Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T03:40:32.024Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2022

W. L. Tiemeijer
Affiliation:
Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Self-Control
Individual Differences and What They Mean for Personal Responsibility and Public Policy
, pp. 312 - 349
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abarca-Gómez, L., Abdeen, Z. A., Hamid, Z. A., Abu-Rmeileh, N. M., Acosta-Cazares, B., Acuin, C., … & Agyemang, C. (2017). Worldwide trends in body-mass index, underweight, overweight, and obesity from 1975 to 2016: A pooled analysis of 2416 population-based measurement studies in 128.9 million children, adolescents, and adults. The Lancet, 390(10113), 2627–2642.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 30–60.Google Scholar
Adelman, R. M., Herrmann, S. D., Bodford, J. E., Barbour, J. E., Graudejus, O., Okun, M. A., & Kwan, V. S. (2017). Feeling closer to the future self and doing better: Temporal psychological mechanisms underlying academic performance. Journal of Personality, 85(3), 398–408.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., & Stewart, J. (with Cohen, S., Cullen, M., Diez Roux, A., Dow, W., et al.). (2007). Reaching for a healthier life: Facts on socioeconomic status and health in the U.S. John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., & Syme, S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49(1), 15–24.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., & Stewart, J. (2010). Health disparities across the lifespan: Meaning, methods, and mechanisms. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186(1), 5–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alesina, A., & Ferrara, E. L. (2000). The determinants of trust (no. w7621). National Bureau of Economic Research.Google Scholar
Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556–574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allan, J. L., Johnston, D. W., Powell, D. J., Farquharson, B., Jones, M. C., Leckie, G., & Johnston, M. (2019). Clinical decisions and time since rest break: An analysis of decision fatigue in nurses. Health Psychology, 38(4), 318–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvaredo, F., Chancel, L., Piketty, T., Saez, E., & Zucman, G. (Eds.). (2018). World inequality report 2018. Belknap Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ampel, B. C., Muraven, M., & McNay, E. C. (2018). Mental work requires physical energy: Self-control is neither exception nor exceptional. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1005.Google Scholar
Anderson, E. S. (1999). What is the point of equality? Ethics, 109(2), 287–337.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. H. (2009). Autonomy gaps as a social pathology: Ideologiekritik beyond paternalism. In Forst, R., Hartmann, M., Jaeggi, R., & Saar, M. (Eds.), Sozialphilosophie und Kritik. Suhrkamp Verlag.Google Scholar
Anokhin, A. P., Grant, J. D., Mulligan, R. C., & Heath, A. C. (2015). The genetics of impulsivity: Evidence for the heritability of delay discounting. Biological Psychiatry, 77(10), 887–894.Google Scholar
Arendt, H. (1968). Between past and future. Viking Press.Google Scholar
Aristotle, . (2011). Nicomachean ethics. Translation Robert C. Bartlett and Susan D. Collins. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Arneson, R. J. (1997). Egalitarianism and the undeserving poor. Journal of Political Philosophy, 5(4), 327–350.Google Scholar
Arneson, R. J. (2000). Egalitarian justice versus the right to privacy. Social Philosophy and Policy, 17(2), 91–119.Google Scholar
Arneson, R. J. (2018). Dworkin and luck egalitarianism. In Olsaretti, S. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of distributive justice. Oxford University Press, 41–64.Google Scholar
Arno, A., & Thomas, S. (2016). The efficacy of nudge theory strategies in influencing adult dietary behaviour: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health, 16(1), 1–11.Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. (2009). Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–422.Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. (2015). Stress weakens prefrontal networks: Molecular insults to higher cognition. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1376–1385.Google Scholar
Austin, M. (2010). Useful fictions: Evolution, anxiety, and the origins of literature. University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. A. (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Banerjee, A., & Duflo, E. (2011). Poor economics: A radical rethinking of the way to fight global poverty. Public Affairs.Google Scholar
Banfield, E. C. (1968). The unheavenly city. Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Bania, N., & Leete, L. (2009). Monthly household income volatility in the US, 1991/92 vs. 2002/03. Economics Bulletin, 29(3), 2100–2112.Google Scholar
Barber, L. K., Grawitch, M. J., & Munz, D. C. (2013). Are better sleepers more engaged workers? A self‐regulatory approach to sleep hygiene and work engagement. Stress and Health, 29(4), 307–316.Google Scholar
Barber, L. K., Munz, D. C., Bagsby, P. G., & Grawitch, M. J. (2009). When does time perspective matter? Self-control as a moderator between time perspective and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(2), 250–253.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (2013). Taking charge of ADHD: The complete, authoritative guide for parents (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bartels, D. M., & Urminsky, O. (2011). On intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 182–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basile, A. G., & Toplak, M. E. (2015). Four converging measures of temporal discounting and their relationships with intelligence, executive functions, thinking dispositions, and behavioral outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 728.Google Scholar
Bauer, J. M., & Reisch, L. A. (2019). Behavioural insights and (un)healthy dietary choices: A review of current evidence. Journal of Consumer Policy, 42(1), 3–45.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2020). Do effect sizes in psychology laboratory experiments mean anything in reality? Psychology: Journal of the Higher School of Economics, 17(4), 803–811.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Alquist, J. L. (2009). Is there a downside to good self-control? Self and Identity, 8(2–3), 115–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Faber, J. E., & Wallace, H. M. (1999). Coping and ego depletion: Recovery after the coping process. In Snyder, C. R. (Ed.), Coping: The psychology of what works (pp. 50–69). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). Recent research on free will: Conceptualizations, beliefs, and processes. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 50, pp. 1–52). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Misguided effort with elusive implications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 574–575.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vonasch, A. J., & Sjåstad, H. (2020). The long reach of self-control. In Mele, A. R. (Ed.), Surrounding self-control (17–46). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Wright, B. R., & Carreon, D. (2019). Self-control “in the wild”: Experience sampling study of trait and state self-regulation. Self and Identity, 18(5), 494–528.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Zell, A. L., & Tice, D. M. (2007). How emotions facilitate and impair self-regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 408–426). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Baumer, E. P., & Wolff, K. T. (2014). Evaluating contemporary crime drop(s) in America, New York City, and many other places. Justice Quarterly, 31(1), 5–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beames, J., Schofield, T. P., & Denson, T. F. (2018). A meta-analysis of improving self-control with practice. In De Ridder, D., Adriaanse, M., & Fujita, K. (Eds.), Routledge international handbook of self-control in health and well-being (pp. 405–417). Routledge.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2012). The development of human reproductive strategies: Progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 310–316.Google Scholar
Bennett, G. G., Merritt, M. M., SollersIII, J. J., Edwards, C. L., Whitfield, K. E., Brandon, D. T., & Tucker, R. D. (2004). Stress, coping, and health outcomes among African-Americans: A review of the John Henryism hypothesis. Psychology & Health, 19(3), 369–383.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., & Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(18), 6771–6779.Google Scholar
Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry, 69(3), 260–265.Google Scholar
Bilano, V., Gilmour, S., Moffiet, T., d’Espaignet, E. T., Stevens, G. A., Commar, A., … & Shibuya, K. (2015). Global trends and projections for tobacco use, 1990–2025: An analysis of smoking indicators from the WHO Comprehensive Information Systems for Tobacco Control. The Lancet, 385(9972), 966–976.Google Scholar
Bin, Y. S., Marshall, N. S., & Glozier, N. (2010). Secular trends in adult sleep duration: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(3), 223–230.Google Scholar
Bjørnskov, C. (2007). Determinants of generalized trust: A cross-country comparison. Public Choice, 130(1–2), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self‐regulation in context. Child Development Perspectives, 4(3), 181–188.Google Scholar
Blázquez, D., Botella, J., & Suero, M. (2017). The debate on the ego-depletion effect: Evidence from meta-analysis with the p-uniform method. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 187–215.Google Scholar
Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116.Google Scholar
Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2006). Mental fatigue, motivation and action monitoring. Biological Psychology, 72(2), 123–132.Google Scholar
Bonnie, R. J. (1983). The moral basis of the insanity defense. American Bar Association Journal, 69(2), 194–197.Google Scholar
BouchardJr., T. J. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits: A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(4), 148–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bovens, L. (2008). The ethics of nudge. In Grune-Yanoff, T. and Hansson, S. O. (Eds.), Preference change: Approaches from philosophy, economics and psychology (pp. 207–219). Springer.Google Scholar
Bowers, J., & Robinson, P. H. (2012). Perceptions of fairness and justice: The shared arms and occasional conflicts of legitimacy and moral credibility. Wake Forest Law Review, 47, 211–284.Google Scholar
