance in peer-to-peer lending. The Review of Financial Studies, 25(8), 24552483. https://doi.org/10.1093/rfs/hhs071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durán, J. I., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2021). Do emotions result in their predicted facial expressions? A meta-analysis of studies on the co-occurrence of expression and emotion. Emotion, 21(7), 15501569. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.384CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A. (2013). Nonverbal dialects and accents in facial expressions of emotion. Emotion Review, 5(1), 9096. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M. G., Lévesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7(1), 131146. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farkas, J. P., Pessa, J. E., Hubbard, B., & Rohrich, R. J. (2013). The science and theory behind facial aging. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery Global Open, 1(1), e8e15. https://doi.org/10.1097/GOX.0b013e31828ed1daCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinstein, Y., & Shehade Switat, M. (2019). Keep a stiff upper lip or wear your heart on your sleeve? Ethnic identity and emotion management among Arab/Palestinians in Israel. Sociology, 53(1), 139155. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038518768173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Evers, C. (2010). Anger in the context of gender. In Potegal, M., Stemmler, G., & Spielberger, C. (Eds.), International Handbook of Anger: Constituent and Concomitant Biological, Psychological, and Social Processes (pp. 349360). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-89676-2_20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A., & Hess, U. (2017). Mimicking emotions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 151155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitousi, D. (2021). How facial aging affects perceived gender: Insights from maximum likelihood conjoint measurement. Journal of Vision, 21(12), 12. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.12.12CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, S., & Geers, A. (2016). Does trait masculinity relate to expressing toughness? The effects of masculinity threat and self-affirmation in college men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 18. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000053Google Scholar
Freeman, A., Tyrovolas, S., Koyanagi, A., et al. (2016). The role of socio-economic status in depression: Results from the COURAGE (aging survey in Europe). BMC Public Health, 16(1), 1098. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3638-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freudenberg, M., Adams, R. B., Kleck, R. E., & Hess, U. (2015). Through a glass darkly: Facial wrinkles affect our processing of emotion in the elderly. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 1051. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamst, G., Arellano-Morales, L., Meyers, L. S., et al. (2020). Shifting can be stressful for African American women: A structural mediation model. Journal of Black Psychology, 46(5), 364387. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798420939721CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamst, G., Ma-Kellams, C., Arellano-Morales, L., et al. (2021). Development and validation of the Asian American Women’s Shifting Scale (AsAWSS). Asian American Journal of Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.1037/aap0000220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, P. A., & Hole, G. J. (2000). The role of spatial and surface cues in the age-processing of unfamiliar faces. Visual Cognition, 7(4), 485509. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062800394621CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, M. J., & Benzeval, M. (2013). The development of socioeconomic inequalities in anxiety and depression symptoms over the lifecourse. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48(12), 19511961. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-013-0720-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, A. C., Hughes, M. C. B., McBride, P., & Fourtanier, A. (2011). Factors associated with premature skin aging (photoaging) before the age of 55: A population-based study. Dermatology, 222(1), 7480. https://doi.org/10.1159/000322623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Halberstadt, A., Cooke, A., Garner, P., et al. (2020). Racialized emotion recognition accuracy and anger bias of children’s faces. Emotion, 22(3), 403417. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harms, P. D., Han, G., & Chen, H. (2012). Recognizing leadership at a distance: A study of leader effectiveness across cultures. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 19(2), 164172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1548051812436812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, F. C. (2014). The rise of respectability politics. Dissent, 61(1), 3337. https://doi.org/10.1353/dss.2014.0010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. J. (2003). Gatekeeping and remaking: The politics of respectability in African American women’s history and Black feminism. Journal of Women’s History, 15(1), 212220. https://doi.org/10.1353/jowh.2003.0025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselhuhn, M. P., Wong, E. M., & Ormiston, M. E. (2013). Self-fulfilling prophecies as a link between men’s facial width-to-height ratio and behavior. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e72259. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichsmeier, R. (2019). “So long as there’s hair there still”: Displaying lack of interest as a practice for negotiating social norms of appearance for older women. Ageing and Society, 39(7), 13601386. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X17001544CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herek, G., Gillis, R., & Cogan, J. (2009). Internalized stigma among sexual minority adults: Insights from a social psychological perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 3243. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2004). Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression. Emotion, 4(4), 378388. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cognition & Emotion, 19(4), 515536. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Simard, A., Stevenson, M. T., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Smiling and sad wrinkles: Age-related changes in the face and the perception of emotions and intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 13771380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., David, S., & Hareli, S. (2016). Emotional restraint is good for men only: The influence of emotional restraint on perceptions of competence. Emotion, 16(2), 208213. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U. & Fischer, A. (2014). Emotional mimicry: Why and when we mimic emotions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 4557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U. & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M. C. B., Williams, G. M., Baker, P., & Green, A. C. (2013). Sunscreen and prevention of skin aging. Annals of Internal Medicine, 158(11), 781790. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-158-11-201306040-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurd, L. C. (2000). Older women’s body image and embodied experience: An exploration. Journal of Women & Aging, 12(3–4), 7797. https://doi.org/10.1300/J074v12n03_06CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurd Clarke, L., & Griffin, M. (2007). The body natural and the body unnatural: Beauty work and aging. Journal of Aging Studies, 21(3), 187201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2006.