Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-5mhkq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-06T05:07:34.646Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Categories in talk-in-interaction: Gendering speaker and recipient

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Victoria Land
Affiliation:
University of Surrey
Celia Kitzinger
Affiliation:
University of York
Susan A. Speer
Affiliation:
University of Manchester
Elizabeth Stokoe
Affiliation:
Loughborough University
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Social scientists and linguists have sometimes written about ‘men's talk’ (Coates, 2003), ‘women's talk’ (Coates, 1996), ‘lesbians’ talk' (Morgan & Wood, 1995) or ‘gay men's talk’ (Leap, 1996) as though the fact that the speakers whose talk is being analysed are men, or women, or gay is sufficient warrant for analysing their talk as such. But, as Schegloff (1997, following Sacks, 1972) has famously pointed out, any given individual can be characterized by a wide range of category terms taken from many different category sets, including, for example, gender, sexuality, political alignment, ethnicity, age, nationality, religion, occupation, place of residence, health status, family position and so on. One consequence of this is that we cannot explain the selection of any given category term simply ‘by saying that they are, after all, such a one’ (Schegloff, 1997: 165). The speaker who is a ‘woman’ is also, for example, a lesbian, a Pagan, an environmentalist, a diabetic, a sister and so on. There is always a range of different characterizations of any one person, all of which are equally ‘true’.

Since Schegloff (1997) highlighted the analytical implications of speakers' multiple category memberships, there has been a concerted effort to locate analytic criteria on the basis of which a speaker can legitimately be heard to be speaking as a member of one category rather than another (e.g., Antaki & Widdicombe, 1998; Kitzinger, 2000a; 2006; 2007b; Speer, 2005b; Stokoe, 2005; Wilkinson & Kitzinger, 2003; 2007).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×