Boyd, B. (2009). On the origin of stories. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 219–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratman, M. (1987). Intention, plans, and practical reason. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Chen, E., & Miller, G. E. (2020). Persistence of skin-deep resilience in African American adults. Health Psychology, 39(10), 921–926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Chen, E., Miller, G. E., Kogan, S. M., & Beach, S. R. (2013). Is resilience only skin deep? Rural African Americans’ socioeconomic status-related risk and competence in preadolescence and psychological adjustment and allostatic load at age 19. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1285–1293.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Miller, G. E., Ehrlich, K. B., & Chen, E. (2018). John Henryism coping and metabolic syndrome among young Black adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(2), 216–221.Google Scholar
Broers, V. J., De Breucker, C., Van den Broucke, S., & Luminet, O. (2017). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of nudging to increase fruit and vegetable choice. European Journal of Public Health, 27(5), 912–920.Google Scholar
Bromberg, U., Wiehler, A., & Peters, J. (2015). Episodic future thinking is related to impulsive decision making in healthy adolescents. Child Development, 86(5), 1458–1468.Google Scholar
Brown, A. (2009). Personal responsibility: Why it matters. Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Brownell, K. D., Kersh, R., Ludwig, D. S., Post, R. C., Puhl, R. M., Schwartz, M. B., & Willett, W. C. (2010). Personal responsibility and obesity: A constructive approach to a controversial issue. Health Affairs, 29(3), 379–387.Google Scholar
Bryan, G., Karlan, D., & Nelson, S. (2010). Commitment devices. Annual Review of Economics, 2(1), 671–698.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., & Gullo, M. J. (2017). The influence of episodic foresight on delay discounting and demand for alcohol. Addictive Behaviors, 66, 1–6.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2016). Prospection and the present moment: The role of episodic foresight in intertemporal choices between immediate and delayed rewards. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 29–47.Google Scholar
Buttrick, N. R., Aczel, B., Aeschbach, L. F., Bakos, B. E., Brühlmann, F., Claypool, H. M., … & Wood, M. J. (2020). Many Labs 5: Registered replication of Vohs and Schooler (2008), experiment 1. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(3), 429–438.Google Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M. (2008). On justifying punishment: The discrepancy between words and actions. Social Justice Research, 21(2), 119–137.Google Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., & Darley, J. M. (2008). Psychological aspects of retributive justice. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 193–236). Elsevier.Google Scholar
Carr, M. D., & Wiemers, E. E. (2016). The increasingly unstable earnings of less-educated workers.Google Scholar
Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), 796–815.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2014). Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 293–299.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Children’s behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal of Personality, 71(4), 495–514.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H., Hogan, S., Ramrakha, S., … & Moffitt, T. E. (2016). Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 1–10.Google Scholar
Castells, M. (1996). The rise of the network society. Vol. I of The information age: Economy, society and culture. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chan, H. M. (2005). Rawls’ theory of justice: A naturalistic evaluation. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 30(5), 449–465.Google Scholar
Chemin, M., De Laat, J., & Haushofer, J. (2013). Negative rainfall shocks increase levels of the stress hormone cortisol among poor farmers in Kenya. Available at SSRN 2294171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, Y. Y., Shein, P. P., & Chiou, W. B. (2012). Escaping the impulse to immediate gratification: The prospect concept promotes a future‐oriented mindset, prompting an inclination towards delayed gratification. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 129–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chetty, R., Stepner, M., Abraham, S., Lin, S., Scuderi, B., Turner, N., … & Cutler, , D. (2016). The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001–2014. JAMA, 315(16), 1750–1766.Google Scholar
Cheung, T. T., Gillebaart, M., Kroese, F., & De Ridder, D. (2014). Why are people with high self-control happier? The effect of trait self-control on happiness as mediated by regulatory focus. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 722.Google Scholar
Chou, E. Y., Parmar, B. L., & Galinsky, A. D. (2016). Economic insecurity increases physical pain. Psychological Science, 27(4), 443–454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christian, M. S., & Ellis, A. P. (2011). Examining the effects of sleep deprivation on workplace deviance: A self-regulatory perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 54(5), 913–934.Google Scholar
Christman, J. (2018). Autonomy in moral and political philosophy. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, C. J., Luguri, J. B., Ditto, P. H., Knobe, J., Shariff, A. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Free to punish: a motivated account of free will belief. Journal of personality and social psychology, 106(4), 501.Google Scholar
Clark, C. J., Winegard, B. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2019). Forget the folk: Moral responsibility preservation motives and other conditions for compatibilism. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 215.Google Scholar
Clark, C. J., Winegard, B. M., & Shariff, A. F. (2021). Motivated free will belief: The theory, new (preregistered) studies, and three meta-analyses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(7), e22–e47.Google Scholar
Cohen, G. A. (1989). On the currency of egalitarian justice. Ethics, 99(4), 906–944.Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., & Baum, A. (2006). Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(3), 414–420.Google Scholar
Cohen, S., & Janicki‐Deverts, D. (2012). Who’s stressed? Distributions of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples from 1983, 2006, and 2009. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(6), 1320–1334.Google Scholar
Conger, R. D., & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 175–199.Google Scholar
Cosenza, M., & Nigro, G. (2015). Wagering the future: Cognitive distortions, impulsivity, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gambling. Journal of Adolescence, 45, 56–66.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–114.Google Scholar
Cunningham, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). How to make nothing out of something: Analyses of the impact of study sampling and statistical interpretation in misleading meta-analytic conclusions. Frontiers in Psychology: Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 1639.Google Scholar
Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Staats, B. R. (2015). The impact of time at work and time off from work on rule compliance: The case of hand hygiene in health care. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(3), 846–862.Google Scholar
Dang, J. (2016a). Commentary: A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1155.Google Scholar
Dang, J. (2016b). Testing the role of glucose in self-control: A meta-analysis. Appetite, 107, 222–230.Google Scholar
Dang, J. (2018). An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Research, 82(4), 645–651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dang, J., Barker, P., Baumert, A., Bentvelzen, M., Berkman, E., Buchholz, N., … & Zinkernagel, A. (2021). A multilab replication of the ego depletion effect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(1), 14–24.Google Scholar
Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2013a). The future is now: Reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2339–2342.Google Scholar
Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2013b). The future is now: Comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite, 71, 120–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniel, T. O., Said, M., Stanton, C. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2015). Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and energy intake in children. Eating Behaviors, 18, 20–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dardot, P., & Laval, C. (2014). The new way of the world: On neoliberal society. Verso Trade.Google Scholar
Dassen, F. C., Jansen, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Houben, K. (2016). Focus on the future: Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite, 96, 327–332.Google Scholar
Daugherty, J. R., & Brase, G. L. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(2), 202–207.Google Scholar
De Bruijn, E. J., & Antonides, G. (2021). Poverty and economic decision making: A review of scarcity theory. Theory and Decision, 1–33.Google Scholar
De Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76–99.Google Scholar
De Ridder, D.T., Van der Weiden, A., Gillebaart, M., Benjamins, J., & Ybema, J. F. (2020). Just do it: Engaging in self-control on a daily basis improves the capacity for self-control. Motivation Science, 6(4), 309–320.Google Scholar
Deacon, A., & Mann, K. (1999). Agency, modernity and social policy. Journal of Social Policy, 28(3), 413–435.Google Scholar
Deck, C., & Jahedi, S. (2015). The effect of cognitive load on economic decision making: A survey and new experiments. European Economic Review, 78, 97–119.Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2014). An evolutionary life history framework for psychopathology. Psychological Inquiry, 25(3–4), 261–300.Google Scholar
Delgado, R. (1985). Rotten social background: Should the criminal law recognize a defense of severe environmental deprivation. Minnesota Journal of Law & Inequality, 3, 9–90.Google Scholar
Delhey, J., & Newton, K. (2005). Predicting cross-national levels of social trust: Global pattern or Nordic exceptionalism? European Sociological Review, 21(4), 311–327.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2015). Elbow room: The varieties of free will worth wanting. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1989). The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C., & Caruso, G. D. (2021). Just deserts: Debating free will. Polity Books.Google Scholar
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5–18.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964.Google Scholar
Doebel, S., Michaelson, L. E., & Munakata, Y. (2020). Good things come to those who wait: Delaying gratification likely does matter for later achievement (a commentary on Watts, Duncan, & Quan, 2018). Psychological Science, 31(1), 97–99.Google Scholar
Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Doris, J. M. (2015). Talking to our selves: Reflection, ignorance, and agency. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3), 259–268.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Kim, B., & Tsukayama, E. (2013). Life stress impairs self-control in early adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 608.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Milkman, K. L., & Laibson, D. (2018). Beyond willpower: Strategies for reducing failures of self-control. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(3), 102–129.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16(12), 939–944.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Tsukayama, E., & Kirby, T. A. (2013). Is it really self-control? Examining the predictive power of the delay of gratification task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(7), 843–855.Google Scholar