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iannacone, M. R., Hughes, M. C. B., & Green, A. C. (2014). Effects of sunscreen on skin cancer and photoaging. Photodermatology, Photoimmunology & Photomedicine, 30(2–3), 5561. https://doi.org/10.1111/phpp.12109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isopahkala-Bouret, U. (2017). “It’s a great benefit to have gray hair!”: The intersection of gender, aging, and visibility in midlife professional women’s narratives. Journal of Women & Aging, 29(3), 267277. https://doi.org/10.1080/08952841.2016.1142773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamal, A., King, B. A., Neff, L. J., et al. (2016). Current cigarette smoking among adults – United States, 2005–2015. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 65(44), 12051211. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6544a2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, J., Gamst, G., Meyers, L., Arellano-Morales, L., & Shorter-Gooden, K. (2015). Development and validation of the African American Women’s Shifting Scale (AAWSS). Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 22(1), 1125. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, K. L., McKay, L. S., & Pollick, F. E. (2011). He throws like a girl (but only when he’s sad): Emotion affects sex-decoding of biological motion displays. Cognition, 119(2), 265280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jyrkinen, M. (2014). Women managers, careers and gendered ageism. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 30(2), 175185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scaman.2013.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jyrkinen, M., & McKie, L. (2012). Gender, age and ageism: Experiences of women managers in Finland and Scotland. Work, Employment and Society, 26(1), 6177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0950017011426313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, E. W. (2006). “No way my boys are going to be like that!”: Parents’ responses to children’s gender nonconformity. Gender & Society, 20(2), 149176. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243205284276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, S. K., DeCelles, K. A., Tilcsik, A., & Jun, S. (2016). Whitened résumés: Race and self-presentation in the labor market. Administrative Science Quarterly, 61(3), 469502. https://doi.org/10.1177/0001839216639577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, S. M., & Lau, A. S. (2013). Revisiting the out-group advantage in emotion recognition in a multicultural society: Further evidence for the in-group advantage. Emotion, 13(2), 203215. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilgore, A. M., Kraus, R., & Littleford, L. N. (2020). “But I’m not allowed to be mad”: How Black women cope with gendered racial microaggressions through writing. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 6(4), 372382. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, M., Daibo, I., & Yogo, M. (2008). The study of emotional contagion from the perspective of interpersonal relationships. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 36(1), 2742. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2008.36.1.27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kite, M. E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11(1), 8396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987.tb00776.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kite, M., Deaux, K., & Miele, M. (1991). Stereotypes of young and old: Does age outweigh gender? Psychology and Aging, 6, 1927. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.6.1.19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloth, N., Damm, M., Schweinberger, S. R., & Wiese, H. (2015). Aging affects sex categorization of male and female faces in opposite ways. Acta Psychologica, 78–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.04.005Google Scholar
Koopmann-Holm, B., & Tsai, J. L. (2014). Focusing on the negative: Cultural differences in expressions of sympathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(6), 10921115. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037684CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubzansky, L. D., Kawachi, I., & Sparrow, D. (1999). Socioeconomic status, hostility, and risk factor clustering in the normative aging study: Any help from the concept of allostatic load? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 21(4), 330338. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02895966CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 305334. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1990). Impression management: A literature review and two-component model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(1), 3447. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, H. M., & Ippolito, M. R. (2014). Being transgender: Navigating minority stressors and developing authentic self-presentation. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 38(1), 4664. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684313501644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingston, R. W., & Pearce, N. A. (2009). The teddy-bear effect: Does having a baby face benefit black chief executive officers? Psychological Science, 20(10), 12291236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02431.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorant, V., Deliège, D., Eaton, W., et al. (2003). Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(2), 98112. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwf182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, F., Xu, F., & Luo, X. (2016). Children’s facial trustworthiness judgments: Agreement and relationship with facial attractiveness. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahler, H. I. M., Kulik, J. A., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F. X. (2006). Effects of two appearance-based interventions on the sun protection behaviors of Southern California beach patrons. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28(3), 263272. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp2803_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahler, H. I. M., Kulik, J. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., & Harrell, J. (2003). Effects of appearance-based intervention on sun protection intentions and self-reported behaviors. Health Psychology, 22(2), 199209. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.2.199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malat, J. R., van Ryn, M., & Purcell, D. (2006). Race, socioeconomic status, and the perceived importance of positive self-presentation in health care. Social Science & Medicine, 62(10), 24792488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.11.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malatesta, C. Z., Fiore, M. J., & Messina, J. J. (1987). Affect, personality, and facial expressive characteristics of older people. Psychology and Aging, 2(1), 6469. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.2.1.64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maranda, E. L., Heifetz, R., Cortizo, J., Hafeez, F., & Nouri, K. (2016). Kraissl lines: A map. JAMA Dermatology, 152(9), 1014. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamadermatol.2016.0325CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal “accents.Psychological Science, 14(4), 373376. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.24461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D., Anguas-Wong, A. M., & Martinez, E. (2008). Priming effects of language on emotion judgments in Spanish – English bilinguals. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(3), 335342. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022108315489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Assar, M. (1992). The effects of language on judgments of universal facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16(2), 8599. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00990324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Nakagawa, S., & Multinational Study of Cultural Display Rules. (2008). Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 925937. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Costello, E. J., Leblanc, W., Sampson, N. A., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Socioeconomic status and adolescent mental disorders. American Journal of Public Health, 102(9), 17421750. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011.300477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMahon, A. M. S. (1994). Understanding language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montepare, J. M., & Dobish, H. (2003). The contribution of emotion perceptions and their overgeneralizations to trait impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(4), 237254. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1027332800296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motro, D., Evans, J. B., Ellis, A. P. J., & Benson, L. I. (2021). Race and reactions to women’s expressions of anger at work: Examining the effects of the “angry Black woman” stereotype. Journal of Applied Psychology, 107(1), 142152. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000884CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naumann, L. P., Vazire, S., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2009). Personality judgments based on physical appearance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(12), 16611671. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209346309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Leary, D. (2020). Socioeconomic Status, Negative Affect, and Health (Order No. 28104038). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (2432565580).Google Scholar
Pitcan, M., Marwick, A., & Boyd, D. (2018). Performing a vanilla self: Respectability politics, social class, and the digital world. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 23, 163179. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmy008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plant, E. A., Hyde, J. S., Keltner, D., & Devine, P. G. (2000). The gender stereotyping of emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1), 8192. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01024.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Re, D. E., & Rule, N. O. (2016). Predicting firm success from the facial appearance of chief executive officers of non-profit organizations. Perception, 45(10), 11371150. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616652043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Re, D. E., & Rule, N. (2017). Distinctive facial cues predict leadership rank and selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(9), 13111322. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217712989CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, S. M., Purcell, R., & McGorry, P. D. (2018). Adolescent and young adult male mental health: Transforming system failures into proactive models of engagement. Journal of Adolescent Health, 62 (3 suppl.), S9S17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.07.024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. F. (1968). Teacher expectations for the disadvantaged. Scientific American, 218(4), 1923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1984). Socially-desirable response and acquiescence in a cross-cultural survey of mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 25(2), 189197. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136668CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruby, M. B., Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Villa, C., & Silberstein, O. (2012). Not all collectivisms are equal: Opposing preferences for ideal affect between East Asians and Mexicans. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(6), 12061209. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). The face of success: Inferences from chief executive officers’ appearance predict company profits. Psychological Science, 19(2), 109111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02054.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2009). She’s got the look: Inferences from female chief executive officers’ faces predict their success. Sex Roles, 61(9–10), 644652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9658-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2010). First impressions of the face: Predicting success and behavior. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 506516. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00282.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2011a). Face and fortune: Inferences of personality from managing partners’ faces predict their law firms’ financial success. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(4), 690696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.05.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2011b). Judgments of power from college yearbook photos and later career success. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(2), 154158. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610385473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., et al. (2010). Polling the face: Prediction and consensus across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rule, N. O., Krendl, A. C., Ivcevic, Z., & Ambady, N. (2013). Accuracy and consensus in judgments of trustworthiness from faces: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 409426. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schieman, S. (2003). Socioeconomic status and the frequency of anger across the life course. Sociological Perspectives, 46(2), 207222. https://doi.org/10.1525/sop.2003.46.2.207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlenker, B. R. (2012). Self-presentation. In Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed., pp. 542570). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Seitz, C. M., Strack, R. W., & Wyrick, D. L. (2012). Cigarette smoking and facial wrinkles: A review of the literature. Journal of Smoking Cessation, 7(1), 1824. https://doi.org/10.1017/jsc.2012.8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slepian, M. L., & Ames, D. R. (2016). Internalized impressions: The link between apparent facial trustworthiness and deceptive behavior is mediated by targets’ expectations of how they will be judged. Psychological Science, 27(2), 282288. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615594897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., LaFrance, M., Knol, K., Tellinghuisen, D., & Moes, P. (2015). Surprising smiles and unanticipated frowns: How emotion and status influence gender categorization. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0202-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y., Lee, E., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11(6), 14501455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoppard, J. M., & Gunn Gruchy, C. D. (1993). Gender, context, and expression of positive emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(2), 143150. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167293192002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swencionis, J. K., Dupree, C. H., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). Warmth‐competence tradeoffs in impression management across race and social‐class divides. Journal of Social Issues, 73(1), 175191. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tea-makorn, P. P., & Kosinski, M. (2020). Spouses’ faces are similar but do not become more similar with time. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 17001. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-73971-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tian, L., Bashir, N. Y., Chasteen, A. L., & Rule, N. O. (2020). The effect of age-stigma concealment on social evaluations. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 42(4), 219234. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1741359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 8694. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 519545. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813833. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 242259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 288307. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tskhay, K. O., Krendl, A. C., & Rule, N. O. (2016). Age-related physical changes interfere with judgments of male sexual orientation from faces. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 1217–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Emotions facilitate the communication of ambiguous group memberships. Emotion, 15(6), 812826. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2017). Internalized homophobia influences perceptions of men’s sexual orientation from photos of their faces. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46(3), 755761. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0628-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuminello, E. R., & Davidson, D. (2011). What the face and body reveal: In-group emotion effects and stereotyping of emotion in African American and European American children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(2), 258274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Baaren, R., Janssen, L., Chartrand, T. L., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). Where is the love? The social aspects of mimicry. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 23812389. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Ven, N., Meijs, M. H. J., & Vingerhoets, A. (2017). What emotional tears convey: Tearful individuals are seen as warmer, but also as less competent. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56(1), 146160. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Ryn, M., & Burke, J. (2000). The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Social Science & Medicine, 50(6), 813828. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00338-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandello, J., & Bosson, J. (2013). Hard won and easily lost: A review and synthesis of theory and research on precarious manhood. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 14, 101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029826CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, T. W., Asman, K., Gentzke, A. S., et al. (2018). Tobacco product use among adults – United States, 2017. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67(44), 12251232. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6744a2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Z., Mao, H., Li, J. Y., & Liu, F. (2016). Smile big or not? Effects of smile intensity on perceptions of warmth and competence. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(5), 787805. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw062Google Scholar
White, E. F. (2001). Dark continent of our bodies: Black feminism and the politics of respectability. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26(8), 13251331. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615590992CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2016). Hypothetical sentencing decisions are associated with actual capital punishment outcomes: The role of facial trustworthiness. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(4), 331338. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615624142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2017). Advances in understanding the detectability of trustworthiness from the face: Toward a taxonomy of a multifaceted construct. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(4), 396400. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416686211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witkower, Z., Rule, N. O., & Tracy, J. L. (in press). Emotions do reliably co-occur with predicted facial signals: Comment on Durán and Fernandez-Dols (2021). Emotion.Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, M. K., Kato, T., & Akamatsu, S. (1996). Relationships between physical traits and subjective impressions of faces – Age and gender information. Electronics and Communications in Japan (Part III: Fundamental Electronic Science), 79(10), 2334. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecjc.4430791003Google Scholar
Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., & Masuda, T. (2007). Are the windows to the soul the same in the East and West? Cultural differences in using the eyes and mouth as cues to recognize emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 303311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., Adelmann, P. K., Murphy, S. T., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1987). Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses. Motivation and Emotion, 11(4), 335346. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00992848CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., Andreoletti, C., Collins, M. A., Lee, S. Y., & Blumenthal, J. (1998a). Bright, bad, babyfaced boys: Appearance stereotypes do not always yield self-fulfilling prophecy effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 13001320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997). Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1(3), 204223. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0103_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., Collins, M. A., & Dutta, R. (1998b). The relationship between appearance and personality across the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(7), 736749. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298247006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., Kikuchi, M., & Fellous, J.-M. (2010). Facial resemblance to emotions: Group differences, impression effects, and race stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(2), 175189. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2008). Social psychological face perception: Why appearance matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(3), 14971517. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., Olson, K., & Hoffman, K. (1993). Stability of babyfaceness and attractiveness across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(3), 453466. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.453CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., Voinescu, L., & Collins, M. A. (1996). “Wide-eyed” and “crooked-faced”: Determinants of perceived and real honesty across the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12), 12581269. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672962212006CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Akha, A. A. S. (2018). Aging and the immune system: An overview. Journal of Immunological Methods, 463, 2126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, M., Levy, A., Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2022). In the body’s eye: The computational anatomy of interoceptive inference. PLoS Computational Biology, 18, e1010490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altose, M. D., Leitner, J., & Cherniack, N. S. (1985). Effects of age and respiratory efforts on the perception of resistive ventilatory loads. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 147153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amery, A., Wasir, H., Bulpitt, C., et al. (1978). Aging and the cardiovascular system. Acta Cardiologica, 33, 443467.Google ScholarPubMed