Dynan, K., Elmendorf, D., & Sichel, D. (2012). The evolution of household income volatility. BE Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 12(2), 1–42.Google Scholar
Eisner, M. (2003). Long-term historical trends in violent crime. Crime and Justice, 30, 83–142.Google Scholar
Eisner, M. (2014). From swords to words: Does macro-level change in self-control predict long-term variation in levels of homicide? Crime and Justice, 43(1), 65–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elias, N. (2000). The civilizing process: Sociogenetic and psychogenetic investigations. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk. Human Nature, 20(2), 204–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Embley, J., Johnson, L. G., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2015). Reproducibility project: Replication report – Replication of study 1 by Vohs & Schooler (2008). OSF, February 3, 2022. https://osf.io/uwt5f/.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2018). Working memory and executive attention: A revisit. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 190–193.Google Scholar
Eshleman, A. (2014). Moral responsibility. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University.Google Scholar
Eskreis-Winkler, L., Gross, J. J., & Duckworth, A. L. (2016). Grit: Sustained self-regulation in the service of superordinate goals. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (3rd ed., pp. 380–395). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist, 59(2), 77–92.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W. (2016). Childhood poverty and adult psychological well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(52), 14949–14952.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W., & De France, K. (2021). Childhood poverty and psychological well-being: The mediating role of cumulative risk exposure. Development and Psychopathology, 1–11. doi:10.1017/S0954579420001947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73(4), 1238–1248.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load: The mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychological Science, 23(9), 979–983.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W., & Rosenbaum, J. (2008). Self-regulation and the income-achievement gap. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(4), 504–514.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.Google Scholar
Evans, T. D., Cullen, F. T., BurtonJr., V. S., Dunaway, R. G., & Benson, M. L. (1997). The social consequences of self‐control: Testing the general theory of crime. Criminology, 35(3), 475–504.Google Scholar
Evers, C., Adriaanse, M., de Ridder, D. T., & de Witt Huberts, J. C. (2013). Good mood food: Positive emotion as a neglected trigger for food intake. Appetite, 68, 1–7.Google Scholar
Falk, A., Kosse, F., & Pinger, P. (2020). Re-revisiting the marshmallow test: A direct comparison of studies by Shoda, Mischel, and Peake (1990) and Watts, Duncan, and Quan (2018). Psychological Science, 31(1), 100–104.Google Scholar
Fayyad, J., Sampson, N. A., Hwang, I., Adamowski, T., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., … & WHO World Mental Health Survey Collaborators. (2017). The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV adult ADHD in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 9(1), 47–65.Google Scholar
Fedorikhin, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2010). Positive mood and resistance to temptation: The interfering influence of elevated arousal. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 698–711.Google Scholar
Fields, S. A., Lange, K., Ramos, A., Thamotharan, S., & Rassu, F. (2014). The relationship between stress and delay discounting: A meta-analytic review. Behavioural Pharmacology, 25(5–6), 434–444.Google Scholar
Fieulaine, N., & Apostolidis, T. (2015). Precariousness as a time horizon: How poverty and social insecurity shape individuals’ time perspectives. In Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., & van Beek, W. (Eds.), Time perspective theory: Review, research and application (pp. 213–228). Springer International.Google Scholar
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider, S. M. (2007). The K-factor, covitality, and personality. Human Nature, 18(1), 47–73.Google Scholar
Finkel, N. J. (2001). Not fair! The typology of commonsense unfairness. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Finkel, N. J., Maloney, S. T., Valbuena, M. Z., & Groscup, J. (1996). Recidivism, proportionalism, and individualized punishment. American Behavioral Scientist, 39(4), 474–487.Google Scholar
Fishbach, A., & Labroo, A. A. (2007). Be better or be merry: How mood affects self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 158–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, O. (1991). Varieties of moral personality: Ethics and psychological realism. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. (2017). The geography of morals: Varieties of moral possibility. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, D. R., & Wright, S. (2018). A hand up or a slap down? Criminalising benefit claimants in Britain via strategies of surveillance, sanctions and deterrence. Critical Social Policy, 38(2), 323–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foucault, M. (2010). The birth of biopolitics: Lectures at the Collège de France, 1978–1979. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Franco-Watkins, A. M., Rickard, T. C., & Pashler, H. (2010). Taxing executive processes does not necessarily increase impulsive decision making. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 193–201.Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. (2015). On equality. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, M. (1953). The methodology of positive economics. In Essays in positive economics. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., & Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 1077–1099.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wänke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(3), 397–419.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2019). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 107–131.Google Scholar
Frohlich, N., & Oppenheimer, J. A. (1993). Choosing justice: An experimental approach to ethical theory. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 352–366.Google Scholar
Fullan, M. (2001). The new meaning of educational change. Routledge.Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(4), 303–327.Google Scholar
Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(3), 508.Google Scholar
Gallup (2017). Gallup 2017 global emotions report, February 7, 2022. https://news.gallup.com/reports/212648/gallup-global-emotions-report-2017.aspx.Google Scholar
Garland, D. (2001) The culture of control: Crime and social order in contemporary society. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garrison, K. E., Finley, A. J., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2019). Ego depletion reduces attention control: Evidence from two high-powered preregistered experiments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(5), 728–739.Google Scholar
Germano, G., & Brenlla, M. E. (2021). Effects of time perspective and self-control on psychological distress: A cross-sectional study in an Argentinian sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110512.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317(5843), 1351–1354.Google Scholar
Gillebaart, M., & de Ridder, D. T. (2015). Effortless self‐control: A novel perspective on response conflict strategies in trait self‐control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(2), 88–99.Google Scholar
Giné, X., Karlan, D., & Zinman, J. (2010). Put your money where your butt is: A commitment contract for smoking cessation. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 2(4), 213–235.Google Scholar
Goldstein, A. N., & Walker, M. P. (2014). The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 679–708.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirsch, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Gottschalk, P., & Moffitt, R. (2009). The rising instability of US earnings. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 23(4), 3–24.Google Scholar
Graafland, J. J. (2009). Doux commerce and self-destruction in a curvilinear relation between competition and virtues. Tilburg University, Conference paper.Google Scholar
Grandner, M. A. (2017). Sleep, health, and society. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 12(1), 1–22.Google Scholar
Grandner, M. A., Hale, L., Jackson, N., Patel, N. P., Gooneratne, N. S., & Troxel, W. M. (2012). Perceived racial discrimination as an independent predictor of sleep disturbance and daytime fatigue. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 10(4), 235–249.Google Scholar
Grandner, M. A., Patel, N. P., Gehrman, P. R., Xie, D., Sha, D., Weaver, T., & Gooneratne, N. (2010). Who gets the best sleep? Ethnic and socioeconomic factors related to sleep complaints. Sleep Medicine, 11(5), 470–478.Google Scholar
Grandner, M. A., Williams, N. J., Knutson, K. L., Roberts, D., & Jean-Louis, G. (2016). Sleep disparity, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic position. Sleep Medicine, 18, 7–18.Google Scholar
Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Stuhlmacher, A. F., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., & Halpert, J. A. (2003). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 447–466.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359, 1775–1785.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Ackerman, J. M., Cantú, S. M., Delton, A. W., Robertson, T. E., Simpson, J. A., … & Tybur, J. M. (2013). When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychological Science, 24(2), 197–205.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015–1026.Google Scholar
Groos, K. (1901). The play of man. Appleton and Company.Google Scholar
Guo, Y., Chen, Z., & Feng, T. (2017). The effect of future time perspective on delay discounting is mediated by the gray matter volume of vmPFC. Neuropsychologia, 102, 229–236.Google Scholar
Hacker, J. S. (2008). The great risk shift (rev. and exp. ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haggard, P., & Eitam, B. (Eds.). (2015). The sense of agency. Social cognition and social neuroscience. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., … & Calvillo, D. P. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495–525.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998–1002.Google Scholar
Hamidovic, A., & de Wit, H. (2009). Sleep deprivation increases cigarette smoking. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 263–269.Google Scholar
Hamoudi, A., Murray, D. W., Sorensen, L., & Fontaine, A. (2015). Self-regulation and toxic stress: A review of ecological, biological, and developmental studies of self-regulation and stress (OPRE report 2015-30). Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Harrison, Y., & Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(3), 236.Google Scholar
Harvey, N. (2020). Behavioral fatigue: Real phenomenon, naïve construct, or policy contrivance? Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2960.Google Scholar
Haskell, T. L. (1985). Capitalism and the origins of the humanitarian sensibility, Part 2. American Historical Review, 90(3), 547–566.Google Scholar
Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862–867.Google Scholar
Haushofer, J., & Shapiro, J. (2016). The short-term impact of unconditional cash transfers to the poor: Experimental evidence from Kenya. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 131(4), 1973–2042.Google Scholar
Heath, G., Roach, G. D., Dorrian, J., Ferguson, S. A., Darwent, D., & Sargent, C. (2012). The effect of sleep restriction on snacking behaviour during a week of simulated shiftwork. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 45, 62–67.Google Scholar
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132–139.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J., Moon, S. H., Pinto, R., Savelyev, P. A., & Yavitz, A. (2010). The rate of return to the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1–2), 114–128.Google Scholar
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259.Google Scholar
Hemerijck, A. (Ed.). (2017). The uses of social investment. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The weirdest people in the world: How the West became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Hershfield, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W. F., Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., & Bailenson, J. N. (2011). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S23–S37.Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., & Grüne-Yanoff, T. (2017). Nudging and boosting: Steering or empowering good decisions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 973–986.Google Scholar
Hill, P. F., & Emery, L. J. (2013). Episodic future thought: Contributions from working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 677–683.Google Scholar
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 298–306.Google Scholar
Hirschman, A. O. (1982). Rival interpretations of market society: Civilizing, destructive, or feeble? Journal of Economic Literature, 20(4), 1463–1484.Google Scholar
Hisler, G. C., Krizan, Z., & DeHart, T. (2019). Does stress explain the effect of sleep on self-control difficulties? A month-long daily diary study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(6), 864–877.Google Scholar
Hockey, G. R. J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45(1–3), 73–93.Google Scholar
Hockey, R. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: Work, effort and control. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1318.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Deutsch, R., Lancaster, K., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Cooling the heat of temptation: Mental self‐control and the automatic evaluation of tempting stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 17–25.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). Working memory and self-regulation. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 2, 204–225.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Yes, but are they happy? Effects of trait self‐control on affective well‐being and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 82(4), 265–277.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174–180.Google Scholar
Hollander-Blumoff, R. (2012). Crime, punishment, and the psychology of self-control. Emory Law Journal, 61, 501–553.Google Scholar
Holroyd, J. (2018). Two ways of socializing responsibility: Circumstantialism versus scaffolded-responsiveness. In Hutchison, K., C. Mackenzie, , & Oshana, M. (Eds.), Social dimensions of moral responsibility (pp. 137–162). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holton, R. (1999). Intention and weakness of will. Journal of philosophy, 96(5), 241–262.Google Scholar
Holton, R. (2009). Willing, wanting, waiting. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hummel, D., & Maedche, A. (2019). How effective is nudging? A quantitative review on the effect sizes and limits of empirical nudging studies. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 80, 47–58.Google Scholar
Ifcher, J., & Zarghamee, H. (2011). Happiness and time preference: The effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review, 101(7), 3109–3129.Google Scholar
Ilkowska, M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Working memory capacity and self-regulation. In Hoyle, R. H. (Ed.), Handbook of personality and self-regulation (pp. 265–290). Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., & Berkman, E. (2015). Six questions for the resource model of control (and some answers). Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(10), 511–524.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., & Friese, M. (2019). The past, present, and future of ego depletion. Social Psychology, 50(5–6), 370–378.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2016). Beyond limited resources: Self-control failure as the product of shifting priorities. In Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 165–181). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Israel, A., Rosenboim, M., & Shavit, T. (2021). Time preference under cognitive load: An experimental study. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 90, 101633.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. J., Beck, E. D., & Mike, A. (2021). Personality interventions. In John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and research (4th ed., pp. 793–805). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Jenson, J., & Saint-Martin, D. (2003). New routes to social cohesion? Citizenship and the social investment state. Canadian Journal of Sociology, 28(1), 77–99.Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1686–1693.Google Scholar
Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Implicit theories about willpower predict self-regulation and grades in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 637–647.Google Scholar
Joireman, J., Balliet, D., Sprott, D., Spangenberg, E., & Schultz, J. (2008). Consideration of future consequences, ego-depletion, and self-control: Support for distinguishing between CFC-Immediate and CFC-Future sub-scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(1), 15–21.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(38), 16489–16493.Google Scholar
Kalis, A. (2011). Failures of agency: Irrational behavior and self-understanding. Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637–671.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 66–71.Google Scholar
Katz, M. B. (2013). The undeserving poor: America’s enduring confrontation with poverty: Fully updated and revised. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kautz, T., Heckman, J. J., Diris, R., Ter Weel, B., & Borghans, L. (2014). Fostering and measuring skills: Improving cognitive and non-cognitive skills to promote lifetime success. OECD Education Working Paper, 110.Google Scholar
Keidel, K., Rramani, Q., Weber, B., Murawski, C., & Ettinger, U. (2021). Individual differences in intertemporal choice. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 991.Google Scholar
Keinan, R., Idan, T., & Bereby-Meyer, Y. (2021). Compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: Active vs. passive risk takers. Judgment and Decision Making, 16(1), 20.Google Scholar
Khatib, H. K. Al., Harding, S. V., Darzi, J., & Pot, G. K. (2017). The effects of partial sleep deprivation on energy balance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71(5), 614–662.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., & Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational snacking: Young children’s decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition, 126(1), 109–114.Google Scholar
Kim, J., Hong, H., Lee, J., & Hyun, M. H. (2017). Effects of time perspective and self-control on procrastination and Internet addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(2), 229–236.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 128(1), 78–87.Google Scholar
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 15(5), 565–579.Google Scholar
Kluegel, J. R., & Mateju, P. (1995). Egalitarian vs. inegalitarian principles of distributive justice. In Kluegel, J. R., Mason, D. S., & Wegener, B. (Eds.), Social justice and political change: Public opinion in capitalist and post-communist states (pp. 209–238). Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Knack, S., & Zak, P. J. (2003). Building trust: Public policy, interpersonal trust, and economic development. Supreme Court Economic Review, 10, 91–107.Google Scholar
Knopik, V. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., DeFries, J. C., & Plomin, R. (2017). Behavioral genetics. Worth Publishers, Macmillan Learning.Google Scholar
Knutson, K. L., Van Cauter, E., Rathouz, P. J., DeLeire, T., & Lauderdale, D. S. (2010). Trends in the prevalence of short sleepers in the USA: 1975–2006. Sleep, 33(1), 37–45.Google Scholar
Kokkoris, M. D., & Stavrova, O. (2021). Staying on track in turbulent times: Trait self-control and goal pursuit during self-quarantine. Personality and Individual Differences, 170, 110454.Google Scholar
Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 665–682.Google Scholar
Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 618–638.Google Scholar
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 768–821.Google Scholar
Krause, A. J., Simon, E. B., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., & Walker, M. P. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(7), 404–418.Google Scholar
Krizan, Z., & Hisler, G. (2016). The essential role of sleep in self-regulation. In Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 182–197). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kroese, F. M., Evers, C., Adriaanse, M. A., & de Ridder, D. T. (2016). Bedtime procrastination: A self-regulation perspective on sleep insufficiency in the general population. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(5), 853–862.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Hoffman, M., Walter, H., & Grafman, J. (2013). An fMRI investigation of the effects of belief in free will on third-party punishment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1143–1149.Google Scholar
Kukowski, C., Bernecker, K., & Brandstätter, V. (2021). Self-control and beliefs surrounding others’ cooperation predict own health-protective behaviors and support for COVID-19 government regulations: Evidence from two European countries. Social Psychological Bulletin, 16(1), 1–28.Google Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498.Google Scholar