Antonucci, T., Akiyama, H., & Takahashi, K. (2004). Attachment and close relationships across the life span. Attachment & Human Development, 6, 353370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Growing a social brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 624636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axelsson, J., Sundelin, T., Olsson, M. J., et al. (2018). Identification of acutely sick people and facial cues of sickness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 285, 20172430.Google ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamonti, P. M., Heisel, M. J., Topciu, R. A., et al. (2010). Association of alexithymia and depression symptom severity in adults aged 50 years and older. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 5156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbas, H. (2015). General cortical and special prefrontal connections: Principles from structure to function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 269289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotions are constructed with interoception and concepts within a predicting brain. In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 3338). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: Affective predictions during object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 13251334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 167218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an Integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, D. M., Thornton, W. L., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Theory of mind through the ages: Older and middle-aged adults exhibit more errors than do younger adults on a continuous false belief task. Experimental Aging Research, 37, 481502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntson, G. G., Norman, G. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Cardiac autonomic balance versus cardiac regulatory capacity. Psychophysiology, 45, 643652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birditt, K. S., & Fingerman, K. L. (2005). Do we get better at picking our battles? Age group differences in descriptions of behavioral reactions to interpersonal tensions. Journals of Gerontology: Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, 121128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 2631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borghi, A. M., & Setti, A. (2017). Abstract concepts and aging: An embodied and grounded perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouhassira, D., Lantéri-Minet, M., Attal, N., Laurent, B., & Touboul, C. (2008). Prevalence of chronic pain with neuropathic characteristics in the general population. Pain, 136, 380387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, R., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia: A general deficit of interoception. Royal Society Open Science, 3, 150664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Hawkley, L. C. (2011). Could an aging brain contribute to subjective well-being? In Todorov, A., Fiske, S. T., & Prentice, D. A. (eds.), Social neuroscience: Toward understanding the underpinnings of the social mind (pp. 249262). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, H., Mcneilly, A. S., Luttrell, L. M., & Martin, B. (2012). Endocrine function in aging. International Journal of Endocrinology, 2012, e872478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., et al. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: Evidence based on over ten years of experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26, 2133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castle, E., Eisenberger, N. I., Seeman, T. E., et al. (2012). Neural and behavioral bases of age differences in perceptions of trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 2084820852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro, V. L., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2019). The same with age: Evidence for age-related similarities in interpersonal accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 15171537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, J. S., Wibral, M., Stawowsky, C., et al. (2021). Predictive coding over the lifespan: Increased reliance on perceptual priors in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 13, 631599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chanes, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 96106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of emotional well-being across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 10681091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charles, S. T., Piazza, J. R., Luong, G., & Almeida, D. M. (2009). Now you see it, now you don’t: Age differences in affective reactivity to social tensions. Psychology and Aging, 24, 645653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y. (2002). Unwanted beliefs: Age differences in beliefs of false information. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 9, 217230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chowdhury, R., Sharot, T., Wolfe, T., Düzel, E., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Optimistic update bias increases in older age. Psychological Medicine, 44, 20032012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coats, A. H., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (2008). Emotion regulation in interpersonal problems: The role of cognitive-emotional complexity, emotion regulation goals, and expressivity. Psychology and Aging, 23, 3951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, M. C., & Bloesch, E. K. (2017). Are older adults less embodied? A review of age effects through the lens of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, B. M. & Lipp, O. V. (2018). The influence of multiple social categories on emotion perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, H. D., & Garfinkel, S. N. (2018). The influence of physiological signals on cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 1318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron, 29, 537545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dressaire, D., Stone, C. B., Nielson, K. A., et al. (2015). Alexithymia impairs the cognitive control of negative material while facilitating the recall of neutral material in both younger and older adults. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 442459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21, 18351844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eldesouky, L., & English, T. (2018). Another year older, another year wiser? Emotion regulation strategy selection and flexibility across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 33, 572585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, F., Liao, X., Lei, T., et al. (2021). Development of the default-mode network during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal resting-state fMRI study. NeuroImage, 226, 117581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, M. J., MacCormack, J. K., Bonar, A. S., & Lindquist, K. A. (2023). Interoceptive ability moderates the effect of physiological reactivity on social judgment. Emotion, epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, M. J., Siegel, E., Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., & Wormwood, J. B. (2022). Affect and social judgment: The roles of physiological reactivity and interoceptive sensitivity. Affective Science, 3, 464479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Glimcher, P., Baker, A. L., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). Emotion and decision-making under uncertainty: Physiological arousal predicts increased gambling during ambiguity but not risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 12551262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenske, N. A., & Lober, C. W. (1986). Structural and functional changes of normal aging skin. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 15, 571585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filippi, P., Congdon, J. V., Hoang, J., et al. (2016). Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: Evidence for acoustic universals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 284, 20170990.Google Scholar