Kurth‐Nelson, Z., Bickel, W., & Redish, A. D. (2012). A theoretical account of cognitive effects in delay discounting. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 1052–1064.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2016). The sense of effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 67–70.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661–679.Google Scholar
Kymlicka, W. (2002). Contemporary political philosophy: An introduction. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LaPorta, R., Lopez-Silanes, F., Schleifer, A., & Vishney, R. W. (1997). Trust in large organizations. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 87, 333–338.Google Scholar
Lasswell, H. D. (2018). Politics: Who gets what, when, how. Pickle Partners.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (2004). Politics of nature. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lemke, T. (2001). “The birth of bio-politics”: Michel Foucault’s lecture at the Collège de France on neo-liberal governmentality. Economy and society, 30(2), 190–207.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. U. (2013). The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science, 24(1), 72–79.Google Scholar
Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Levitt, E., Sanchez-Roige, S., Palmer, A. A., & MacKillop, J. (2020). Steep discounting of future rewards as an impulsivity phenotype: A concise review. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 47, 113–138.Google Scholar
Li, J. B., Willems, Y. E., Stok, F. M., Deković, M., Bartels, M., & Finkenauer, C. (2019). Parenting and self-control across early to late adolescence: A three-level meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(6), 967–1005.Google Scholar
Li, J. B., Yang, A., Dou, K., & Cheung, R. Y. (2020). Self-control moderates the association between perceived severity of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) and mental health problems among the Chinese public. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(13), 4820.Google Scholar
Lim, J., & Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 375–389.Google Scholar
Lin, H., & Epstein, L. H. (2014). Living in the moment: Effects of time perspective and emotional valence of episodic thinking on delay discounting. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(1), 12–19.Google Scholar
Lipsky, M. (1980/2010). Street-level bureaucracy: Dilemmas of the individual in public service. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Liu, L., Feng, T., Chen, J., & Li, H. (2013). The value of emotion: How does episodic prospection modulate delay discounting? PLoS One, 8(11), e81717.Google Scholar
Lowe, C. J., Safati, A., & Hall, P. A. (2017). The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: A meta-analytic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 586–604.Google Scholar
Lunn, P. D., Belton, C. A., Lavin, C., McGowan, F. P., Timmons, S., & Robertson, D. A. (2020). Using behavioral science to help fight the coronavirus. Journal of Behavioral Public Administration, 3(1), 1–15.Google Scholar
Macaskill, A. C., Hunt, M. J., & Milfont, T. L. (2019). On the associations between delay discounting and temporal thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 141, 166–172.Google Scholar
Mackenbach, J. P., Stirbu, I., Roskam, A. J. R., Schaap, M. M., Menvielle, G., Leinsalu, M., & Kunst, A. E. (2008). Socioeconomic inequalities in health in 22 European countries. New England Journal of Medicine, 358(23), 2468–2481.Google Scholar
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147–186.Google Scholar
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976–980.Google Scholar
Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2007). Attention, self-control, and health behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 280–283.Google Scholar
Maranges, H. M., & Reynolds, T. A. (2020). Heritability. The Wiley Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences: Models and Theories, 243–247.Google Scholar
Martarelli, C. S., Pacozzi, S. G., Bieleke, M., & Wolff, W. (2021). High trait self-control and low boredom proneness help COVID-19 homeschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 331.Google Scholar
Martin, N. D., Rigoni, D., & Vohs, K. D. (2017). Free will beliefs predict attitudes toward unethical behavior and criminal punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(28), 7325–7330.Google Scholar
May, J., & Holton, R. (2012). What in the world is weakness of will? Philosophical Studies, 157(3), 341–360.Google Scholar
McCormick, K. A. (2013). Anchoring a revisionist account of moral responsibility. Journal of Ethics and Social Philosophy, 7(3), 1–20.Google Scholar
McEwen, C. A., & McEwen, B. S. (2017). Social structure, adversity, toxic stress, and intergenerational poverty: An early childhood model. Annual Review of Sociology, 43, 445–472.Google Scholar
McGeer, V. (2015). Building a better theory of responsibility. Philosophical Studies, 172(10), 2635–2649.Google Scholar
McGeer, V. (2019). Scaffolding agency: A proleptic account of the reactive attitudes. European Journal of Philosophy, 27(2), 301–323.Google Scholar
McGonigal, K. (2011). The willpower instinct: How self-control works, why it matters, and what you can do to get more of it. Penguin.Google Scholar
Mead, L. (1986). Beyond entitlement: The limits of benevolence. Free Press.Google Scholar
Meldrum, R. C., Campion Young, B., Soor, S., Hay, C., Copp, J. E., Trace, M., … & Kernsmith, P. D. (2020). Are adverse childhood experiences associated with deficits in self-control? A test among two independent samples of youth. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 47(2), 166–186.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2010). Weakness of will and akrasia. Philosophical Studies, 150(3), 391–404.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (1995). Autonomous agents: From self-control to autonomy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Melley, T. (2000). Empire of conspiracy: The culture of paranoia in postwar America. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Mennell, S. (2007). The American civilizing process. Polity Books.Google Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science: The reward and communication systems of science are considered. Science, 159(3810), 56–63.Google Scholar
Messner, S. F., Raffalovich, L. E., & Sutton, G. M. (2010). Poverty, infant mortality, and homicide rates in cross‐national perspective: Assessments of criterion and construct validity. Criminology, 48(2), 509–537.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19.Google Scholar
Miller, D. (1999). Principles of social justice. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1994). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.Google Scholar
Miller, G. E., Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Brody, G. H., & Chen, E. (2016). Viral challenge reveals further evidence of skin-deep resilience in African Americans from disadvantaged backgrounds. Health Psychology, 35(11), 1225–1234.Google Scholar
Miller, G. E., Yu, T., Chen, E., & Brody, G. H. (2015). Self-control forecasts better psychosocial outcomes but faster epigenetic aging in low-SES youth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(33), 10325–10330.Google Scholar
Miller, W. I. (1998). The anatomy of disgust. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Milyavskaya, M., Berkman, E. T., & De Ridder, D. T. (2019). The many faces of self-control: Tacit assumptions and recommendations to deal with them. Motivation Science, 5(1), 79–85.Google Scholar
Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). Attentional and motivational mechanisms of self-control. In De Ridder, D., Adriaanse, M., & Fujita, K. (Eds.), Routledge international handbook of self-control in health and well-being (pp. 11–23). Routledge.Google Scholar
Milyavskaya, M., Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2021). Self‐control in daily life: Prevalence and effectiveness of diverse self‐control strategies. Journal of Personality, 89(4),634–651.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329–337.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 31(2), 254–261.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 687–696.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.Google Scholar
Moffitt, R. A. (2020). Reconciling trends in US male earnings volatility: Results from a four data set project (no. w27664). National Bureau of Economic Research.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … & Sears, M. R. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698.Google Scholar
Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., & Scholer, A. A. (2016). Understanding self-regulation failure: A motivated effort-allocation account. In Hirt, E. R., Clarkson, J. J., & Jia, L. (Eds.), Self-regulation and ego control (pp. 425–459). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Monroe, A. E., Brady, G. L., & Malle, B. F. (2017). This isn’t the free will worth looking for: General free will beliefs do not influence moral judgments, agent-specific choice ascriptions do. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(2), 191–199.Google Scholar
Morris, P. A., Hill, H. D., Gennetian, L. A., Rodrigues, C., & Wolf, S. (2015). Income volatility in US households with children: Another growing disparity between the rich and the poor? IRP Discussion Paper Series No. 1429-15. Institute for Research on Poverty, University of Wisconsin Institute for Research on Poverty.Google Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2011). Severe environmental deprivation (aka RSB): A tragedy, not a defense. Alabama Civil Rights & Civil Liberties Law Review, 2, 147–173.Google Scholar
Mounk, Y. (2017). The age of responsibility. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. Times Books.Google Scholar
Mullen, E., & Nadler, J. (2008). Moral spillovers: The effect of moral violations on deviant behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1239–1245.Google Scholar
Muraven, M. (2008). Prejudice as self‐control failure. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(2), 314–333.Google Scholar
Muraven, M. (2010). Building self-control strength: Practicing self-control leads to improved self-control performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 465–468.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., Shiffman, S., & Paty, J. A. (2005). Daily fluctuations in self-control demands and alcohol intake. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19(2), 140–147.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. Journal of personality and social psychology, 91(3), 524.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 894–906.Google Scholar
Murray, C. (1984). Losing ground. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Murray, D. W., Rosanbalm, K., & Christopoulos, C. (2016). Self-regulation and toxic stress report 3: A comprehensive review of self-regulation interventions from birth through young adulthood. Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Murray, D. W., Rosanbalm, K., Christopoulos, C., & Hamoudi, A. (2015). Self-regulation and toxic stress: Foundations for understanding self-regulation from an applied developmental perspective (OPRE report 2015-21). Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T. (2011). The threat of shrinking agency and free will disillusionism. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. & Nadel, L. (Eds.), Conscious will and responsibility: A tribute to Benjamin Libet (pp. 173–188). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T., Shepard, J., Crone, D. L., Everett, J. A., Earp, B. D., & Levy, N. (2020). Does encouraging a belief in determinism increase cheating? Reconsidering the value of believing in free will. Cognition, 203, 104342.Google Scholar
Nadler, J. (2005). Flouting the law. Texas Law Review, 83, 1399–1441.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2015). Bound: Essays on free will and responsibility. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, S., & Knobe, J. (2007). Moral responsibility and determinism: The cognitive science of folk intuitions. Nous, 41(4), 663–685.Google Scholar
Nivette, A., Ribeaud, D., Murray, A., Steinhoff, A., Bechtiger, L., Hepp, U., … & Eisner, M. (2021). Non-compliance with COVID-19-related public health measures among young adults in Switzerland: Insights from a longitudinal cohort study. Social Science & Medicine, 268, 113370.Google Scholar
Nozick, R. (1974). Anarchy, state, and utopia. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006a). Improved self-control: The benefits of a regular program of academic study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28(1), 1–16.Google Scholar
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006b). Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11(4), 717–733.Google Scholar
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2007). Improvements in self-control from financial monitoring. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(4), 487–501.Google Scholar
O’Donnell, S., Daniel, T. O., & Epstein, L. H. (2017). Does goal relevant episodic future thinking amplify the effect on delay discounting? Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 10–16.Google Scholar
Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: Trait variable? Behavioural Processes, 87(1), 1–9.Google Scholar
Odum, A. L., & Baumann, A. A. (2010). Delay discounting: State and trait variable. In Madden, G. J. & Bickel, W. K. (Eds.), Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting (pp. 39–65). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Odum, A. L., Becker, R. J., Haynes, J. M., Galizio, A., Frye, C. C., Downey, H., … & Perez, D. M. (2020). Delay discounting of different outcomes: Review and theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 113(3), 657–679.Google Scholar
OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development) (2017). Behavioural insights and public policy: Lessons from around the world. OECD.Google Scholar
OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development) (2020). Regulatory policy and COVID-19: Behavioural insights for fast-paced decision making. OECD.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self-regulatory thought. Social Cognition, 18(2), 101–129.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Cachia, J. Y. (2016). The problems with positive thinking and how to regulate them. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and application (pp. 547–570). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Olsaretti, S. (2013). Rescuing justice and equality from libertarianism. Economics & Philosophy, 29(1), 43–63.Google Scholar
O’Neill, J., Daniel, T. O., & Epstein, L. H. (2016). Episodic future thinking reduces eating in a food court. Eating Behaviors, 20, 9–13.Google Scholar
Ong, Q., Theseira, W., & Ng, I. Y. (2019). Reducing debt improves psychological functioning and changes decision-making in the poor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(15), 7244–7249.Google Scholar
Oorschot, W. V. (2000). Who should get what, and why? On deservingness criteria and the conditionality of solidarity among the public. Policy & Politics, 28(1), 33–48.Google Scholar
Osberg, L., & Bechert, I. (2016). Social values for equality and preferences for state in-tervention: Is the USA Exceptional?, Working Paper 2016-04, Department of Economics, Dalhouse University.Google Scholar
Ostry, J. D., Loungani, P., & Berg, A. (2019). Confronting inequality. Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Pandey, A., Hale, D., Das, S., Goddings, A. L., Blakemore, S. J., & Viner, R. M. (2018). Effectiveness of universal self-regulation-based interventions in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(6), 566–575.Google Scholar
Pare, P. P., & Felson, R. (2014). Income inequality, poverty and crime across nations. British Journal of Sociology, 65(3), 434–458.Google Scholar
Park, C. L., Wright, B. R., Pais, J., & Ray, D. M. (2016). Daily stress and self-control. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 35(9), 738–753.Google Scholar
Payne, B. K. (2005). Conceptualizing control in social cognition: How executive functioning modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 488–503.Google Scholar
Pearl, R. L., & Puhl, R. M. (2018). Weight bias internalization and health: A systematic review. Obesity Reviews, 19(8), 1141–1163.Google Scholar
Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 40, e31.Google Scholar
Pereboom, D. (2014). Free will, agency, and meaning in life. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66(1), 138–148.Google Scholar
Pilcher, J. J., & Huffcutt, A. I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep, 19(4), 318–326.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: The decline of violence in history and its causes. Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Diamond, B., Farrington, D. P., Tremblay, R. E., Welsh, B. C., & Gonzalez, J. M. R. (2016). A meta-analysis update on the effects of early family/parent training programs on antisocial behavior and delinquency. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 12(2), 229–248.Google Scholar
Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). On the malleability of self‐control: Theoretical and policy implications regarding a general theory of crime. Justice Quarterly, 27(6), 803–834.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D. A., & Helzer, E. G. (2010). Stubborn moralism and freedom of the will. In Baumeister, R. F., Mele, A. R., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Free will and consciousness: How might they work (pp. 102–120). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, G. V., Willcutt, E. G., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., & Rohde, L. A. (2014). ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: An updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 434–442.Google Scholar
Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–709.Google Scholar
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 322–338.Google Scholar
Pratt, J., Brown, D., Brown, M., Hallsworth, S., & Morrison, W. (Eds.). (2013). The new punitiveness. Routledge.Google Scholar
Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: A meta‐analysis. Criminology, 38(3), 931–964.Google Scholar
Pridemore, W. A. (2002). What we know about social structure and homicide: A review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Violence and Victims, 17(2), 127–156.Google Scholar
Pridemore, W. A. (2008). A methodological addition to the cross‐national empirical literature on social structure and homicide: A first test of the poverty‐homicide thesis. Criminology, 46(1), 133–154.Google Scholar
Pridemore, W. A. (2011). Poverty matters: A reassessment of the inequality–homicide relationship in cross-national studies. British Journal of Criminology, 51(5), 739–772.Google Scholar
Protzko, J., Ouimette, B., & Schooler, J. (2016). Believing there is no free will corrupts intuitive cooperation. Cognition, 151, 6–9.Google Scholar
Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: A review and update. Obesity, 17(5), 941–964.Google Scholar
Ranci, C., Parma, A., Bernard, , & Beckfeild, J. (2017). The rise of economic insecurity in the EU: Concepts and measures. Lives Working Paper, 62.Google Scholar
Rank, M. R. (2004). One nation, underprivileged: Why American poverty affects us all. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1999). A theory of justice (rev. ed.). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reimers, S., Maylor, E. A., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 973–978.Google Scholar
Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2005). Delay of gratification and delay discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. Psychological Record, 55(3), 439–460.Google Scholar
Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Caspi, A., Ambler, A., d’Arbeloff, T., de Bruine, M., Elliott, M., … & Moffitt, T. E. (2021). Childhood self-control forecasts the pace of midlife aging and preparedness for old age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(3).Google Scholar
Richter, M., & Stanek, J. (2015). The muscle metaphor in self-regulation in the light of current theorizing on muscle physiology. In Gendolla, G. H. E., Tops, M., & Koole, S. L. (Eds.), Handbook of biobehavioral approaches to self-regulation (pp. 55–67). Springer.Google Scholar
Rimke, H. (2000). Governing citizens through self-help literature. Cultural Studies, 14(1), 61–78.Google Scholar
Rimke, H. (2017). Self-help ideology. In F. M. Moghaddam (Ed.), SAGE encyclopedia of political behavior (pp. 734–737). SAGE.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W. (2009). Back to the future: Personality and assessment and personality development. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(2), 137–145.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 3–25.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 1–25.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. H., Goodwin, G. P., & Reisig, M. D. (2010). The disutility of injustice. NYU Law Review, 85, 1940–2033.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Holcomb, L. (2021). In defense of moral credibility. University of Pennsylvania Law School, Public Law Research Paper 21-07.Google Scholar
Rogers, T., Milkman, K. L., & Volpp, K. G. (2014). Commitment devices: Using initiatives to change behavior. JAMA, 311(20), 2065–2066.Google Scholar
Ronson, J. (2015). So you’ve been publicly shamed. Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
Rose, N. (2000). Government and control. British Journal of Criminology, 40(2), 321–339.Google Scholar
Rose, N., & Miller, P. (2010). Political power beyond the state: Problematics of government. British Journal of Sociology, 61, 271–303.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ruiter, M. E., DeCoster, J., Jacobs, L., & Lichstein, K. L. (2010). Sleep disorders in African Americans and Caucasian Americans: A meta-analysis. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 8(4), 246–259.Google Scholar