Fjell, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2010). Structural brain changes in aging: Courses, causes, and cognitive consequences. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 21, 187221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, S. M., Raja, S. N., Bulcao, C., & Goldstein, D. S. (2000). Age-related thermoregulatory differences during core cooling in humans. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 279, R349R354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frontera, W. R. (2017). Physiologic changes of the musculoskeletal system with aging: A brief review. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Clinics of North America, 28, 705711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galvez-Pol, A., Antoine, S., Li, C., & Kilner, J. M. (2022). People can identify the likely owner of heartbeats by looking at individuals’ faces. Cortex, 151, 176187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gates, K. M., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Sandsten, M., & Blandon, A. Y. (2015). Estimating time-varying RSA to examine psychophysiological linkage of marital dyads. Psychophysiology, 52, 10591065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginty, A. T., Kraynak, T. E., Fisher, J. P., & Gianaros, P. J. (2017). Cardiovascular and autonomic reactivity to psychological stress: Neurophysiological substrates and links to cardiovascular disease. Autonomic Neuroscience, 207, 29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Global Burden of Cardiovascular Diseases Collaboration (2018). The burden of cardiovascular diseases among US states, 1990–2016. JAMA Cardiology, 3, 375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, P., & Taylor, E. W. (2007). Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology, 74, 263285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guardia, T., Geerligs, L., Tsvetanov, K. A., Ye, R., & Campbell, K. L. (2022). The role of the arousal system in age‐related differences in cortical functional network architecture. Human Brain Mapping, 43, 985997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunzelmann, T., Kupfer, J., & Brauhler, E. (2002). Alexithymia in the elderly general population. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 7480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurera, J. W., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2022). Arousal reappraisal in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 37, 350356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, X., Qin, W., Liu, Y., et al. (2014). Abnormal salience network in normal aging and in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 34463464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, A. M., Esposito, G., & Tsakiris, M. (2021). Body in the face of uncertainty: The role of autonomic arousal and interoception in decision‐making under risk and ambiguity. Psychophysiology, 58, e13840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesp, C., Smith, R., Parr, T., et al. (2021). Deeply felt affect: The emergence of valence in deep active inference. Neural Computation, 33, 398446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoemann, K., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Concepts dissolve artificial boundaries in the study of emotion and cognition, uniting body, brain, and mind. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 6776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoemann, K., Xu, F., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Emotion words, emotion concepts, and emotional development in children: A constructionist hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 55, 18301849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, Y.-F., Waszak, F., Strömmer, J., & Hämäläinen, J. A. (2021). Human brain ages with hierarchy-selective attenuation of prediction errors. Cerebral Cortex, 31, 21562168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacowitz, D. M., Livingstone, K. M., & Castro, V. L. (2017). Aging and emotions: Experience, regulation, and perception. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacowitz, D. M., Livingstone, K. M., Harris, J. A., & Marcotte, S. L. (2015). Mobile eye tracking reveals little evidence for age differences in attentional selection for mood regulation. Emotion, 15, 151161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacowitz, D. M., & Ossenfort, K. L. (2017). Aging, attention and situation selection: Older adults create mixed emotional environments. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 15, 69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katsumi, Y., Theriault, J. E., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2022). Allostasis as a core feature of hierarchical gradients in the human brain. Network Neuroscience, 6, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., & Tranel, D. (2009). Interoceptive awareness declines with age. Psychophysiology, 46, 11301136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircanski, K., Notthoff, N., DeLiema, M., et al. (2018). Emotional arousal may increase susceptibility to fraud in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 33, 325337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., et al. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunzmann, U., & Grühn, D. (2005). Age differences in emotional reactivity: The sample case of sadness. Psychology and Aging, 20, 4759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 201206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakatta, E. G. (1993). Cardiovascular regulatory mechanisms in advanced age. Physiological Reviews, 73, 413467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lalley, P. M. (2013). The aging respiratory system: Pulmonary structure, function and neural control. Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology, 187, 199210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., & Riedel, R. (1998). Sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39, 377385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasch, H., Castell, D. O., & Castell, J. A. (1997). Evidence for diminished visceral pain with aging: Studies using graded intraesophageal balloon distension. American Journal of Physiology-Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology, 272, G1G3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebois, L. A.M., Hertzog, C., Slavich, G. M., Barrett, L. F., & Barsalou, L. W. (2016). Establishing the situated features associated with perceived stress. Acta Psychologica, 169, 119132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebherr, M., Schiebener, J., Averbeck, H., & Brand, M. (2017). Decision making under ambiguity and objective risk in higher age: A review on cognitive and emotional contributions. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lima, C. F., Alves, T., Scott, S. K., & Castro, S. L. (2014). In the ear of the beholder: How age shapes emotion processing in nonverbal vocalizations. Emotion, 14, 145160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., MacCormack, J. K., & Shablack, H. (2015). The role of language in emotion: Predictions from psychological constructionism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., Wager, T. D., Weber, J., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 19101922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 121143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, K. M., Castro, V. L., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2020). Age differences in beliefs about emotion regulation strategies. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75, 316326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, K. M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2015). Situation selection and modification for emotion regulation in younger and older adults. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 904910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohani, M., Payne, B. R., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2018). Emotional coherence in early and later adulthood during sadness reactivity and regulation. Emotion, 18, 789804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCormack, J. K., Armstrong-Carter, E. L., Humphreys, K. L., & Muscatell, K. A. (2021a). Neurophysiological contributors to advantageous risk-taking: An experimental psychopharmacological investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 926936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCormack, J. K., Henry, T. R., Davis, B. M., Oosterwijk, S., & Lindquist, K. A. (2021b). Aging bodies, aging emotions: Interoceptive differences in emotion representations and self-reports across adulthood. Emotion, 21, 227246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCormack, J. K., & Lindquist, K. A. (2017). Bodily contributions to emotion: Schachter’s legacy for a psychological constructionist view on emotion. Emotion Review, 9, 3645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCormack, J. K., Stein, A. G., Giovanello, K. S., et al. (2020). Affect in the aging brain: A neuroimaging meta-analysis of functional activation and connectivity differences in older vs. Younger adult affective experience and perception. Affective Science, 1, 128154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magai, C., Consedine, N. S., Krivoshekova, Y. S., Kudadjie-Gyamfi, E., & McPherson, R. (2006). Emotion experience and expression across the adult life span: Insights from a multimodal assessment study. Psychology and Aging, 21, 303317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M. (2020). How arousal-related neurotransmitter systems compensate for age-related decline. In Thomas, A. K. & Gutchess, A. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognitive aging (pp. 101120). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattila, A. K., Kronholm, E., Jula, A., et al. (2008). Alexithymia and somatization in general population. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 716722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (2017). Allostasis and the epigenetics of brain and body health over the life course. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 551552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melcangi, R. C., Magnaghi, V., & Martini, L. (2000). Aging in peripheral nerves: Regulation of myelin protein genes by steroid hormones. Progress in Neurobiology, 60, 291308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendes, W. B. (2010). Weakened links between mind and body in older age: The case for maturational dualism in the experience of emotion. Emotion Review, 2, 240244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In A. W. Toga (ed.), Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference, vol. 2 (pp. 597611). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikkelsen, M. B., O’Toole, M. S., Lyby, M. S., Wallot, S., & Mehlsen, M. (2019). Emotional reactivity and interoceptive sensitivity: Exploring the role of age. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 14401448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, R. J., Symmonds, M., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (2014). The brain ages optimally to model its environment: Evidence from sensory learning over the adult lifespan. PLoS Computational Biology, 10, e1003422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, J., Geary, H., Millgate, E., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018). Direct and indirect effects of age on interoceptive accuracy and awareness across the adult lifespan. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 11931202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neiss, M. B., Leigland, L. A., Carlson, N. E., & Janowsky, J. S. (2009). Age differences in perception and awareness of emotion. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 13051313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nusser, L., Pollatos, O., & Zimprich, D. (2020). Age-related effects on interoceptive accuracy, general interoceptive sensibility, and specific interoceptive sensibility. European Journal of Health Psychology, 27, 154170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, A. D., & Bergeman, C. S. (2004). The complexity of emotions in later life. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 59, 117122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palve, S. S., & Palve, S. B. (2018). Impact of aging on nerve conduction velocities and late responses in healthy individuals. Journal of Neurosciences in Rural Practice, 9, 112116.Google ScholarPubMed
Paneni, F., Diaz, C. C., Libby, P., Lüscher, T. F., & Camici, G. G. (2017). The aging cardiovascular system. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 69, 19521967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellicer, A., Simón, C., & Remohí, J. (1995). Effects of aging on the female reproductive system. Human Reproduction, 10, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polidori, M. C. (2021). Physiology of aging as a basis of complexity in aging medicine and geriatrics. In Gu, D. & Dupre, M. E. (eds.), Encyclopedia of gerontology and population aging (pp. 38243829). New York: Springer International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, K. S., Kanoski, S., Barrett, L. F., & Tsakiris, M. (2021). Functions of interoception: From energy regulation to experience of self. Trends in Neurosciences, 44, 2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riediger, M., Voelkle, M. C., Ebner, N. C., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Beyond “happy, angry, or sad?”: Age-of-poster and age-of-rater effects on multidimensional emotion perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 968982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 863881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, T., Murray, J., Halberstadt, J., & Vater, T. (2012). Age-related differences in deception. Psychology and Aging, 27, 543549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, R. M. (1992). Changes in gastrointestinal function attributed to aging. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 55, 1203S1207S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruzzoli, M., Pirulli, C., Brignani, D., Maioli, C., & Miniussi, C. (2012). Sensory memory during physiological aging indexed by mismatch negativity. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 625.e21–625.e30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sands, M., Garbacz, A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2016). Just change the channel? Studying effects of age on emotion regulation using a TV watching paradigm. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 788795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sands, M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2017). Situation selection across adulthood: The role of arousal. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 791798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sands, M., Livingstone, K. M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2018). Characterizing age-related positivity effects in situation selection. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42, 396404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, A., Sato, Y., & Suzuki, H. (1985). Aging effects on conduction velocities of myelinated and unmyelinated fibers of peripheral nerves. Neuroscience Letters, 53, 1520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satpute, A. B., Kragel, P. A., Barrett, L. F., Wager, T. D., & Bianciardi, M. (2019). Deconstructing arousal into wakeful, autonomic and affective varieties. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Koutstaal, W., & Norman, K. A. (1997). False memories and aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 229236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlegel, K., Vicaria, I. M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2020). Facets of interpersonal accuracy across the lifespan: Is there a single skill in older age? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 44, 253278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulkin, J. (2011). Social allostasis: Anticipatory regulation of the internal milieu. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 2, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 23492356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sennesh, E., Theriault, J., Brooks, D., et al. (2022). Interoception as modeling, allostasis as control. Biological Psychology, 167, 108242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seth, A. K., & Friston, K. J. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20160007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, C., Westlin, C., Quigley, K. S., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2022). Allostasis, action, and affect in depression: Insights from the theory of constructed emotion. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 18, 553580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallcross, A. J., Ford, B. Q., Floerke, V. A., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). Getting better with age: The relationship between age, acceptance, and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 734749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shao, J., Du, W., Lin, T., et al. (2019). Credulity rather than general trust may increase vulnerability to fraud in older adults: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect, 31, 146162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simmons, W. K., Avery, J., Barcalow, J. C., et al. (2013). Keeping the body in mind: Insula functional organization and functional connectivity integrate interoceptive, exteroceptive, and emotional awareness. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 29442958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, J. T., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2015). Caring more and knowing more reduces age-related differences in emotion perception. Psychology and Aging, 30, 383395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenhaut, P., Demeyer, I., De Raedt, R., & Rossi, G. (2018). The role of personality in the assessment of subjective and physiological emotional reactivity: A comparison between younger and older adults. Assessment, 25, 285301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steppan, J., Barodka, V., Berkowitz, D. E., & Nyhan, D. (2011). Vascular stiffness and increased pulse pressure in the aging cardiovascular system. Cardiology Research and Practice, 2011, e263585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterling, P., & Laughlin, S. (2015). Principles of neural design. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Books.Google Scholar
Sullivan, M. D., Huang, R., Rovetti, J., Sparrow, E. P., & Spaniol, J. (2021). Associations between phasic arousal and decisions under risk in younger and older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 105, 262271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sze, J. A., Goodkind, M. S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2012). Aging and emotion recognition: Not just a losing matter. Psychology and Aging, 27, 940950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., & Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 248260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Touroutoglou, A., Zhang, J., Andreano, J. M., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Dissociable effects of aging on salience subnetwork connectivity mediate age-related changes in executive function and affect. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Autonomic, subjective, and expressive responses to emotional films in older and younger Chinese Americans and European Americans. Psychology and Aging, 15, 684693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsakiris, M., & De Preester, H. (eds.). (2018). The interoceptive mind: From homeostasis to awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchino, B. N., Birmingham, W., & Berg, C. A. (2010). Are older adults less or more physiologically reactive? A meta-analysis of age-related differences in cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory tasks. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B, 154162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Bloor, L. E., & Campo, R. A. (2005). Aging and cardiovascular reactivity to stress: Longitudinal evidence for changes in stress reactivity. Psychology and Aging, 20, 134143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umetani, K., Singer, D. H., McCraty, R., & Atkinson, M. (1998). Twenty-four hour time domain heart rate variability and heart rate: Relations to age and gender over nine decades. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 31, 593601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Urry, H. L., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation in older age. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 352357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verdú, E., Ceballos, D., Vilches, J. J., & Navarro, X. (2000). Influence of aging on peripheral nerve function and regeneration. Journal of the Peripheral Nervous System, 5, 191208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volynets, S., Glerean, E., Hietanen, J. K., Hari, R., & Nummenmaa, L. (2020). Bodily maps of emotions are culturally universal. Emotion, 20, 11271136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Mohr, M., Finotti, G., Esposito, G., Bahrami, B., & Tsakiris, M. (2022). Individuals with higher interoceptive accuracy are less suggestible to other people’s judgements. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/d3wsfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolpe, N., Ingram, J. N., Tsvetanov, K. A., et al. (2016). Ageing increases reliance on sensorimotor prediction through structural and functional differences in frontostriatal circuits. Nature Communications, 7, 13034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, D. J., Svoboda, R. C., Bae, K. K., & Haase, C. M. (2021). Individual differences in sadness coherence: Associations with dispositional affect and age. Emotion, 21, 465477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xia, C., Touroutoglou, A., Quigley, K. S., Barrett, L. F., & Dickerson, B. C. (2017). Salience network connectivity modulates skin conductance responses in predicting arousal experience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29, 827836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F., & Griffiths, T. L. (2011). Probabilistic models of cognitive development: Towards a rational constructivist approach to the study of learning and development. Cognition, 120, 299301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×