Rung, J. M., & Madden, G. J. (2018). Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(9), 1349–1381.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., Bons, T. A., & Hur, Y. M. (2008). The genetics and evolution of the general factor of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(5), 1173–1185.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sachweh, P. (2012). The moral economy of inequality: Popular views on income differentiation, poverty and wealth. Socio-Economic Review, 10(3), 419–445.Google Scholar
Sachweh, P. (2016). Social justice and the welfare state: Institutions, outcomes, and attitudes in comparative perspective. In Sabbagh, C. & Schmitt, M. (Eds.), Handbook of social justice theory and research (pp. 293–313). Springer.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. J., & Lauritsen, J. L. (1994). Violent victimization and offending: Individual-, situational-, and community-level risk factors. In Reiss, Jr. A. J., Roth, J. A., & National Research Council, (Eds.), Social influences. Vol. III of Understanding and preventing violence (pp. 1–114). National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Saunders, B., Milyavskaya, M., Etz, A., Randles, D., Inzlicht, M., & Vazire, S. (2018). Reported self-control is not meaningfully associated with inhibition-related executive function: A Bayesian analysis. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 39.Google Scholar
Scheffler, S. (2003). What is egalitarianism? Philosophy & Public Affairs, 31(1), 5–39.Google Scholar
Scheffler, S. (2005). Choice, circumstance, and the value of equality. Politics, Philosophy & Economics, 4(1), 5–28.Google Scholar
Schlosser, M. (2015). Agency. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(2), 241–255.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 162–173.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 33–46.Google Scholar
Schmidt, A. T., & Engelen, B. (2020). The ethics of nudging: An overview. Philosophy Compass, 15(4), e12658.Google Scholar
Schnell, T., & Krampe, H. (2020). Meaning in life and self-control buffer stress in times of COVID-19: Moderating and mediating effects with regard to mental distress. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 983.Google Scholar
Scholten, H., Scheres, A., De Water, E., Graf, U., Granic, I., & Luijten, M. (2019). Behavioral trainings and manipulations to reduce delay discounting: A systematic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(6), 1803–1849.Google Scholar
Schoofs, D., Wolf, O. T., & Smeets, T. (2009). Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123(5), 1066–1075.Google Scholar
Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers: Experiments on question form, wording, and context in attitude surveys. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(22), 7191–7198.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2011). Stress-induced modulation of instrumental behavior: From goal-directed to habitual control of action. Behavioural Brain Research, 219(2), 321–328.Google Scholar
Segerstrom, S., Boggero, I. and Evans, D. (2016). Pause and plan. In Vohs, K., & Baumeister, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications (pp. 131–145). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682–685.Google Scholar
Shamosh, N. A., & Gray, J. R. (2008). Delay discounting and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 36(4), 289–305.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Greene, J. D., Karremans, J. C., Luguri, J. B., Clark, C. J., Schooler, J. W., … & Vohs, K. D. (2014). Free will and punishment: A mechanistic view of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1563–1570.Google Scholar
Sheehy-Skeffington, J., & Haushofer, J. (2014). The behavioural economics of poverty. In Bhawuk, D. P., Carr, S. C., Gloss, A. E., & Thompson, L. F. (Eds.), Barriers to and opportunities for poverty reduction, poverty reduction (pp. 96–112). United Nations Development Programme.Google Scholar
Sheeran, P., & Webb, T. L. (2016). The intention–behavior gap. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(9), 503–518.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Lieder, F., Kool, W., Griffiths, T. L., Cohen, J. D., & Botvinick, M. M. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99–124.Google Scholar
Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278–292.Google Scholar
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 978–986.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., Siegel, B. S., Dobbins, M. I., Earls, M. F., McGuinn, L., … & Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129(1), e232–e246.Google Scholar
Sie, M. (2018). Sharing responsibility: The importance of tokens of appraisals to our moral practices. In Hutchison, K., Mackenzie, C., & Oshana, M. (Eds.), Social dimensions of moral responsibility (pp. 300–323). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sjåstad, H., & Baumeister, R. F. (2018). The future and the will: Planning requires self-control, and ego depletion leads to planning aversion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 127–141.Google Scholar
Slater, T. (2012). The myth of “Broken Britain”: Welfare reform and the production of ignorance. Antipode, 45(4), 1–22.Google Scholar
Slopen, N., & Williams, D. R. (2014). Discrimination, other psychosocial stressors, and self-reported sleep duration and difficulties. Sleep, 37(1), 147–156.Google Scholar
Smart, J. J. C. (1961/1973). An outline of a system of utilitarian ethics. In Smart, J. J. C. & Williams, B. (Eds.), Utilitarianism: For and against. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, I., Saed, K., & St-Onge, M. P. (2019). Sleep and food intake. In Grandner, M. A. (Ed.), Sleep and health (pp. 243–255). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Snider, S. E., LaConte, S. M., & Bickel, W. K. (2016). Episodic future thinking: Expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 40(7), 1558–1566.Google Scholar
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., & Casey, B. J. (2010). A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 124–133.Google Scholar
Sonnentag, S., & Jelden, S. (2009). Job stressors and the pursuit of sport activities: A day-level perspective. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14(2), 165–181.Google Scholar
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(2), 330–348.Google Scholar
Spears, D. (2011). Economic decision-making in poverty depletes behavioral control. The BE Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 11(1), 1–44.Google Scholar
Spierenburg, P. (2006). Democracy came too early: A tentative explanation for the problem of American homicide. American Historical Review, 111(1), 104–114.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. (2014). How is willpower possible? The puzzle of synchronic self-control and the divided mind. Noûs, 48(1), 41–74.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. (2018). Addiction and fallibility. Journal of Philosophy, 115(11), 569–587.Google Scholar
Stamatakis, K. A., Kaplan, G. A., & Roberts, R. E. (2007). Short sleep duration across income, education, and race/ethnic groups: Population prevalence and growing disparities during 34 years of follow-up. Annals of Epidemiology, 17(12), 948–955.Google Scholar
Standing, G. (2011). The precariat: The new dangerous class. Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Starcke, K., & Brand, M. (2012). Decision making under stress: A selective review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1228–1248.Google Scholar
Stein, J. S., Wilson, A. G., Koffarnus, M. N., Daniel, T. O., Epstein, L. H., & Bickel, W. K. (2016). Unstuck in time: Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology, 233(21–22), 3771–3778.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80(1), 28–44.Google Scholar
Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 742–752.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (1994). The impossibility of moral responsibility. Philosophical Studies: An International Journal for Philosophy in the Analytic Tradition, 75(1–2), 5–24.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (1986). Freedom and Belief. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2004). Free agents. Philosophical Topics, 32(1/2), 371–402.Google Scholar
Strawson, G., & Watson, G. (1998). Free will. In Craig, E. (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. Routledge.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (2008). Freedom and resentment and other essays. Routledge.Google Scholar
Stringhini, S., Carmeli, C., Jokela, M., Avendaño, M., Muennig, P., Guida, F., … & Chadeau-Hyam, M. (2017). Socioeconomic status and the 25×25 risk factors as determinants of premature mortality: A multicohort study and meta-analysis of 1.7 million men and women. The Lancet, 389(10075), 1229–1237.Google Scholar
Stroud, S. (2008). Weakness of will. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R. (2014). Why nudge? The politics of libertarian paternalism. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H., & Benartzi, S. (2004). Save More Tomorrow™: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112(S1), S164–S187.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. J., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Thiele, L. P. (2006). The heart of judgment: Practical wisdom, neuroscience, and narrative. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, S. C. (2009). The role of personal control in adaptive functioning. In Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 202–213). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 53–67.Google Scholar
Tiemeijer, W. L. (2016). Eigen schuld? Een gedragswetenschappelijk perspectief op problematische schulden. Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Tonry, M. (2014). Why crime rates are falling throughout the Western world. Crime and Justice, 43(1), 1–63.Google Scholar
Tu, K. C., Chen, S. S., & Mesler, R. M. (2021). Trait self-construal, inclusion of others in the self and self-control predict stay-at-home adherence during COVID-19. Personality and Individual Differences, 175, 110687.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E., & Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Is H2 = 0 a null hypothesis anymore? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14(3), 410–411.Google Scholar
Urminsky, O., & Zauberman, G. (2015). The psychology of intertemporal preferences. In Keren, G., & G. W. (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (Vol. 2, pp. 141–181). Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E. M. (2002). The moral foundations of trust. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vaihinger, H. (1925/2015). The philosophy of “as-if”: A system of the theoretical, practical, and religious fictions of mankind. CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform.Google Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., … & Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460–471.Google Scholar
Van der Linden, D., & Eling, P. (2006). Mental fatigue disturbs local processing more than global processing. Psychological research, 70(5), 395–402.Google Scholar
Van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta psychologica, 113(1), 45–65.Google Scholar
Van der Steeg, M., & Waterreus, I. (2015). Gedragsinzichten benutten voor beter onderwijsbeleid. Economisch Statistische Berichten, 100(4707), 219–221.Google Scholar
Van der Weiden, A., Benjamins, J., Gillebaart, M., Ybema, J. F., & de Ridder, D. (2020). How to form good habits? A longitudinal field study on the role of self-control in habit formation. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 560.Google Scholar
Van Dillen, L. F., Papies, E. K., & Hofmann, W. (2013). Turning a blind eye to temptation: How cognitive load can facilitate self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 427–443.Google Scholar
Van Gelder, J. L., Hershfield, H. E., & Nordgren, L. F. (2013). Vividness of the future self predicts delinquency. Psychological Science, 24(6), 974–980.Google Scholar
Van Gelder, J. L., Luciano, E. C., Weulen Kranenbarg, M., & Hershfield, H. E. (2015). Friends with my future self: Longitudinal vividness intervention reduces delinquency. Criminology, 53(2), 158–179.Google Scholar
Van Krieken, R. (1989). Violence, self-discipline and modernity: Beyond the “civilizing process.” The Sociological Review, 37(2), 193–218.Google Scholar
Van Krieken, R. (1990). The organization of the soul: Elias and Foucault on discipline and the self. European Journal of Sociology, 31(2), 353–371.Google Scholar
Van Oorschot, W. (2006). Making the difference in social Europe: Deservingness perceptions among citizens of European welfare states. Journal of European Social Policy, 16(1), 23–42.Google Scholar
Vargas, M. (2013). Building better beings: A theory of moral responsibility. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vargas, M. (2022). Instrumentalist theories of moral responsibility. In Nelkin, D. andPereboom, D. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of moral responsibility (pp. 3–26). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vazsonyi, A. T., Mikuška, J., & Kelley, E. L. (2017). It’s time: A meta-analysis on the self-control-deviance link. Journal of Criminal Justice, 48, 48–63.Google Scholar
Vazsonyi, A. T., Pickering, L. E., Junger, M., & Hessing, D. (2001). An empirical test of a general theory of crime: A four-nation comparative study of self-control and the prediction of deviance. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 38(2), 91–131.Google Scholar
Vecchio, R., & Cavallo, C. (2019). Increasing healthy food choices through nudges: A systematic review. Food Quality and Preference, 78, 103714.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2008). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, selfregulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological science, 11(3), 249–254.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Schmeichel, B. J., Lohmann, S., Gronau, Q. F., Finley, A. J., Ainsworth, S. E., ... & Albarracín, D. (2021). A multisite preregistered paradigmatic test of the ego-depletion effect. Psychological Science, 32(10), 1566–1581.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). The value of believing in free will: Encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychological Science, 19(1), 49–54.Google Scholar
Volz, S., Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2021). Eating up cognitive resources: Does attentional consumption lead to food consumption? Appetite, 162, 105165.Google Scholar
Vukasovic´, T., & Bratko, D. (2015). Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychological Bulletin, 141(4), 769–785.Google Scholar
Wagner, D. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2014). Emotion and self-regulation failure. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 613–628). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, D. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2015). Self-regulation and its failure: The seven deadly threats to self-regulation. In Mikulincer, M. E., Shaver, P. R., Borgida, E. E., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 805–842). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Wallace, R. J. (1994). Responsibility and the moral sentiments. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wang, Y. J., Dou, K., & Tang, Z. W. (2017). The relationship between trait self-control, consideration for future consequence and organizational citizenship behavior among Chinese employees. Work, 58(3), 341–347.Google Scholar
Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don’t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of personality and social psychology, 78(4), 753.Google Scholar
Watson, L., Levit, T., & Lavack, A. (2018). Obesity and stigmatization at work. In Thomson, S. B., & Grandy, G. (Eds.), Stigmas, work and organizations (pp. 11–34). Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Watts, B., & Fitzpatrick, S. (2018). Welfare conditionality. Routledge.Google Scholar
Watts, B., Fitzpatrick, S., Bramley, G., & Watkins, D. (2014). Welfare sanctions and conditionality in the UK. Joseph Rowntree Foundation.Google Scholar
Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., & Quan, H. (2018). Revisiting the marshmallow test: A conceptual replication investigating links between early delay of gratification and later outcomes. Psychological Science, 29(7), 1159–1177.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (2001). The Protestant and the spirit of capitalism. Routledge.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral emotions: An attributional approach. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wenzel, M., & Okimoto, T. G. (2016). Retributive justice. In Sabbagh, C., & Schmitt, M. (Eds.), Handbook of social justice theory and research (pp. 237–256). Springer.Google Scholar
Wesley, M. J., & Bickel, W. K. (2014). Remember the future II: Meta-analyses and functional overlap of working memory and delay discounting. Biological Psychiatry, 75(6), 435–448.Google Scholar
West, R., Michie, S., Rubin, G. J., & Amlôt, R. (2020). Applying principles of behaviour change to reduce SARS-CoV-2 transmission. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 451–459.Google Scholar
Western, B., Bloome, D., Sosnaud, B., & Tach, L. (2012). Economic insecurity and social stratification. Annual Review of Sociology, 38, 341–359.Google Scholar
Western, B., Bloome, D., Sosnaud, B., & Tach, L. M. (2016). Trends in income insecurity among US children, 1984–2010. Demography, 53(2), 419–447.Google Scholar
Whinnery, J., Jackson, N., Rattanaumpawan, P., & Grandner, M. A. (2014). Short and long sleep duration associated with race/ethnicity, sociodemographics, and socioeconomic position. Sleep, 37(3), 601–611.Google Scholar
White, M. (2013). The manipulation of choice: Ethics and libertarian paternalism. Springer.Google Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66(5), 297–333.Google Scholar
Whitehead, M., Jones, R., Lilley, R., Pykett, J., & Howell, R. (2017). Neuroliberalism: Behavioural government in the twenty-first century. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wiese, C. W., Tay, L., Duckworth, A. L., D’Mello, S., Kuykendall, L., Hofmann, W., … & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Too much of a good thing? Exploring the inverted‐U relationship between self‐control and happiness. Journal of Personality, 86(3), 380–396.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R., & Pickett, K. (2009). The spirit level: Why more equal societies almost always do better. Penguin.Google Scholar
Willems, Y. E., Boesen, N., Li, J., Finkenauer, C., & Bartels, M. (2019). The heritability of self-control: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 324–334.Google Scholar
Willems, Y. E., Li, J. B., Hendriks, A. M., Bartels, M., & Finkenauer, C. (2018). The relationship between family violence and self-control in adolescence: A multi-level meta-analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(11), 2468.Google Scholar
Willems, Y. E., de Zeeuw, E. L., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Boomsma, D. I., Bartels, M., & Finkenauer, C. (2020). Out of control: Examining the association between family conflict and self-control in adolescence in a genetically sensitive design. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(2), 254–262.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wolff, W., Martarelli, C. S., Schüler, J., & Bieleke, M. (2020). High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(15), 5420.Google Scholar
Wood, W. (2016). The role of habits in self-control. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 95–108). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wouters, C. (2007). Informalization: Manners and emotions since 1890. SAGE.Google Scholar
Wouters, C. (2011a). How civilizing processes continued: Towards an informalization of manners and a third nature personality. Sociological Review, 59, 140–159.Google Scholar
Wouters, C. (2011b). Informalization. In Southerton, D. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consumer culture (Vol. 1, pp. 780–782). SAGE.Google Scholar
Wouters, E. B. C., & Dunning, M. (2019). Civilisation and informalisation. Springer.Google Scholar
Wright, B. R. E., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1999). Low self‐control, social bonds, and crime: Social causation, social selection, or both? Criminology, 37(3), 479–514.Google Scholar
Wu, J., Guo, Z., Gao, X., & Kou, Y. (2020). The relations between early-life stress and risk, time, and prosocial preferences in adulthood: A meta-analytic review. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 557–572.Google Scholar
Xu, P., & Cheng, J. (2021). Individual differences in social distancing and mask-wearing in the pandemic of COVID-19: The role of need for cognition, self-control and risk attitude. Personality and Individual Differences, 175, 110706.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18(5), 459–482.Google Scholar
Yoo, S. S., Gujar, N., Hu, P., Jolesz, F. A., & Walker, M. P. (2007). The human emotional brain without sleep: A prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Current Biology, 17(20), R877–R878.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175.Google Scholar
Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual difference metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.Google Scholar
Zuboff, S. (2019). The age of surveillance capitalism: The fight for a human future at the new frontier of power. Profile Books.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • W. L. Tiemeijer, Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam
  • Book: Self-Control
  • Online publication: 20 August 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009089678.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • W. L. Tiemeijer, Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam
  • Book: Self-Control
  • Online publication: 20 August 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009089678.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • W. L. Tiemeijer, Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam
  • Book: Self-Control
  • Online publication: 20 August 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009089678.014
Available formats
×